1. Introduction
Although in the past, neurodivergent individuals faced systematic exclusion from the labor force due to their different ways of thinking, nowadays we see a significant shift in recognizing their indispensable role in the workforce, as neurodivergent individuals make up an aggregated 22% of the population (
Doyle, 2020;
Weber et al., 2022). Particularly in innovation-oriented organizations, there is a growing acknowledgment of the necessity to accommodate neurodivergent employees and develop inclusive work practices to address the challenges stemming from these conditions in order to take advantage of their unique work perspectives (
Krzeminska et al., 2019).
Neurodivergent employees display particular information processing patterns and are thought to have differently wired brains than neurotypical employees (
Krzeminska et al., 2019;
Roberts et al., 2012). Beyond the challenges raised by these distinct cognitive patterns and information processing tendencies, neurodivergent employees also bring unique perspectives that have the potential to enhance creative performance, especially when neurotypical and neurodivergent employees work together and exchange their different perspectives on the task (
van Rijswijk et al., 2025). However, due to stigma and prejudices associated with neurodivergent conditions, neurodivergent employees often mask their condition and do not seek a formal diagnosis.
Fellowes (
2024) identified three key reasons why many neurodivergent individuals do not have a formal diagnosis: (1) official diagnosis is not accessible due to long waiting lists, a lower ability to navigate the healthcare system, or prohibitive costs, (2) neurodivergent individuals do not seek a formal diagnosis because it brings the risk of social stigmatization, and (3) formal diagnosis is less accurate than self-identification. Due to the personal and social challenges faced throughout life by many neurodivergent employees, their self-awareness concerning their diagnosis is often accurate (
Fellowes, 2024;
Overton et al., 2023). As such, the prevalence of neurodivergence in organizations could be higher, as employees may choose not to seek a formal diagnosis, yet they are well aware of their neurodivergent condition.
Although the literature claims that neurodivergent employees contribute with their different cognitive styles and competencies to the task (
Artamoshina et al., 2023;
Cope & Remington, 2022), we know little about such differences in organizational settings (
van Rijswijk et al., 2024). Moreover, because scholars who investigate the integration of neurodivergent employees in work environments predominantly rely on formal diagnoses and thereby inadvertently overlook a substantial cohort of undiagnosed neurodivergent individuals who are improperly categorized as neurotypical, it becomes important to understand whether self-identification of neurodivergence adds to the predictive power of formal diagnosis in relation to cognitive differences.
The main aim of our paper is, therefore, to investigate the extent to which self-identified neurodivergence adds to the predictive power of formal diagnosis concerning such differences. More specifically, our study evaluates differences in decision-making styles, rationality, and cognitive styles among neurodivergent and neurotypical employees, and explores the extent to which the self-identified neurodivergent conditions add to the initial predictive power of the formal diagnosis of neurodivergence. By doing so, our paper presents essential insights that can be used to assess neurodivergence in organizational research, empowering scholars to pursue more accurate research on cognitive differences tied to neurodivergence in work settings.
2. Theoretical Background
Neurodivergence is marked by distinct cognitive wiring compared to the neurotypical majority (
Krzeminska et al., 2019;
Roberts et al., 2012). This cognitive diversity is a natural outcome of evolutionary progress and results in neurodivergent individuals using information in unique ways that manifest itself in the encoding (i.e., acquiring information), retention (i.e., remembering information), and retrieval (i.e., processing information into new outcomes) stages of information processing (
Miller, 1956). While detailing each stage for every neurodivergent condition would be exhaustive, briefly describing some variances compared to neurotypical individuals is worthwhile. During the encoding stage, neurodivergent individuals often encounter challenges in gathering information. People with conditions like autism and ADHD may absorb vast amounts of information but can struggle with the interpretation and prioritization of it (
Remington et al., 2012). Conversely, dyslexia and dyscalculia primarily hinder the absorption of information, especially in written forms (
Landerl et al., 2009), and environmental cues can obstruct information retrieval for individuals with a heightened sensitivity that is common for highly gifted and autistic people (
Johnsen, 2021;
Weber et al., 2022). Information retention is for ADHD-diagnosed people often unstructured as part of the difficulties in making the distinction between relevant and irrelevant information (
Roberts et al., 2012). However, in many cases of autism, this priority problem leads to high levels of information storage on topics of interest to that person (
Remington et al., 2012). The perceived differences in information encoding and retention lead neurodivergent individuals to unexpected outcomes in the information retrieval stage, resulting in both advantages and disadvantages in work settings. For example, the unstructured form of information encoding and retention in the case of ADHD often leads to failure to follow established organizational procedures, yet, at the same time, may also result in creative insights and inspiration for further innovations (
Artamoshina et al., 2023). Conversely, autism often leads to information processing difficulties in ever-changing environments, yet these employees often excel in tasks that require great attention to detail (
Cope & Remington, 2022).
Due to the consequences of the unique cognitive wiring of neurodivergent individuals, reflected in different decision-making styles, rationality, and thinking patterns, they often encounter challenges as employees. However, in practice, a significant number of these employees remain undiagnosed (
Fellowes, 2024;
Lewis, 2016). Sometimes, individuals display only a spectrum of characteristics of a neurological condition and do not meet the formal criteria for diagnosis. Moreover, research indicates that in certain populations, diagnosticians often lack sufficient knowledge of the characteristics of neurodivergent conditions in these populations, leading to misdiagnosis or a complete lack of diagnosis (
Hartung & Lefler, 2019). Furthermore, other neurodivergent employees may consciously avoid seeking a diagnosis as a protective measure against potential social stigmatization (
Fellowes, 2024;
Pryke-Hobbes et al., 2023).
The lack of formal diagnoses complicates efforts to conduct comprehensive research on neurodivergence in work environments. We, therefore, set out to explore the extent to which self-identified neurodivergence has additional predictive power to the formally diagnosed neurodivergence on the cognitive functioning of employees, and we take into account differences in decision-making styles, cognitive styles, as well as cognitive competencies. We expect that self-identified neurodivergence adds significantly to the predictive power of the models concerning differences in cognitive functioning between neurotypical and neurodivergent employees.
3. Methods
We employed a snowball approach to gather data for our study, reaching out to diverse organizational teams as part of a broader study on cognitive differences in group dynamics. We start by briefing team managers individually on the study’s purpose and procedure, asking for their assistance and consent. Subsequently, with their commitment and approval, we distributed the digital survey by e-mail to the team members. After a few weeks, we sent a short reminder to enhance the volume of our sample. In total, we collected data from 357 participants across 19 organizations in 7 industries (1 organization in banking, 5 in healthcare, 1 in pensions and insurance, 2 in education, 1 in employee recruitment, 7 in IT, and 2 in the construction industry), all located in The Netherlands and Bonaire. The sample included 43 participants who reported being formally diagnosed with a neurodivergence condition and 95 reported that they self-identify with at least one of the neurodivergence conditions. These results are aligned with previous research showing that the incidence of neurodivergence is much higher than what is indicated through formal diagnoses as people avoid the risk of stigmatization tied to a formal diagnosis for a neurodivergent condition or simply because the availability of specialized diagnosing services is low or too expensive (
Fellowes, 2024).
Decision-making styles were evaluated using the General Decision-Making Style Inventory (
Scott & Bruce, 1995). Five items evaluated each of the following decision-making styles: rational (item example “I double-check my information sources to be sure I have the right facts before making decisions”), intuitive (item example “When I make decisions, I tend to rely on my intuition”), dependent (item example “I often need the assistance of other people when making important decisions”), avoidant (item example “I postpone decision-making whenever possible”), and spontaneous (item example “I often make decisions on the spur of the moment”). Previous research extensively tested the reliability and validity of this questionnaire (
Curşeu & Schruijer, 2012;
Spicer & Sadler-Smith, 2005) as an accurate indicator of decision-making tendencies.
Cognitive styles were evaluated using the Verbalizer–Visualizer Questionnaire (
Richardson, 1977), which was extensively used to assess individual preferences in visual versus verbal information processing. For the verbal cognitive style, examples of items include: “I enjoy doing work that requires the use of words”, while for the visual cognitive style, examples of items include “My thinking often consists of mental pictures or images” (answered as true or false).
Rationality was evaluated using the three items of the Cognitive Reflection Test (
Frederick, 2005), which provides an objective measure of a lower-order factor of general mental ability (
Blacksmith et al., 2019). Previous results have extensively documented the usefulness of this method for assessing participants’ capacity to engage in analytic and deliberate information processing (
Hoppe & Kusterer, 2011;
Kahan, 2013).
Diagnosed neurodivergence was evaluated by asking participants whether they were formally diagnosed with one of the following conditions (“Which of the following conditions have third parties formally diagnosed about you?”, answered as yes or no for the diagnoses autism, ADHD, ADD, DCD, dyslexia, dyscalculia, and high giftedness) and entered in the analyses as a combines dummy variable (1 = at least one presence versus 0 = absence).
Self-awareness of neurodivergence was evaluated by asking participants whether they believe they have one of the neurodivergent traits associated with the following conditions: (“Which of the following conditions do you personally think apply to you?”, answered as yes or no for the diagnoses autism, ADHD, ADD, DCD, dyslexia, dyscalculia, and high giftedness) and entered in the analyses as a combined dummy variable (1 = at least one presence versus 0 = absence).
Education was evaluated as a categorical variable by asking participants to specify their highest completed level of education: (1) vocational education; (2) secondary education; (3) bachelor’s degree from a university of applied sciences; (4) master’s degree from a university of applied sciences or bachelor’s degree from a research university; (5) master’s degree from a research university; (6) and doctoral degree.
To preserve anonymity, we did not collect demographic information, except for education, which was deemed relevant as a control variable because educational achievements can be related to neurodivergent conditions and because education is correlated with cognitive competencies, cognitive functioning, and information processing styles (
Guerra-Carrillo et al., 2017).
4. Results
Means, standard deviations, and correlations among the study variables are presented in
Table 1.
We have used stepwise OLS regression analyses to explore the predictive value of the neurodivergent conditions assessed in our research. In the first step of the regression, we entered education as a control variable and the formally diagnosed neurodivergent conditions, except for dyscalculia and DCD, which were only present in the self-identified answer set. Overall, 43 participants (12.1%) reported at least one formally diagnosed neurodivergent condition, while 95 participants (26.7%) reported self-awareness of at least one neurodivergent condition. These percentages are in line with previously reported incidences of neurodivergence in the general population (
Weber et al., 2022). Neurodivergent conditions were entered as dummy variables, with the neurotypical employees serving as a general reference category. Because the predictors included in the two steps of the regression analyses were expected to be correlated, we explored the variance inflation scores to check for multicollinearity problems in our analyses. None of the scores exceeded 1.6; therefore, we can conclude that multicollinearity is not a problem in the regression analyses. The results of the regression analyses for the five decision-making styles are presented in
Table 2, while the results for rationality and cognitive styles are presented in
Table 3.
Education had a negative association with the intuitive style (β = −0.11, p = 0.04) and a positive association with the dependent (β = 0.13, p = 0.02) and avoidant (β = 0.12, p = 0.03) decision-making styles, rationality (β = 0.19, p < 0.001), as well as with the verbal cognitive styles (β = 0.14, p = 0.01). Self-identified high giftedness had a positive association with the rational decision-making style (β = 0.12, p = 0.04) and rationality (β = 0.14, p = 0.02). Self-identified autism had a negative association with the intuitive decision-making style (β = −0.22, p < 0.001). Formally diagnosed ADHD had a positive association with the spontaneous decision-making style (β = 0.16, p = 0.008), a positive association that was observed for self-identified ADHD as well (β = 0.16, p = 0.01). Moreover, self-identified ADHD had a positive association with the verbal cognitive style (β = 0.17, p = 0.009). Formally diagnosed ADD had a negative association with the rational decision-making style (β = −0.27, p < 0.001), an association that was not observed for self-identified ADD (β = −0.23, p < 0.001). Self-identified ADD had a positive association with the avoidant (β = 0.14, p = 0.01) and with the spontaneous decision-making style (β = 0.12, p = 0.03). Formally diagnosed dyslexia had a negative association with intuitive (β = −0.13, p = 0.04) and spontaneous decision-making styles (β = −0.13, p = 0.048), an association that was not observed for self-identified dyslexia. Self-identified dyslexia, however, was negatively associated with the verbal cognitive style (β = −0.23, p < 0.001). Self-identified dyscalculia had a significant negative association with rationality (β = −0.12, p = 0.02).
5. Discussion
Our study evaluates differences in decision-making styles, rationality, and cognitive styles among neurodivergent versus neurotypical employees by considering both formally diagnosed and self-identified conditions. We argued that employees are often aware of their neurodivergence, yet they do not seek formal diagnosis due to the inaccessibility of healthcare, high costs, or the fear of being marginalized and excluded at work (
Fellowes, 2024). As expected, the number of self-identified neurodivergence in the sample was more than double the number of participants who reported a formal diagnosis. Given the incidence of self-identified neurodivergence, we expected (and observed) alignment between formally diagnosed and self-identified conditions in predicting the cognitive outcomes, and we also expected that the self-identified conditions would add to the predictive power of formally diagnosed conditions.
Effects of neurodivergent conditions on decision-making styles were generally aligned with previous empirical results. A formal diagnosis of ADD significantly and negatively predicted the rational decision-making style and the self-identification did not add to the predictive value of the formal one. This is consistent with prior studies that reported rational dysfunction when decision-making requires cognitive control (
Mäntylä et al., 2012;
Roberts et al., 2012). Moreover, consistent with the extensive decision-making strategies employed by highly gifted individuals (
Ball et al., 1994), self-identified high giftedness was significantly associated with rational decision-making. The intuitive decision-making style was negatively predicted by education and by autism, with self-identified autism qualifying the negative association of the formal diagnosis. Such a pattern of results is due to the logical and systematic information processing characteristic of autism (
Cope & Remington, 2022). Similarly, both formally diagnosed and self-identified ADHD were significantly and positively linked with spontaneous decision-making, aligning with the impulsivity associated with ADHD (
Faraone et al., 2000;
Roberts et al., 2012). The spontaneous decision-making style was also positively associated with self-identified ADD (which is explainable since ADD and ADHD share mostly similar characteristics and are often aggregated in studies) and negatively associated with formally diagnosed dyslexia. The dependent decision-making style was only predicted positively by education. The avoidant decision-making style was predicted positively by the formally diagnosed and self-identified ADD, aligning with the avoidance and procrastination failure patterns highlighted by cognitive behavioral models in prior research on this condition (
Knouse & Safren, 2010;
Roberts et al., 2012). The results concerning decision-making styles are aligned with the cognitive differences associated with different neurodivergent conditions and show the relevance of using self-identification in organizational studies focused on the impact of neurodivergence on work processes.
Rationality was positively predicted by education and self-identified high giftedness and negatively predicted by dyscalculia. These results are fully aligned with the cognitive performance differences reported in previous studies on the differences between dyslexia and dyscalculia (
Landerl et al., 2009) and point out the relevance of self-identified high giftedness that qualifies the effect of the formally diagnosed high giftedness (which becomes insignificant in Model 2). Concerning cognitive styles, education and ADHD positively predict the verbal style that is also negatively predicted by self-identified dyslexia. The only significant predictor of the visual style was self-identified dyslexia. The results for rationality and cognitive styles also highlight the relevance of self-identification in empirical neurodivergence research.
Future research should explore the differences between formally diagnosed and self-identified conditions, using larger data sets to allow for more robust conclusions at the level of specific conditions. A possible explanation for these differences is that scholars have noted considerable diversity in diagnostic characteristics, influenced by demographic factors (
Hartung & Lefler, 2019). The group of formally diagnosed individuals may be more cognitively homogeneous than the self-identified group, as the need for a formal diagnosis may stem from the fact that these individuals more frequently experience deficits in cognitive tasks or social settings. In addition, larger data sets may also enable future research to explore the impact of co-occurrence of neurodivergent conditions on the predictive value of self-identification.
6. Limitations
The first limitation of our paper is the self-reported nature of the data. We asked participants to report both their formally diagnosed and self-identified neurodivergent conditions. Future research could use independent clinical evaluations to distinguish between formal diagnoses and self-identification in research on neurodivergence, as well as whether self-identification is actually accurate. A second limitation concerns the fact that the diagnostic conditions added in the second regression model had considerable overlap with the formal diagnosis introduced in the first step of the regression analysis. Our multicollinearity scores did not point out significant problems related to multicollinearity, yet this overall remains a boundary condition of our research design. A third limitation concerns the generalizability of our findings across different work contexts and countries. While our data encompasses nineteen organizations across seven different industries, these industries are less focused on low-skilled jobs. Since our data were collected in The Netherlands and Bonaire, this might limit the generalization to other countries, such as those with lower levels of awareness and acceptance of neurodivergent conditions. Finally, as previously noted, our study does not contain extensive demographic data, limiting our ability to control for these variables.
7. Conclusions
Overall, the addition of self-awareness of neurodivergent conditions added significantly to the predictive models for three decision-making styles (intuitive, avoidant, and spontaneous), both cognitive styles and rationality. Therefore, we can state that assessing neurodivergence based on self-awareness (self-identification) is a valid strategy for research purposes related to neurodivergence in organizational settings. Relying on self-identified conditions increases the incidence of neurodivergence reported in the workplace and allows for a more realistic estimation of neurodivergence in various social settings. We do not plea for the absolute superiority of self-identification over the formal diagnosis of neurodivergence, as the clinical competencies, skills, and knowledge of lay persons cannot match those of trained clinicians. However, our results show that in empirical research, self-identification can accurately capture some cognitive differences ascribed to neurodivergence.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, J.v.R. and P.L.C.; methodology, J.v.R. and P.L.C.; software, P.L.C.; validation, J.v.R. and P.L.C.; formal analysis, P.L.C.; investigation, J.v.R.; resources, J.v.R. and P.L.C.; data curation, J.v.R.; writing—original draft preparation, J.v.R. and P.L.C.; writing—review and editing, J.v.R. and P.L.C.; supervision, P.L.C.; project administration, J.v.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
This research received no external funding.
Institutional Review Board Statement
The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethical Review Board of the Open Universiteit (U202209082 11 November 2022).
Informed Consent Statement
Informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in the study.
Data Availability Statement
The database for this research was submitted with the paper and is available from the corresponding author upon motivated request.
Conflicts of Interest
Author Jan van Rijswijk was employed by the company Relatics BV. The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
- Artamoshina, P., Shirokova, G., Osiyevskyy, O., & Bodolica, V. (2023). ADHD symptoms of CEOs and business model innovation in the SME context. Technovation, 128, 102845. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ball, C., Mann, L., & Stamm, C. (1994). Decision-making abilities of intellectually gifted and non-gifted children. Australian Journal of Psychology, 46(1), 13–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blacksmith, N., Yang, Y., Behrend, T. S., & Ruark, G. A. (2019). Assessing the validity of inferences from scores on the cognitive reflection test. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 32(5), 599–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cope, R., & Remington, A. (2022). The strengths and abilities of autistic people in the workplace. Autism Adulthood, 4(1), 22–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Curşeu, P. L., & Schruijer, S. G. L. (2012). Decision styles and rationality: An analysis of the predictive validity of the general decision-making style inventory. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 72(6), 1053–1062. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Doyle, N. (2020). Neurodiversity at work: A biopsychosocial model and the impact on working adults. British Medical Bulletin, 135(1), 108–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., Spencer, T., Wilens, T., Seidman, L. J., Mick, E., & Doyle, A. E. (2000). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults: An overview. Biological Psychiatry, 48(1), 9–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fellowes, S. (2024). Establishing the accuracy of self-diagnosis in psychiatry. Philosophical Psychology, 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19(4), 25–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guerra-Carrillo, B., Katovich, K., & Bunge, S. A. (2017). Does higher education hone cognitive functioning and learning efficacy? Findings from a large and diverse sample. PLoS ONE, 12(8), e0182276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hartung, C. M., & Lefler, E. K. (2019). Sex and gender in psychopathology: DSM–5 and beyond. Psychological Bulletin, 145(4), 390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hoppe, E. I., & Kusterer, D. J. (2011). Behavioral biases and cognitive reflection. Economics Letters, 110(2), 97–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnsen, S. K. (2021). Definitions, models, and characteristics of gifted students. In Identifying gifted students: A practical guide (3rd ed., pp. 1–32). Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Kahan, D. M. (2013). Ideology, motivated reasoning, and cognitive reflection. Judgment and Decision Making, 8(4), 407–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Knouse, L. E., & Safren, S. A. (2010). Current status of cognitive behavioral therapy for adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. The Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 33(3), 497–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krzeminska, A., Austin, R. D., Bruyère, S. M., & Hedley, D. (2019). The advantages and challenges of neurodiversity employment in organizations. Journal of Management & Organization, 25(4), 453–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Landerl, K., Fussenegger, B., Moll, K., & Willburger, E. (2009). Dyslexia and dyscalculia: Two learning disorders with different cognitive profiles. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(3), 309–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lewis, L. F. (2016). Realizing a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder as an adult. International Journal of Mental Health Nursing, 25(4), 346–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mäntylä, T., Still, J., Gullberg, S., & Del Missier, F. (2012). Decision making in adults with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 16(2), 164–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63(2), 81–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Overton, G. L., Marsà-Sambola, F., Martin, R., & Cavenagh, P. (2023). Understanding the self-identification of autism in adults: A scoping review. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11(4), 682–702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pryke-Hobbes, A., Davies, J., Heasman, B., Livesey, A., Walker, A., Pellicano, E., & Remington, A. (2023). The workplace masking experiences of autistic, non-autistic neurodivergent and neurotypical adults in the UK. PLoS ONE, 18(9), e0290001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Remington, A. M., Swettenham, J. G., & Lavie, N. (2012). Lightening the load: Perceptual load impairs visual detection in typical adults but not in autism. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(2), 544–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Richardson, A. (1977). Verbalizer-visualizer: A cognitive style dimension. Journal of Mental Imagery, 1, 109–129. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts, W., Milich, R., & Fillmore, M. T. (2012). Constraints on information processing capacity in adults with ADHD. Neuropsychology, 26(6), 695–703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scott, S. G., & Bruce, R. A. (1995). Decision-making style: The development and assessment of a new measure. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 55(5), 818–831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spicer, D. P., & Sadler-Smith, E. (2005). An examination of the general decision making style questionnaire in two UK samples. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 20(2), 137–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van Rijswijk, J., Curseu, P. L., & van Oortmerssen, L. A. (2024). Cognitive and neurodiversity in groups: A systemic and integrative review. Small Group Research, 55(1), 44–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van Rijswijk, J., Curseu, P. L., & van Oortmerssen, L. A. (2025). Unlocking neurodiversity: The role of horizontal differentiation and social cognitive integration as antecedents of team performance. Team Performance Management: An International Journal. (ahead-of-print). [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weber, C., Krieger, B., Häne, E., Yarker, J., & McDowall, A. (2022). Physical workplace adjustments to support neurodivergent workers: A systematic review. Applied Psychology. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1.
Means, standard deviations, and correlations.
Table 1.
Means, standard deviations, and correlations.
| Mean | SD | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | 12 | 13 | 14 | 15 | 16 | 17 | 18 | 19 | 20 |
---|
1. Education | 3.356 | 1.511 | 1 | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
2. FD—High Giftedness | 0.02 | 0.139 | 0.074 | 1 | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
3. FD—Autism | 0.01 | 0.118 | 0.051 | −0.017 | 1 | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
4. FD—ADHD | 0.04 | 0.207 | 0.101 | 0.067 | 0.089 | 1 | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
5. FD—ADD | 0.02 | 0.148 | 0.040 | −0.021 | 0.143 ** | 0.150 ** | 1 | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
6. FD—Dyslexia | 0.04 | 0.194 | −0.038 | −0.029 | 0.099 | 0.096 | −0.031 | 1 | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
7. SI—High Giftedness | 0.07 | 0.260 | 0.205 ** | 0.349 ** | −0.033 | 0.148 ** | 0.030 | −0.057 | 1 | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
8. SI—Autism | 0.07 | 0.260 | 0.034 | 0.038 | 0.334 ** | 0.096 | 0.030 | −0.001 | 0.170 ** | 1 | | | | | | | | | | | | |
9. SI—ADHD | 0.07 | 0.251 | 0.092 | 0.123 * | −0.032 | 0.428 ** | 0.111 * | 0.061 | 0.226 ** | 0.269 ** | 1 | | | | | | | | | | | |
10. SI—ADD | 0.08 | 0.274 | −0.023 | 0.032 | −0.035 | −0.015 | 0.232 ** | −0.007 | 0.074 | 0.193 ** | 0.330 ** | 1 | | | | | | | | | | |
11. SI—DCD. | 0.00 | 0.053 | 0.058 | −0.007 | −0.006 | 0.245 ** | 0.350 ** | −0.011 | 0.189 ** | −0.015 | 0.197 ** | 0.178 ** | 1 | | | | | | | | | |
12. SI—Dyslexia | 0.08 | 0.274 | −0.077 | 0.032 | 0.052 | 0.134 * | −0.045 | 0.574 ** | −0.004 | 0.035 | 0.043 | −0.013 | −0.016 | 1 | | | | | | | | |
13. SI—Dyscalculia | 0.02 | 0.129 | 0.070 | 0.139 ** | −0.016 | 0.077 | −0.020 | 0.198 ** | 0.047 | 0.047 | 0.052 | 0.041 | −0.007 | 0.121 * | 1 | | | | | | | |
14. DM—Rational | 0.596 | 3.834 | −0.019 | −0.011 | −0.037 | −0.082 | −0.276 ** | −0.010 | 0.069 | 0.046 | −0.047 | −0.102 | −0.186 ** | −0.042 | 0.018 | 1 | | | | | | |
15. DM—Intuitive | 0.671 | 3.431 | −0.101 | −0.007 | −0.148 ** | 0.095 | 0.032 | −0.065 | −0.058 | −0.193 ** | 0.098 | 0.099 | 0.045 | 0.029 | −0.032 | −0.270 ** | 1 | | | | | |
16. DM—Dependent | 0.765 | 3.377 | 0.115 * | 0.036 | −0.040 | 0.081 | 0.034 | 0.014 | −0.054 | −0.057 | 0.090 | 0.047 | 0.002 | 0.033 | 0.067 | 0.006 | −0.062 | 1 | | | | |
17. DM—Avoidant | 0.953 | 2.441 | 0.134 * | 0.126 * | −0.055 | 0.139 ** | 0.192 ** | 0.022 | 0.131 * | 0.133 * | 0.217 ** | 0.295 ** | 0.109 * | 0.048 | 0.109 * | −0.101 | −0.042 | 0.410 ** | 1 | | | |
18. DM—Spontaneous | 0.647 | 2.462 | 0.107 * | 0.080 | −0.004 | 0.239 ** | 0.150 ** | −0.077 | 0.040 | 0.083 | 0.276 ** | 0.188 ** | 0.159 ** | 0.003 | −0.012 | −0.339 ** | 0.262 ** | 0.072 | 0.130 * | 1 | | |
19. CS—Verbal | 1.770 | 4.087 | 0.180 ** | 0.050 | 0.008 | 0.051 | −0.050 | −0.092 | 0.133 * | 0.041 | 0.170 ** | 0.026 | 0.087 | −0.212 ** | −0.031 | 0.087 | 0.038 | −0.081 | −0.093 | −0.051 | 1 | |
20. CS—Visual | 2.468 | 8.177 | −0.008 | −0.027 | 0.079 | 0.061 | 0.097 | 0.155 ** | −0.020 | 0.076 | 0.031 | 0.083 | −0.047 | 0.228 ** | 0.097 | −0.099 | 0.084 | 0.052 | 0.190 ** | 0.154 ** | −0.669 ** | 1 |
21. CRT | 1.104 | 1.894 | 0.196 ** | 0.124 * | −0.010 | −0.028 | −0.071 | 0.059 | 0.183 ** | 0.027 | 0.036 | 0.103 | 0.005 | 0.029 | −0.066 | 0.104 * | −0.138 ** | −0.103 | −0.026 | −0.019 | 0.061 | −0.019 |
Table 2.
Results of the regression analyses for decision-making styles.
Table 2.
Results of the regression analyses for decision-making styles.
| Rational | Intuitive | Dependent | Avoidant | Spontaneous |
---|
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 1 | Model2 | Model 1 | Model 2 |
---|
Constant | 3.87 *** (0.08) | 3.89 *** (0.08) | 3.59 *** (0.09) | 3.57 *** (0.09) | 3.18 *** (0.10) | 3.15 *** (0.10) | 2.14 *** (0.12) | 2.04 *** (0.12) | 2.32 *** (0.08) | 2.27 *** (0.08) |
Formal diagnosis (step 1) | | | | | | | | | | |
Education | −0.002 (0.02) | −0.01 (0.02) | −0.05 * (0.02) | −0.04 † (0.02) | 0.06 * (0.03) | 0.06 * (0.03) | 0.07 * (0.03) | 0.07 * (0.03) | 0.03 (0.02) | 0.04 † (0.02) |
High giftedness | −0.06 (0.22) | −0.24 (0.24) | −0.05 (0.25) | −0.004 (0.27) | 0.13 (0.29) | 0.24 (0.32) | 0.78 * (0.35) | 0.55 (0.37) | 0.28 (0.24) | 0.34 (0.25) |
Autism | 0.04 (0.27) | −0.09 (0.29) | −0.87 ** (0.30) | −0.38 (0.32) | −0.37 (0.35) | −0.18 (0.38) | −0.80 † (0.42) | −0.87 (0.44) | −0.19 (0.29) | −0.12 (0.31) |
ADHD | −0.11 (0.15) | −0.09 (0.17) | 0.39 * (0.17) | 0.32 † (0.20) | 0.25 (0.20) | 0.20 (0.23) | 0.44 † (0.24) | 0.37 (0.27) | 0.69 *** (0.16) | 0.49 ** (0.18) |
ADD | −1.09 *** (0.21) | −0.91 *** (0.23) | 0.17 (0.24) | 0.02 (0.26) | 0.15 (0.28) | 0.14 (0.30) | 1.23 *** (0.34) | 0.91 * (0.35) | 0.51 * (0.23) | 0.27 (0.24) |
Dyslexia | −0.05 (0.16) | 0.08 (0.20) | −0.23 (0.18) | −0.45 * (0.22) | 0.07 (0.21) | −0.12 (0.26) | 0.17 (0.25) | −0.02 (0.30) | −0.29 † (0.17) | −0.42 * (0.21) |
Self-identification (step 2) | | | | | | | | | | |
High giftedness | | 0.28 * (0.13) | | −0.14 (0.15) | | −0.33 † (0.18) | | 0.07 (0.21) | | −0.23 (0.14) |
Autism | | 0.13 (0.13) | | −0.57 *** (0.15) | | −0.22 (0.18) | | 0.32 (0.21) | | 0.05 (0.14) |
ADHD | | −0.01 (0.15) | | 0.28 (0.18) | | 0.25 (0.20) | | 0.14 (0.23) | | 0.40 * (0.16) |
ADD | | −0.11 (0.13) | | 0.27 † (0.14) | | 0.12 (0.17) | | 0.80 *** (0.19) | | 0.28 * (0.13) |
DCD | | −1.26 † (0.65) | | −0.12 (0.73) | | −0.44 (0.86) | | −0.29 (0.99) | | 0.73 (0.67) |
Dyslexia | | −0.15 (0.14) | | 0.23 (0.16) | | 0.13 (0.18) | | 0.17 (0.21) | | 0.15 (0.15) |
Dyscalculia | | 0.10 (0.25) | | −0.09 (0.28) | | 0.30 (0.33) | | 0.47 (0.38) | | −0.16 (0.26) |
N | | | | | | | | | | |
R2 | 0.08 | 0.11 | 0.05 | 0.11 | 0.02 | 0.05 | 0.09 | 0.17 | 0.09 | 0.15 |
F change | 4.95 *** | 1.54 | 3.11 ** | 3.09 ** | 1.34 | 1.23 | 5.49 *** | 4.77 *** | 5.78 *** | 3.14 ** |
Table 3.
Results of the regression analyses for rationality and cognitive styles.
Table 3.
Results of the regression analyses for rationality and cognitive styles.
| CRT | Visual Cognitive Style | Verbal Cognitive Style |
---|
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 1 | Model 2 |
---|
Constant | 1.40 *** (0.14) | 1.36 *** (0.14) | 8.10 *** (0.32) | 7.85 *** (0.32) | 3.44 *** (0.23) | 3.59 *** (0.23) |
Formal diagnosis (step 1) | | | | | | |
Education | 0.15 *** (0.04) | 0.14 *** (0.04) | −0.02 (0.09) | 0.01 (0.09) | 0.20 *** (0.06) | 0.16 * (0.06) |
High giftedness | 0.90 * (0.41) | 0.66 (0.44) | −0.37 (0.94) | −0.76 (1.00) | 0.39 (0.67) | 0.16 (0.70) |
Autism | −0.11 (0.49) | −0.01 (0.53) | 1.01 (1.12) | 0.72 (1.20) | 0.21 (0.80) | 0.56 (0.85) |
ADHD | −0.28 (0.28) | −0.21 (0.32) | 0.38 (0.64) | 0.49 (0.72) | 0.42 (0.46) | −0.17 (0.51) |
ADD | −0.48 (0.39) | −0.75 † (0.42) | 1.49 † (0.89) | 1.91 * (0.95) | −0.82 (0.64) | −1.32 † (0.67) |
Dyslexia | 0.42 (0.20) | 0.59 (0.36) | 1.90 ** (0.67) | 0.23 (0.82) | −0.84 † (0.48) | 0.39 (0.58) |
Self-identification (step 2) | | | | | | |
High giftedness | | 0.58 * (0.25) | | −0.04 (0.56) | | 0.42 (0.39) |
Autism | | −0.04 (0.25) | | 0.36 (0.56) | | −0.08 (0.40) |
ADHD | | −0.16 (0.28) | | −0.22 (0.63) | | 1.18 ** (0.45) |
ADD | | 0.56 (0.23) | | 0.70 (0.53) | | −0.11 (0.37) |
DCD | | −0.07 (1.20) | | −4.74 † (2.70) | | 2.62 (1.91) |
Dyslexia | | 0.004 (0.26) | | 1.89 ** (0.56) | | −1.50 *** (0.41) |
Dyscalculia | | −1.03 * (0.46) | | 1.32 (1.03) | | −0.45 (0.72) |
N | | | | | | |
R2 | 0.06 | 0.11 | 0.04 | 0.09 | 0.05 | 0.12 |
F change | 3.96 *** | 2.41 * | 2.32 * | 2.67 * | 2.86 * | 4.01 *** |
| Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).