Bilingual Proficiency Effects on Word Recall and Recognition
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Language Proficiency Effect in Recall and Recognition
1.2. Models Account for Language Proficiency Paradox
1.3. False Recall in L1 and L2
1.4. Current Study
2. Experiment 1
2.1. Method
2.1.1. Participants
2.1.2. Material
2.1.3. Design and Procedure
2.1.4. Data Treatment
2.2. Result
2.2.1. Semantic Judgement Task
2.2.2. Word Recognition Task
- Response reaction times
- Signal detection theory’s measures
2.3. Discussion
3. Experiment 2
3.1. Method
3.1.1. Participants
3.1.2. Material, Design, Procedure, and Data Treatment
3.2. Result
3.2.1. Semantic Judgment Task
3.2.2. Picture Endorsement Task
- Response reaction times
- Signal detection theory measurements
3.2.3. Comparisons Across Experiments 1 and 2: Cue Effect
- Response reaction times
- Signal detection theory’s measures
3.3. Discussion
4. Experiment 3
4.1. Method
4.1.1. Participants
4.1.2. Material, Design, and Procedure
4.1.3. Data Treatment
4.2. Result
4.2.1. Semantic Judgement Task
4.2.2. Free Recall Task
4.3. Discussion
5. Discussion
5.1. L2 Advantages on Word Recognition
5.2. Bilingual Memory in Picture Endorsement Task
5.3. Similar Recall Accuracy and More False Recall in L1 vs. L2
5.4. Level-Based Bilingual Cognitive Resource Account
5.5. L1 and L2 Word Recognition in Chinese ESL Learners
6. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Abadie, M., & Camos, V. (2019). False memory at short and long term. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(8), 1312–1334. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Anastasi, J., Rhodes, M., Marquez, S., & Velino, V. (2005). The incidence of false memories in native and non-native speakers. Memory, 13(8), 815–828. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Anderson, J. R., & Bower, G. H. (1972). Recognition and retrieval processes in free recall. Psychological Review, 79(2), 97–123. [Google Scholar]
- Arndt, J., & Beato, M. S. (2017). The role of language proficiency in producing false memories. Journal of Memory and Language, 95, 146–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Atkinson, R. C., & Juola, J. F. (1974). Search and decision processes in recognition memory. In D. H. Krantz, R. C. Atkinson, & P. Suppes (Eds.), Contemporary developments in mathematical psychology (pp. 243–290). Freeman. [Google Scholar]
- Badham, S. P., Whitney, C., Sanghera, S., & Maylor, E. A. (2017). Word frequency influences on the list length effect and associative memory in young and older adults. Memory, 25(6), 816–830. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Balota, D. A., & Neely, J. H. (1980). Test-expectancy and word-frequency effects in recall and recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 576–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barron, R. W. (1986). Word recognition in early reading: A review of the direct and indirect access hypotheses. Cognition, 24(1–2), 93–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 1–48. [Google Scholar]
- Beato, M. S., & Arndt, J. (2021). The effect of language proficiency and associative strength on false memory. Psychological Research, 85(8), 3134–3151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bobb, S. C., & Wodniecka, Z. (2013). Language switching in picture naming: What asymmetric switch costs (do not) tell us about inhibition in bilingual speech planning. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 568–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bock, K., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1994). Language production: Grammatical encoding. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of Psycholinguistics (pp. 945–984). Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Boris, B. B., & Parker, C. P. (2000). fv. Journal of Business & Psychology, 15(2), 229–246. [Google Scholar]
- Brainerd, C. J., Bialer, D. M., & Chang, M. (2022). Fuzzy-trace theory and false memory: Meta-analysis of conjoint recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 48(11), 1680. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brandt, K. R., Cooper, L. M., & Dewhurst, S. A. (2005). Expertise and recollective experience: Recognition memory for familiar and unfamiliar academic subjects. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19(9), 1113–1125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Braze, D., & Gong, T. (2017). Orthography, word recognition, and reading. In E. Fernandez, & H. Cairns (Eds.), The handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 269–293). Wiley. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brysbaert, M., & Duyck, W. (2010). Is it time to leave behind the Revised Hierarchical Model of bilingual language processing after fifteen years of service? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(3), 359–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buchler, N. E., & Reder, L. M. (2007). Modeling age-related memory deficits: A two-parameter solution. Psychology and Aging, 22(1), 104–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cai, Q., & Brysbaert, M. (2010). SUBTLEX-CH: Chinese word and character frequencies based on film subtitles. PLoS ONE, 56, e10729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Calabria, M., Branzi, F. M., Marne, P., Hernández, M., & Costa, A. (2015). Age-related effects over bilingual language control and executive control. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 65–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caplan, D., & Waters, G. (2005). The relationship between age, processing speed, working memory capacity, and language comprehension. Memory, 13(3–4), 403–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caramazza, A. (1997). How many levels of processing are there in lexical access? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14(1), 177–208. [Google Scholar]
- Chen, X. (2023). Exploration on the Teaching Path of Integrating “Telling Chinese Stories” into College English. Frontiers in Educational Research, 6(4). [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chevalère, J., Lemaire, P., & Camos, V. (2020). Age-Related Changes in Verbal Working Memory Strategies. Experimental Aging Research, 46(2), 93–127. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Chikamatsu, N. (1996). The effects of L1 orthography on L2 word recognition: A study of American and Chinese learners of Japanese. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18(4), 403–432. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, J. (1962). The statistical power of abnormal-social psychological research: A review. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 65, 145–153. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Cop, U., Drieghe, D., & Duyck, W. (2015). Eye movement patterns in natural reading: A comparison of monolingual and bilingual reading of a novel. PLoS ONE, 10(8), e0134008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cox, G. E., Hemmer, P., Aue, W. R., & Criss, A. H. (2018). Information and processes underlying semantic and episodic memory across tasks, items, and individuals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(4), 545–590. [Google Scholar]
- Criss, A. H., Aue, W. R., & Smith, L. (2011). The effects of word frequency and context variability in cued recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 64(2), 119–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58(1), 17–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dennis, S., & Humphreys, M. S. (2001). A context noise model of episodic word recognition. Psychological Review, 108(2), 452–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dhaene, S., & Woumans, E. (2023). Text recall and use of advance organisers in first and second language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 45(1), 264–275. [Google Scholar]
- Diana, R. A., & Reder, L. M. (2006). The low-frequency encoding disadvantage: Word frequency affects processing demands. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(4), 805–815. [Google Scholar]
- Dijkstra, T. O. N., Wahl, A., Buytenhuijs, F., Van Halem, N., Al-Jibouri, Z., De Korte, M., & Rekké, S. (2019). Multilink: A computational model for bilingual word recognition and word translation. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(4), 657–679. [Google Scholar]
- Dijkstra, T., & van Heuven, W. J. B. (2002). The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5(3), 175–197. [Google Scholar]
- Dirix, N., Vander Beken, H., De Bruyne, E., Brysbaert, M., & Duyck, W. (2020). Reading text when studying in a second language: An eye-tracking study. Reading Research Quarterly, 55(3), 371–397. [Google Scholar]
- Dobbins, I. G., Kroll, N. E., Yonelinas, A. P., & Liu, Q. (1998). Distinctiveness in recognition and free recall: The role of recollection in the rejection of the familiar. Journal of Memory and Language, 38(4), 381–400. [Google Scholar]
- Durgunoǧlu, A. Y., & Roediger, H. L., III. (1987). Test differences in accessing bilingual memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 26(4), 377–391. [Google Scholar]
- Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–1160. [Google Scholar]
- Feng, G., Miller, K., Shu, H., & Zhang, H. (2001). Rowed to recovery: The use of phonological and orthographic information in reading Chinese and English. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(4), 1079–1100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fleisher, L. S., & Jenkins, J. R. (1978). Effects of contextualized and decontextualized practice conditions on word recognition. Learning Disability Quarterly, 1(3), 39–47. [Google Scholar]
- Flexser, A. J., & Tulving, E. (1978). Retrieval independence in recognition and recall. Psychological Review, 85(3), 153–171. [Google Scholar]
- Francis, W. S., & Baca, Y. (2014). Effects of language dominance on item and order memory in free recall, serial recall and order reconstruction. Memory, 22(8), 1060–1069. [Google Scholar]
- Francis, W. S., & Gutiérrez, M. (2012). Bilingual recognition memory: Stronger performance but weaker levels-of-processing effects in the less fluent language. Memory & Cognition, 40(3), 496–503. [Google Scholar]
- Francis, W. S., & Strobach, E. N. (2013). The bilingual L2 advantage in recognition memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(6), 1296–1303. [Google Scholar]
- Francis, W. S., Arteaga, M. M., Liaño, M. K., & Taylor, R. S. (2020). Temporal dynamics of free recall: The role of rehearsal efficiency in word frequency and bilingual language proficiency effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(8), 1477–1508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Francis, W. S., Strobach, E. N., Penalver, R. M., Martínez, M., Gurrola, B. V., & Soltero, A. (2019). Word-context associations in episodic memory are learned at the conceptual level: Word frequency, bilingual proficiency, and bilingual status effects on source memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45(10), 1852–1871. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Francis, W. S., Taylor, R. S., Gutiérrez, M., Liaño, M. K., Manzanera, D. G., & Penalver, R. M. (2018). The effects of bilingual language proficiency on recall accuracy and semantic clustering in free recall output: Evidence for shared semantic associations across languages. Memory, 26(10), 1364–1378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Freund, R. D., Brelsford, J. W., Jr., & Atkinson, R. C. (1969). Recognition vs. recall: Storage or retrieval differences? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 21(3), 214–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frost, R. (1998). Toward a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: True issues and false trails. Psychological Bulletin, 123(1), 71–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frost, R. (2012). A universal approach to modeling visual word recognition and reading: Not only possible, but also inevitable. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(5), 310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fu, C., Wang, Q., & Zhang, L. (Eds.). (2020). An overview of Chinese culture. China Renmin University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Gillund, G., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1984). A retrieval model for both recognition and recall. Psychological Review, 91(1), 1–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Glanzer, M., & Bowles, N. (1976). Analysis of the word-frequency effect in recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 2(1), 21–31. [Google Scholar]
- Glanzer, M., & Duarte, A. (1971). Repetition between and within languages in free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 625–630. [Google Scholar]
- Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Cera, C., & Sandoval, T. C. (2008). More use almost always means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(3), 787–814. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Grant, H. M., Bredahl, L. C., Clay, J., Ferrie, J., Groves, J. E., McDorman, T. A., & Dark, V. J. (1998). Context-dependent memory for meaningful material: Information for students. Applied Cognitive Psychology: The Official Journal of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 12(6), 617–623. [Google Scholar]
- Green, D., & Gabrys-Barker, D. (2018). The interactional challenge: L2 learning and use in the third age. In Third age learners of foreign languages (pp. 31–47). Multilingual Matters. [Google Scholar]
- Gregg, V. (1976). Word frequency, recognition and recall. In J. Brown (Ed.), Recall and recognition (pp. 183–215). John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
- Gurrola, B. V., & Francis, W. S. (2024). False memories in bilinguals: Integration of information across languages and limits on proficiency effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 32(1), 387–395. [Google Scholar]
- Haist, F., Shimamura, A. P., & Squire, L. R. (1992). On the relationship between recall and recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(4), 691–702. [Google Scholar]
- Han, J. I., & Kim, J. Y. (2017). The Influence of Orthography on the Production of Alphabetic, Second-Language Allophones by Speakers of a Non-alphabetic Language. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 46(4), 963–982. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Healy, A. F., & Kubovy, M. (1978). The effects of payoffs and prior probabilities on indices of performance and cutoff location in recognition memory. Memory & Cognition 6, 544–553. [Google Scholar]
- Heath, S. B. (1982). What no bedtime story means: Narrative skills at home and school. Language in Society, 11(1), 49–76. [Google Scholar]
- Hendry, L., & Tehan, G. (2005). An item/order trade-off explanation of word length and generation effects. Memory, 13(3–4), 364–371. [Google Scholar]
- Henson, R. N., Rugg, M. D., Shallice, T., Josephs, O., & Dolan, R. J. (1999). Recollection and familiarity in recognition memory: An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Neuroscience, 19(10), 3962–3972. [Google Scholar]
- Hintzman, D. L. (1976). Repetition and memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 10, 47–91. [Google Scholar]
- Hollingworth, H. L. (1913). Characteristic differences between recall and recognition. The American Journal of Psychology, 24(4), 532–544. [Google Scholar]
- Howe, M. L. (2006). Developmentally invariant dissociations in children’s true and false memories: Not all relatedness is created equal. Child Development, 77(4), 1112–1123. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Howe, M. L., Wimmer, M. C., Gagnon, N., & Plumpton, S. (2009). An associative-activation theory of children’s and adults’ memory illusions. Journal of Memory and Language, 60(2), 229–251. [Google Scholar]
- Hunt, R. R., & Einstein, G. O. (1981). Relational and item-specific information in memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 497–514. [Google Scholar]
- Jacoby, L. L. (1983). Remembering the data: Analyzing the interactive processes in reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 485–508. [Google Scholar]
- Jescheniak, J. D., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1994). Word frequency effects in speech production: Retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological form. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(4), 824–843. [Google Scholar]
- Jones, G. V. (1984). Analyzing recognition and recall. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7(2), 242–243. [Google Scholar]
- Kimball, D. R., Smith, T. A., & Kahana, M. J. (2007). The fSAM model of false recall. Psychological Review, 114(4), 954. [Google Scholar]
- Kintsch, W. (1968). Recognition and free recall of organized lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 78(3), 481–487. [Google Scholar]
- Koda, K. (1996). L2 word recognition research: A critical review. The Modern Language Journal, 80(4), 450–460. [Google Scholar]
- Kroll, J. F., & Stewart, E. (1994). Category Interference in Translation and Picture Naming: Evidence for Asymmetric Connections Between Bilingual Memory Representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33(2), 149–174. [Google Scholar]
- Kuo, W. J., Yeh, T. C., Lee, J. R., Chen, L. F., Lee, P. L., Chen, S. S., & Hsieh, J. C. (2004). Orthographic and phonological processing of Chinese characters: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 21(4), 1721–1731. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1–26. [Google Scholar]
- Kühberger, A., Fritz, A., & Scherndl, T. (2014). Publication bias in psychology: A diagnosis based on the correlation between effect size and sample size. PLoS ONE, 9(9), e105825. [Google Scholar]
- Li, D., & Yuan, L. (2023). Research and practice of telling Chinese stories in English in college english teaching. Frontiers in Educational Research, 6(18), 47–52. [Google Scholar]
- Liao, H. Y. (Ed.). (2022). A glimpse of Chinese culture (Revised ed.). Foreign Language Teaching and Research Press. [Google Scholar]
- Lim, L. C. L., & Goh, W. D. (2019). False recognition modality effects in short-term memory: Reversing the auditory advantage. Cognition, 193, 104008. [Google Scholar]
- Liu, R., Li, W., Xiong, B., Zhang, Z., & Yan, L. (2024). Navigating the Mind’s Bilingual Maze: Understanding Cognitive Advantages Across Variables. Lecture Notes in Education Psychology and Public Media, 46, 115–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lohnas, L. J., & Kahana, M. J. (2013). Parametric effects of word frequency in memory for mixed frequency lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory & Cognition, 39(6), 1943–1946. [Google Scholar]
- Ma, D. G. (Ed.). (2022). Senior high school English vocabulary. Zhejiang Education Publishing House. [Google Scholar]
- Ma, X., Ma, X., Li, P., & Liu, Y. (2020). Differences in working memory with emotional distraction between proficient and non-proficient bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1414. [Google Scholar]
- Macizo, P., Bajo, T., & Paolieri, D. (2012). Language switching and language competition. Second Language Research, 28(2), 131–149. [Google Scholar]
- MacLeod, C. M., & Kampe, K. E. (1996). Word frequency effects on recall, recognition, and word fragment completion tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(1), 132–142. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Macmillan, N. A., & Kaplan, H. L. (1985). Detection theory analysis of group data: Estimating sensitivity from average hit and false-alarm rates. Psychological Bulletin, 98(1), 185–199. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Malmberg, K. J., & Nelson, T. O. (2003). The word frequency effect for recognition memory and the elevated-attention hypothesis. Memory & Cognition, 31(1), 35–43. [Google Scholar]
- Manard, M., Carabin, D., Jaspar, M., & Collette, F. (2014). Age-related decline in cognitive control: The role of fluid intelligence and processing speed. BMC Neuroscience, 15, 7. [Google Scholar]
- Mandler, G. (1980). Recognizing: The judgment of previous occurrence. Psychological Review, 87(3), 252–271. [Google Scholar]
- Mandler, G., Goodman, G. O., & Wilkes-Gibbs, D. L. (1982). The word-frequency paradox in recognition. Memory & Cognition, 10(1), 33–42. [Google Scholar]
- Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(4), 940–967. [Google Scholar]
- Mattay, V. S., Fera, F., Tessitore, A., Hariri, A. R., Berman, K. F., Das, S., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Goldberg, T. E., Callicott, J. H., & Weinberger, D. R. (2006). Neurophysiological correlates of age-related changes in working memory capacity. Neuroscience Letters, 392(1–2), 32–37. [Google Scholar]
- Messer, M. H., Verhagen, J., Boom, J., Mayo, A. Y., & Leseman, P. P. (2015). Growth of verbal short-term memory of nonwords varying in phonotactic probability: A longitudinal study with monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Memory and Language, 84, 24–36. [Google Scholar]
- Meuter, R. F., & Allport, A. (1999). Bilingual language switching in naming: Asymmetrical costs of language selection. Journal of Memory and Language, 40(1), 25–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miller, A. C., & Keenan, J. M. (2011). Understanding the centrality deficit: Insight from foreign language learners. Memory & Cognition, 39(5), 873–883. [Google Scholar]
- Mizrahi, R., Wixted, J., & Gollan, T. (2021). Order effects in bilingual recognition memory partially confirm predictions of the frequency-lag hypothesis. Memory, 29(4), 444–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Myung, J. I., Montenegro, M., & Pitt, M. A. (2007). Analytic Expressions for the BCDMEM Model of Recognition Memory. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 51(3), 198–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nation, I. S., & Nation, I. S. P. (2001). Learning vocabulary in another language (Vol. 10, pp. 126–132). Cambridge university press. [Google Scholar]
- Ning, X., Li, C., & Yang, J. (2018). Word familiarity modulated the effects of category familiarity on memory performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Norman, K. A., & O’Reilly, R. C. (2003). Modeling hippocampal and neocortical contributions to recognition memory: A complementary-learning-systems approach. Psychological Review, 110(4), 611–646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nott, C. R., & Lambert, W. E. (1968). Free Recall of Bilinguals. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 7(6), 1065–1071. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oxford, R. L., & Scarcella, R. C. (1994). Second language vocabulary learning among adults: State of the art in vocabulary instruction. System, 22(2), 231–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ozubko, J. D., & Joordens, S. (2007). The mixed truth about frequency effects on free recall: Effects of study list composition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(5), 871–876. [Google Scholar]
- Payne, J. D., Schacter, D. L., Propper, R. E., Huang, L. W., Wamsley, E. J., Tucker, M. A., Walker, M. P., & Stickgold, R. (2009). The role of sleep in false memory formation. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 92(3), 327–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peirce, J., Gray, J. R., Simpson, S., MacAskill, M., Hochenberger, R., Sogo, H., & Lindeløv, J. K. (2019). PsychoPy2: Experiments in behavior made easy. Behavior Research Methods 51, 195–203. [Google Scholar]
- Perbal, S., Droit-Volet, S., Isingrini, M., & Pouthas, V. (2002). Relationships between age-related changes in time estimation and age-related changes in processing speed, attention, and memory. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 9(3), 201–216. [Google Scholar]
- Perfetti, C., & Stafura, J. (2014). Word knowledge in a theory of reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18(1), 22–37. [Google Scholar]
- Popov, V., & Reder, L. M. (2020). Frequency effects on memory: A resource-limited theory. Psychological Review, 127(1), 1–46. [Google Scholar]
- Popov, V., So, M., & Reder, L. (2019). Word frequency affects binding probability not memory precision. PsyArXiv. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Qian, D. D. (1996). ESL vocabulary acquisition: Contextualization and decontextualization. Canadian Modern Language Review, 53(1), 120–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Core Team. (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing [computer software]. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [Google Scholar]
- Raaijmakers, J. G., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1981). Search of associative memory. Psychological Review, 88(2), 93–134. [Google Scholar]
- Ratcliff, R., Clark, S. E., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1990). List-strength effect: I. Data and discussion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(2), 163–178. [Google Scholar]
- Reder, L. M., Nhouyvanisvong, A., Schunn, C. D., Ayers, M. S., Angstadt, P., & Hiraki, K. (2000). A mechanistic account of the mirror effect for word frequency: A computational model of remember-know judgments in a continuous recognition paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(2), 294–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rhodes, S., Greene, N. R., & Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2019). Age-related differences in recall and recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(5), 1529–1547. [Google Scholar]
- Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(4), 803–814. [Google Scholar]
- Sahlin, B. H., Harding, M. G., & Seamon, J. G. (2005). When do false memories cross language boundaries in English—Spanish bilinguals? Memory & Cognition, 33, 1414–1421. [Google Scholar]
- Sánchez-Pérez, M. D. M., & Manzano-Agugliaro, F. (2021). Worldwide trends in bilingual education research: A half-century overview. Education Sciences, 11(11), 730. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Serino, S., Actis-Grosso, R., Maisto, M., Ricciardelli, P., & Steca, P. (2025). Emotion in action: A study on the enactment effect on emotional action sentences. Cognition, 254, 105974. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shiffrin, R. M., Ratcliff, R., & Clark, S. E. (1990). List-strength effect: II. Theoretical mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(2), 179–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sirotin, Y. B., Kimball, D. R., & Kahana, M. J. (2005). Going beyond a single list:Modeling the effects of prior experience on episodic free recall. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(5), 787–805. [Google Scholar]
- Sıtkı, M., Ikier, S., & Şener, N. (2024). Reduced false memory in the second language of Turkish-English bilinguals. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Snodgrass, J. G., & Corwin, J. (1988). Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: Applications to dementia and amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117(1), 34–50. [Google Scholar]
- Srivastava, J. (2013). Media multitasking performance: Role of message relevance and formatting cues in online environments. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(3), 888–895. [Google Scholar]
- Suarez, M., & Beato, M. S. (2023). False memory in a second language: The importance of controlling the knowledge of word meaning. PLoS ONE, 18(5), e0285747. [Google Scholar]
- Thomas, M. S., & Allport, A. (2000). Language switching costs in bilingual visual word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 43(1), 44–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Toth, J. P. (1996). Conceptual automaticity in recognition memory: Levels-of-processing effects on familiarity. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 50(1), 123–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. M. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80(5), 352–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tzeng, O. J., & Hung, D. L. (1978). Reading the Chinese character:Some basic research. Acta Psychologica Taiwanica, 20(1), 45–49. [Google Scholar]
- Ünaldi, İ., Bardakci, M., Akpinar, K. D., & Dolaş, F. (2013). A comparison of contextualized, decontextualized and corpus-informed vocabulary instruction: A quasi-experimental study. Dil ve Edebiyat Eğitimi Dergisi, 2(8), 78–95. [Google Scholar]
- Van Kesteren, M. T. R., Rijpkema, M., Ruiter, D. J., Morris, R. G. M., & Fernandez, G. (2014). Building on prior knowledge: Schema-dependent encoding processes relate to academic performance. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26, 2250–2261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vander Beken, H., & Brysbaert, M. (2018). Studying texts in a second language: The importance of test type. Bilingualism-Language and Cognition, 21(5), 1062–1074. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, J., Otgaar, H., Howe, M. L., Lippe, F., & Smeets, T. (2018). The nature and consequences of false memories for visual stimuli. Journal of Memory and Language, 101, 124–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, W. S. Y. (1973). The Chinese language. Scientific American, 228(2), 50–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, Y., & Gennari, S. P. (2019). How language and event recall can shape memory for time. Cognitive Psychology, 108, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watkins, M. J., LeCompte, D. C., & Kim, K. (2000). Role of study strategy in recall of mixed lists of common and rare words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 26(1), 239–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wen, Y., Filik, R., & Van Heuven, W. J. (2018). Electrophysiological dynamics of Chinese phonology during visual word recognition in Chinese-English bilinguals. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 6869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Whitford, V., & Titone, D. (2012). Second-language experience modulates first-and second-language word frequency effects: Evidence from eye movement measures of natural paragraph reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19(1), 73–80. [Google Scholar]
- Wilma, A., Bainbridge, Elizabeth, H., Hall, Chris, I., & Baker. (2019). Drawings of real-world scenes during free recall reveal detailed object and spatial information in memory. Nature Communications, 10, 5. [Google Scholar]
- Yonelinas, A. P., Otten, L. J., Shaw, K. N., & Rugg, M. D. (2005). Separating the brain regions involved in recollection and familiarity in recognition memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(11), 3002–3008. [Google Scholar]
- Yoo, J., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2016). Serial-position effects on a free-recall task in bilinguals. Memory, 24(3), 409–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
CH List | EN List | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Chinese History | English History | Chinese History | English History | |||||
M (SD) | Range | M (SD) | Range | M (SD) | Range | M (SD) | Range | |
Experiment 1: Word Recognition | ||||||||
Age at Learning Start | 1.14 (0.91) | 0–3 | 7.97 (1.97) | 3–12 | 1.01 (1.07) | 0–4 | 7.52 (1.95) | 3–10 |
Language Exposure (%time) | 0.84 (0.12) | 0.5–1 | 0.11 (0.09) | 0–0.4 | 0.79 (0.14) | 0.5–1 | 0.21 (0.17) | 0.01–0.9 |
Order of Language Dominance | 1 (0) | 1–1 | 2.14 (0.35) | 2–3 | 1 (0) | 1–1 | 2.23 (0.43) | 2–3 |
Self-reported Speaking Proficiency | 9.28 (1.07) | 6–10 | 5.86 (1.53) | 1–8 | 9.29 (1.61) | 4–10 | 6.45 (1.67) | 3–10 |
Self-reported Understanding Proficiency | 9.24 (1.15) | 6–10 | 5.34 (1.4) | 1–8 | 9.32 (1.4) | 5–10 | 6.13 (1.84) | 3–10 |
Self-reported Reading Proficiency | 9.21 (0.94) | 7–10 | 7.1 (1.32) | 1–8 | 9.16 (1.49) | 5–10 | 6.94 (1.31) | 3–10 |
Self-reported Writing Proficiency | 8.38 (1.61) | 3–10 | 6.38 (1.52) | 3–9 | 8.65 (1.7) | 5–10 | 6.23 (1.78) | 3–10 |
Experiment 2: Picture Endorsement | ||||||||
Age at Learning Start | 0.88 (0.75) | 0–3 | 7.16 (2.07) | 3–12 | 1.21 (0.90) | 0–3 | 7.31 (2.48) | 2–11 |
Language Exposure (%time) | 0.81 (0.11) | 0.60–0.97 | 0.10 (0.07) | 0.01–0.30 | 0.80 (0.13) | 0.60–0.99 | 0.07 (0.06) | 0.00–0.03 |
Order of Language Dominance | 1 (0.00) | 1–1 | 2.23 (0.43) | 2–3 | 1 (0.00) | 1–1 | 2.21 (0.41) | 2–3 |
Self-reported Speaking Proficiency | 9.27 (1.05) | 7–10 | 5.73 (1.85) | 2–10 | 9.14 (1.43) | 5–10 | 5.72 (2.09) | 1–9 |
Self-reported Understanding Proficiency | 9.18 (1.11) | 6–10 | 5.60 (1.77) | 3–10 | 9.34 (1.17) | 5–10 | 5.55 (1.66) | 3–9 |
Self-reported Reading Proficiency | 8.92 (1.27) | 5–10 | 6.87 (1.75) | 3–10 | 9.00 (1.34) | 5–10 | 6.97 (1.92) | 3–9 |
Self-reported Writing Proficiency | 8.58 (1.46) | 6–10 | 5.92 (1.89) | 2–10 | 7.93 (1.87) | 3–10 | 6.17 (1.83) | 2–9 |
Experiment 3: Word Recall | ||||||||
Age at Learning Start | 1.20 (0.93) | 0–3 | 6.68 (2.13) | 3–12 | 0.92 (0.80) | 0–3 | 6.69 (2.33) | 2–10 |
Language Exposure (%time) | 0.83 (0.12) | 0.50–0.99 | 0.09 (0.07) | 0.01–0.30 | 0.85 (0.12) | 0.50–0.99 | 0.10 (0.09) | 0.01–0.50 |
Order of Language Dominance | 1 (0) | 1–1 | 2.24 (0.44) | 2–8 | 1 (0) | 1–1 | 2.08 (0.26) | 2–3 |
Self-reported Speaking Proficiency | 9.30 (1.11) | 6–10 | 5.77 (1.64) | 2–10 | 9.28 (1.02) | 6–10 | 6.34 (1.56) | 1–10 |
Self-reported Understanding Proficiency | 9.33 (1.11) | 6–10 | 5.47 (1.87) | 2–9 | 9.05 (1.05) | 7–10 | 5.98 (1.22) | 3–7 |
Self-reported Reading Proficiency | 9.15 (1.09) | 6–10 | 6.97 (1.33) | 2–9 | 9.06 (0.97) | 7–10 | 7.36 (1.06) | 3–7 |
Self-reported Writing Proficiency | 8.61 (1.52) | 4–10 | 5.53 (1.44) | 2–8 | 8.13 (1.43) | 5–10 | 6.27 (1.14) | 4–8 |
Dependent Variable | Fixed Effect | Estimated Coefficient | Standard Error | z | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Experiment 1 | |||||
Accuracy | Intercept | 2.74 | 0.19 | 14.21 | *** |
Language | −1.01 | 0.31 | −3.28 | ** | |
Rts | Intercept | 1.00 | 0.02 | 54.83 | *** |
Language | 0.23 | 0.03 | 6.71 | *** | |
Experiment 2 | |||||
Accuracy | Intercept | 3.49 | 0.26 | 13.66 | *** |
Language | −1.19 | 0.37 | −3.22 | ** | |
Rts | Intercept | 1.01 | 0.02 | 49.19 | *** |
Language | 0.30 | 0.04 | 7.99 | *** | |
Experiment 3 | |||||
Accuracy | Intercept | 3.30 | 0.25 | 13.27 | *** |
Language | −0.69 | 0.29 | −2.37 | * | |
Rts | Intercept | 0.87 | 0.02 | 45.21 | *** |
Language | 0.25 | 0.03 | 8.74 | *** |
Dependent Variable | Fixed Effect | Estimated Coefficient | Standard Error | z | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Exp. 1: Word Recognition | |||||
Rts (experimental items) | Intercept | 0.95 | 0.02 | 40.73 | *** |
Language | −0.06 | 0.05 | −1.24 | 0.22 | |
Rts (foil items) | Intercept | 1.05 | 0.03 | 34.54 | *** |
Language | 0.02 | 0.06 | 0.39 | 0.70 | |
Exp. 2: Picture Endorsement | |||||
Rts (experimental items) | Intercept | 1.22 | 0.03 | 39.64 | *** |
Language | 0.02 | 0.05 | 0.29 | 0.769 | |
Rts (foil items) | Intercept | 1.49 | 0.04 | 33.66 | *** |
Language | 0.12 | 0.08 | 1.49 | 0.142 | |
Exp. 3: Free Recall | |||||
Accuracy | Intercept | −1.24 | 0.20 | −6.36 | *** |
Language | 0.35 | 0.27 | 1.31 | 0.192 |
Hit Rate | FA Rate | d’ | |
---|---|---|---|
Exp1: Word Recognition | |||
CH list | 0.72 (0.02) | 0.17 (0.01) | 1.63 (0.07) |
EN list | 0.87 (0.01) | 0.11 (0.01) | 2.58 (0.09) |
Exp2: Picture Endorsement | |||
CH list | 0.66 (0.02) | 0.25 (0.02) | 1.21 (0.07) |
EN list | 0.79 (0.01) | 0.18 (0.02) | 1.87 (0.09) |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Wang, Y.; Yang, K.; Zhou, S.; Zhang, H.; Ma, T.; Shi, X.; Ma, W. Bilingual Proficiency Effects on Word Recall and Recognition. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 437. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040437
Wang Y, Yang K, Zhou S, Zhang H, Ma T, Shi X, Ma W. Bilingual Proficiency Effects on Word Recall and Recognition. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(4):437. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040437
Chicago/Turabian StyleWang, Yaqi, Kai Yang, Simin Zhou, Hao Zhang, Tinghui Ma, Xiujuan Shi, and Wen Ma. 2025. "Bilingual Proficiency Effects on Word Recall and Recognition" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 4: 437. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040437
APA StyleWang, Y., Yang, K., Zhou, S., Zhang, H., Ma, T., Shi, X., & Ma, W. (2025). Bilingual Proficiency Effects on Word Recall and Recognition. Behavioral Sciences, 15(4), 437. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040437