Impact of Respectfulness on Semantic Integration During Discourse Processing
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Experiment 1
2.1. Methods
2.1.1. Participants
2.1.2. Experimental Materials
2.1.3. Experimental Procedure
2.1.4. Data Analysis
2.2. Results
2.3. Discussion
3. Experiment 2
3.1. Methods
3.1.1. Participants
3.1.2. Materials, Procedure, and Data Analysis
3.2. Results
3.3. Discussion
4. Experiment 3
4.1. Methods
4.1.1. Participants
4.1.2. Procedure, and Data Analysis
4.2. Results
4.3. Discussion
5. General Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Agha, A. (2007). Language and social relations (pp. xv, 427, pp. xv, 427). Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (1987). Politeness: Some universals in language usage (pp. xiv, 345, pp. xiv, 345). Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Buchanan, T. W., Etzel, J. A., Adolphs, R., & Tranel, D. (2006). The influence of autonomic arousal and semantic relatedness on memory for emotional words. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 61(1), 26–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chang, R., Yang, X., & Yang, Y. (2020). Processing good-fit anomalies is modulated by contextual accessibility during discourse comprehension: ERP evidence. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 35(10), 1423–1434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Christianson, K., Zhou, P., Palmer, C., & Raizen, A. (2017). Effects of context and individual differences on the processing of taboo words. Acta Psychologica, 178, 73–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dhooge, E., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2011). How do speakers resist distraction? Evidence from a Taboo picture-word interference task. Psychological Science, 22(7), 855–859. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–1160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grosser, G. S., & Walsh, A. A. (1966). Sex differences in the differential recall of taboo and neutral words. The Journal of Psychology, 63(2), 219–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guillet, R., & Arndt, J. (2009). Taboo words: The effect of emotion on memory for peripheral information. Memory & Cognition, 37(6), 866–879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hadley, C. B., & MacKay, D. G. (2006). Does emotion help or hinder immediate memory? Arousal versus priority-binding mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(1), 79–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hagoort, P., Baggio, G., & Willems, R. M. (2009). Semantic unification. MIT Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hagoort, P., Hald, L., Bastiaansen, M., & Petersson, K. M. (2004). Integration of word meaning and world knowledge in language comprehension. Science, 304(5669), 438–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ji, L. (2021). When politeness processing encounters failed syntactic/semantic processing. Acta Psychologica, 219, 103391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ji, L., & Cai, L. (2022). The interplay between respectfulness and lexical-semantic in reading Chinese: Evidence from ERPs. Cognitive Neurodynamics, 16(1), 101–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ji, L., Cai, L., & Ji, A. (2022). Respectfulness-processing revisited: An ERP study of Chinese sentence reading. PLoS ONE, 17(6), e0258570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ji, L., & Ji, A. (2023). The interplay between honorific processing and syntactic category processing: ERPs to written Mandarin Chinese. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 21(2), 169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jiang, X., Li, Y., & Zhou, X. (2013). Is it over-respectful or disrespectful? Differential patterns of brain activity in perceiving pragmatic violation of social status information during utterance comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 51(11), 2210–2223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jiang, X., & Zhou, X. (2015). Who is respectful? Effects of social context and individual empathic ability on ambiguity resolution during utterance comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ju, P., Zhou, Z., Xie, Y., Hui, J., & Yang, X. (2024). Music training influences online temporal order processing during reading comprehension. Acta Psychologica, 248, 104340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kwon, N., & Sturt, P. (2016). Attraction effects in honorific agreement in Korean. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lassonde, K. A., & O’Brien, E. J. (2013). Occupational stereotypes: Activation of male bias in a gender-neutral world. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 43(2), 387–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lee, H.-Y. (2019). Linguistic politeness in the Chinese language and culture. Theory and Practice in Language Studies, 10(1), 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leech, G., & Larina, T. V. (2014). Politeness: West and East. Russian Journal of Linguistics, 4, 9–34. [Google Scholar]
- Lee-Wong, S. M. (2000). Politeness and face in Chinese culture. In E. Hess-Lüttich, & R. Watts (Eds.), Cross cultural communication. Peter Lang. [Google Scholar]
- MacKay, D. G., Shafto, M., Taylor, J. K., Marian, D. E., Abrams, L., & Dyer, J. R. (2004). Relations between emotion, memory, and attention: Evidence from taboo Stroop, lexical decision, and immediate memory tasks. Memory & Cognition, 32(3), 474–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Masson, M. E. (1982). Cognitive processes in skimming stories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 8(5), 400–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mathewson, K. J., Arnell, K. M., & Mansfield, C. A. (2008). Capturing and holding attention: The impact of emotional words in rapid serial visual presentation. Memory & Cognition, 36(1), 182–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paul, B. T., Chen, J., Le, T., Lin, V., & Dimitrijevic, A. (2021). Cortical alpha oscillations in cochlear implant users reflect subjective listening effort during speech-in-noise perception. PLoS ONE, 16(7), e0254162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shi, X. (2006). On construction and interpretation of English and Chinese Taboos. Journal of Zhejiang University (Humanities and Social Sciences), 4, 166–173. [Google Scholar]
- Shi, Y., Zhou, T., Zhao, S., Sun, Z., & Zhu, Z. (2024). The immediate integration of semantic selectional restrictions of Chinese social hierarchical verbs with extralinguistic social hierarchical information in comprehension. Language and Cognition, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stenbäck, V., Marsja, E., Hällgren, M., Lyxell, B., & Larsby, B. (2021). The contribution of age, working memory capacity, and inhibitory control on speech recognition in noise in young and older adult listeners. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 64(11), 4513–4523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thomas, L., & LaBar, K. (2005). Emotional arousal enhances word repetition priming. Cognition and Emotion, 19(7), 1027–1047. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, L., Bastiaansen, M., Yang, Y., & Hagoort, P. (2011). The influence of information structure on the depth of semantic processing: How focus and pitch accent determine the size of the N400 effect. Neuropsychologia, 49(5), 813–820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- White, S. J., Warrington, K. L., McGowan, V. A., & Paterson, K. B. (2015). Eye movements during reading and topic scanning: Effects of word frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(1), 233–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wu, Y., Gao, Y., Zhao, S., & Wang, S. (2019). The effects of discourse context and world knowledge on pronoun resolution. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 51(3), 293–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yang, C. (2010). Translation of English and Chinese addressing terms from the cultural Aspect. Journal of Language Teaching and Research, 1(5), 738–742. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yang, X., Zhang, X., Zhang, Y., Zhang, Q., & Li, X. (2020). How working memory capacity modulates the time course of semantic integration at sentence and discourse level. Neuropsychologia, 140, 107383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zeelenberg, R., Bocanegra, B. R., & Pecher, D. (2011). Emotion-induced impairments in speeded word recognition tasks. Experimental Psychology, 58(5), 400–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhou, X. (2008). Xiandai Hanyu Limao Yuyan Yanjiu [A Study of Politeness Language in Modern Chinese]. China Social Science Press. [Google Scholar]
Global Resource Theory | Binding Theory | |
---|---|---|
Experiment 1 | Respect condition: RT for Semantically Inconsistent > RT for Semantically Consistent. Disrespect condition: no differences/smaller differences compared to the Respect condition. | Respect condition: RT for Semantically Inconsistent > RT for Semantically Consistent. Disrespect condition: larger differences compared to the Respect condition. |
Experiment 2 | We expect to obtain similar results to those in Experiment 1. | We expect to obtain similar results to those in Experiment 1. |
Experiment 3 | We anticipate that the results may resemble those of Experiment 1. | We anticipate that the results may resemble those of Experiment 1. |
Sentence Type | Example |
---|---|
Respect, Semantically Consistent | 7岁的小明关上门,/他对老师说:/“这是您要的材料。”/然后继续写作业。/小明非常懂事。 |
Gloss: Seven-year-old Xiaoming closed the door,/He said to the teacher: /“This is you[respectful] requested material.”/Then continued doing homework./Xiaoming is very well-behaved. | |
Translation: Seven-year-old Xiaoming closed the door,/said to the teacher,/“This is the material you[respectful] asked for,”/and then continued doing his homework./Xiaoming is very considerate. | |
Respect, Semantically Inconsistent | 30岁的小明关上门,/他对老师说:/“这是您要的材料。”/然后继续写作业。/小明非常懂事。 |
Gloss: Thirty-year-old Xiaoming closed the door,/He said to the teacher:/“This is you[respectful] requested material.”/Then continued doing homework./Xiaoming is very well-behaved. | |
Translation: Thirty-year-old Xiaoming closed the door,/said to the teacher,/“This is the material you[respectful] asked for,”/and then continued doing his homework./Xiaoming is very considerate. | |
Disrespect, Semantically Consistent | 7岁的小明关上门,/他对老师说:/“这是你要的材料。”/然后继续写作业。/小明非常懂事。 |
Gloss: Seven-year-old Xiaoming closed the door,/He said to the teacher:/“This is you[plain] requested material.”/Then continued doing homework./Xiaoming is very well-behaved. | |
Translation: Seven-year-old Xiaoming closed the door,/said to the teacher,/“This is the material you[plain] asked for,”/and then continued doing his homework./Xiaoming is very considerate. | |
Disrespect, Semantically Inconsistent | 30岁的小明关上门,/他对老师说:/“这是你要的材料。”/然后继续写作业。/小明非常懂事。 |
Gloss: Thirty-year-old Xiaoming closed the door,/He said to the teacher:/“This is you[plain] requested material.”/Then continued doing homework./Xiaoming is very well-behaved. | |
Translation: Thirty-year-old Xiaoming closed the door,/said to the teacher,/“This is the material you[plain] asked for,”/and then continued doing his homework./Xiaoming is very considerate. |
Model Parameters | β | SE | df | t | p | 95%CI |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Respect | −0.01 | 0.02 | 77.43 | −0.30 | 0.76 | [−0.045, 0.033] |
Semantic Consistency | 0.02 | 0.02 | 65.22 | 0.96 | 0.34 | [−0.021, 0.061] |
Respect * Semantic Consistency | 0.03 | 0.03 | 3450.99 | 2.25 | 0.03 | [0.004, 0.053] |
Sentence Type | Sentence Type |
---|---|
Respect, Semantically Consistent | 7岁的小明关上门,/他对老师说:/“朱老师,这是相关的材料。”/然后继续写作业。/小明非常懂事。 |
Gloss: Seven-year-old Xiaoming closed the door,/He said to the teacher:/“Teacher Zhu[respectful], this is the relevant material,”/Then continued writing homework./Xiaoming is very well-behaved. | |
Translation: Seven-year-old Xiaoming closed the door,/said to the teacher:/“Teacher Zhu[respectful], this is the relevant material,”/and then continued doing his homework./Xiaoming is very considerate. | |
Respect, Semantically Inconsistent | 30岁的小明关上门,/他对老师说:/“朱老师,这是相关的材料。”/然后继续写作业。/小明非常懂事。 |
Gloss: Thirty-year-old Xiaoming closed the door,/He said to the teacher:/“Teacher Zhu[respectful], this is the relevant material,”/Then continued writing homework./Xiaoming is very well-behaved. | |
Translation: Thirty-year-old Xiaoming closed the door,/said to the teacher,/“Teacher Zhu[respectful], This is the material you[respectful] asked for,”/and then continued doing his homework./Xiaoming is very considerate. | |
Disrespect, Semantically Consistent | 7岁的小明关上门,/他对老师说:/“朱文轶,这是相关的材料。”/然后继续写作业。/小明非常懂事。 |
Gloss: Seven-year-old Xiaoming closed the door,/He said to the teacher:/“Zhu Wenyi[plain], this is the relevant material,”/Then continued writing homework./Xiaoming is very well-behaved. | |
Translation: Seven-year-old Xiaoming closed the door,/said to the teacher,/“Zhu Wenyi[plain], This is the material you[respectful] asked for,”/and then continued doing his homework./Xiaoming is very considerate. | |
Disrespect, Semantically Inconsistent | 30岁的小明关上门,/他对老师说:/“朱文轶,这是相关的材料。”/然后继续写作业。/小明非常懂事。 |
Gloss: Thirty-year-old Xiaoming closed the door,/ He said to the teacher:/“Zhu Wenyi[plain], this is the relevant material,”/Then continued writing homework./Xiaoming is very well-behaved. | |
Translation: Thirty-year-old Xiaoming closed the door,/said to the teacher,/“Zhu Wenyi[plain], this is the relevant material,”/and then continued doing his homework./Xiaoming is very considerate. |
Model Parameters | β | SE | df | t | p | 95%CI |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Respect | −0.03 | 0.02 | 74.40 | −1.70 | 0.09 | [−0.073, 0.006] |
Semantic Consistency | 0.12 | 0.03 | 64.50 | 4.67 | <0.01 | [0.067, 0.167] |
Respect * Semantic Consistency | 0.01 | 0.02 | 76.75 | 0.61 | 0.54 | [−0.023, 0.044] |
Model Parameters | β | SE | df | t | p | 95%CI |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Respect | 0.04 | 0.02 | 3012.00 | 2.57 | 0.01 | [0.009, 0.066] |
Semantic Consistency | 0.08 | 0.02 | 3012.20 | 5.36 | <0.01 | [0.049, 0.106] |
Respect * Semantic Consistency | 0.03 | 0.02 | 3011.88 | 2.20 | 0.03 | [0.003, 0.061] |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Yu, W.; Xie, Y.; Yang, X. Impact of Respectfulness on Semantic Integration During Discourse Processing. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 448. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040448
Yu W, Xie Y, Yang X. Impact of Respectfulness on Semantic Integration During Discourse Processing. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(4):448. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040448
Chicago/Turabian StyleYu, Wenjing, Yuhan Xie, and Xiaohong Yang. 2025. "Impact of Respectfulness on Semantic Integration During Discourse Processing" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 4: 448. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040448
APA StyleYu, W., Xie, Y., & Yang, X. (2025). Impact of Respectfulness on Semantic Integration During Discourse Processing. Behavioral Sciences, 15(4), 448. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040448