The Roles of Rule Type and Word Term in the Deductive Reasoning of Adults with and without Dyslexia
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Reasoning
1.2. Dyslexia and Reasoning
1.3. Investigating Deductive Reasoning in Dyslexia
2. Method
2.1. Participants
2.2. Materials
2.3. Design
2.4. Procedure
3. Results
3.1. Rule Type: Accuracy
3.2. Rule Type: Completion Time
3.3. Word Type: Accuracy
3.4. Word Type: Completion Time
4. Discussion
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Lyon, G.; Shaywitz, S.; Shaywitz, B. A definition of dyslexia. Ann. Dyslexia 2003, 53, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vellutino, F.R.; Fletcher, J.M.; Snowling, M.J.; Scanlon, D.M. Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2004, 45, 2–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Smith-Spark, J.H.; Fisk, J.E. Working memory functioning in developmental dyslexia. Memory 2007, 15, 34–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Provazza, S.; Adams, A.-M.; Giofrè, D.; Roberts, D.J. Double trouble—Visual and phonological impairments in English dyslexic readers. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brosnan, M.; Demetre, J.; Hamill, S.; Robson, K.; Shepherd, H.; Cody, G. Executive functioning in adults and children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 2002, 40, 2144–2155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Smith-Spark, J.H.; Henry, L.A.; Messer, D.J.; Edvardsdottir, E.; Zięcik, A.P. Executive functions in adults with developmental dyslexia. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2016, 53–54, 323–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith-Spark, J.H.; Zięcik, A.P.; Sterling, C. Adults with developmental dyslexia show selective impairments in time-based and self-initiated prospective memory: Self-report and clinical evidence. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2017, 62, 247–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bacon, A.M.; Handley, S.J.; McDonald, E.L. Reasoning and dyslexia: A spatial strategy may impede reasoning with visually rich information. Br. J. Psychol. 2007, 98, 79–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bacon, A.M.; Handley, S.J. Dyslexia and reasoning: The importance of visual processes. Br. J. Psychol. 2010, 101, 433–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jacobs, J.B.; Newton, E.J.; Smith-Spark, J.H. Dyslexia and syllogistic reasoning in adults: Differences in strategy usage. Dyslexia 2021, 27, 153–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wason, P.C. Reasoning. In New Horizons in Psychology; Foss, B.M., Ed.; Penguin: Harmondsworth, UK, 1966; Volume 1, pp. 135–151. ISBN 978-014-020-775-0. [Google Scholar]
- Evans, J.S.B.T.; Over, D.E.; Manktelow, K.I. Reasoning, decision making and rationality. Cognition 1993, 49, 165–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baron, J. Thinking and Deciding, 4th ed.; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2008; ISBN 978-052-186-207-3. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson-Laird, P.N. Deductive reasoning. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1999, 50, 109–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stanovich, K.E.; West, R.F. Individual differences in rational thought. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 1998, 127, 161–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson-Laird, P.N. How We Reason; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2006; ISBN 978-019-856-976-3. [Google Scholar]
- Goel, V.; Dolan, R.J. Differential involvement of left prefrontal cortex in inductive and deductive reasoning. Cognition 2004, 93, B109–B121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goel, V.; Gold, B.; Kapur, S.; Houle, S. The seats of reason? An imaging study of deductive and inductive reasoning. NeuroReport 1997, 8, 1305–1310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bem, S.; de Jong, H.L. Theoretical Issues in Psychology: An Introduction, 2nd ed.; Sage Publications: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2006; ISBN 978-076-194-201-6. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson-Laird, P.N. Deductive reasoning. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. 2010, 1, 8–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Panagiotidou, E.; Serrano, F.; Moreno-Ríos, S. Testing the visual impedance effect in children with and without reading difficulties using a new visual reasoning task. Dyslexia 2020, 26, 67–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cappagli, G.; Carzola, B.; Potente, C.; Gori, M. Proportional reasoning deficit in dyslexia. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zwaan, R.A.; Stanfield, R.A.; Yaxley, R.H. Do language comprehenders routinely represent the shapes of objects? Psychol. Sci. 2002, 13, 168–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Knauff, M.; Johnson-Laird, P.N. Visual imagery can impede reasoning. Mem. Cogn. 2002, 30, 363–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Knauff, M.; May, E. Mental imagery, reasoning, and blindness. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2006, 59, 161–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bacon, A.M.; Handley, S.J.; Newstead, S.E. Verbal and spatial strategies in reasoning. In Methods of Thought: Individual Differences in Reasoning Strategies; Roberts, M., Newton, E., Eds.; Psychology Press: Hove, UK, 2005; pp. 81–105. ISBN 978-041-565-555-2. [Google Scholar]
- Bacon, A.M.; Handley, S.J. Reasoning and dyslexia: Is visual memory a compensatory resource? Dyslexia 2014, 20, 330–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Inhelder, B.; Piaget, J. The Growth of Logical Thinking from Childhood to Adolescence; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1958; ISBN 978-131-500-967-4. [Google Scholar]
- Markovits, H.; Barrouillet, P. Introduction: Why is understanding the development of reasoning important? Think. Reason. 2004, 10, 113–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Evans, J.S.B.T. A brief history of the Wason selection task. In The Thinking Mind: A Festschrift for Ken Manktelow; Galbraith, N., Lucas, E., Over, D., Eds.; Psychology Press: Abingdon, UK, 2016; pp. 1–14. ISBN 978-1-138-93786-4. [Google Scholar]
- Stenning, K.; van Lambalgen, M. The natural history of hypotheses about the selection task: Towards a philosophy of science for investigating human reasoning. In Psychology of Reasoning: Theoretical and Historical Perspectives; Manktelow, K., Chung, M.C., Eds.; Psychology Press: London, UK, 2004; pp. 127–156. ISBN 978-184-169-310-1. [Google Scholar]
- Ragni, M.; Kola, I.; Johnson-Laird, P.N. On selecting evidence to test hypotheses: A theory of selection tasks. Psychol. Bull. 2018, 144, 779–796. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kellen, D.; Klauer, K.C. Theories of the Wason Selection Task: A critical assessment of boundaries and benchmarks. Comput. Brain Behav. 2019, 3, 341–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wason, P.C.; Shapiro, D. Natural and contrived experience in a reasoning problem. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1971, 23, 63–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Evans, J.S.B.T. Matching bias in conditional reasoning: Do we understand it after 25 years? Think. Reason. 1998, 4, 45–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Higgins, E.T. Effects of presupposition on deductive reasoning. J. Verb. Learning Verb. Behav. 1976, 15, 419–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lei, Y.; Li, F.; Long, C.; Li, P.; Chen, Q.; Ni, Y.; Li, H. How does typicality of category members affect the deductive reasoning? An ERP study. Exp. Brain Res. 2010, 204, 47–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brysbaert, M.; Mandera, P.; Keuleers, E. The word frequency effect in word processing: An updated review. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2018, 27, 45–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brysbaert, M.; Buchmeier, M.; Conrad, M.; Jacobs, A.M.; Bölte, J.; Böhl, A. The word frequency effect: A review of recent developments and implications for the choice of frequency estimates in German. Exp. Psychol. 2011, 58, 412–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Monaghan, P.; Chang, Y.-N.; Welbourne, S.; Brysbaert, M. Exploring the relations between word frequency, language exposure, and bilingualism in a computational model of reading. J. Mem. Lang. 2017, 93, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoch, S.J.; Tschirgi, J.E. Logical knowledge and cue redundancy in deductive reasoning. Mem. Cogn. 1985, 13, 453–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Evans, J.S.B.T.; Over, D.E. Rationality and Reasoning; Psychology Press: Hove, UK, 1996; ISBN 978-086-377-438-6. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts, M.J.; Newton, E.J. Inspection times, the change task, and the rapid-response selection task. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 2001, 54, 1031–1048. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roberts, M.J.; Newton, E.J. Rapid-response versus free-time selection tasks using different logical connectives. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2011, 23, 858–872. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stanovich, K.E.; West, R.F. Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behav. Brain Sci. 2000, 23, 645–665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Osman, M. An evaluation of dual-process theories of reasoning. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2004, 11, 988–1010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Da Silva, S. System 1 vs. System 2 Thinking. Psych 2023, 5, 1057–1076. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Evans, J.S.B.T. In two minds: Dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2003, 7, 454–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paul, I.; Bott, C.; Wienbruch, C.; Elbert, T.R. Word Processing differences between dyslexic and control children. BMC Psychiatry 2006, 6, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Provazza, S.; Giofrè, D.; Adams, A.M.; Roberts, D.J. The clock counts—Length effects in English dyslexic readers. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paivio, A.; Yuille, J.C.; Madigan, S.A. Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. J. Exp. Psychol. 1968, 76, 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rofes, A.; Zakariás, L.; Ceder, K.; Lind, M.; Johansson, M.B.; de Aguiar, V.; Bjekić, J.; Fyndanis, V.; Gavarró, A.; Simonsen, H.G.; et al. Imageability ratings across languages. Behav. Res. Methods 2018, 50, 1187–1197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Strain, E.; Patterson, K.; Seidenberg, M.S. Semantic effects in single-word naming. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1995, 21, 1140–1154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Steacy, L.M.; Compton, D.L. Examining the role of imageability and regularity in word reading accuracy and learning efficiency among first and second graders at risk for reading disabilities. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2019, 178, 226–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brysbaert, M.; Warriner, A.B.; Kuperman, V. Concreteness ratings for 40 thousand generally known English word lemmas. Behav. Res. Methods 2014, 46, 904–911. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Paivio, A. Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. Can. J. Psychol. 1991, 45, 255–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Panagiotidou, E.; Serrano, F.; Moreno-Rios, S. Reasoning and reading in adults. A new reasoning task for detecting the visual impendance effect. Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 2018, 14, 150–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gebauer, G.; Laming, D. Rational choices in Wason’s selection task. Psychol. Res. 1997, 60, 284–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kessler, R.C.; Adler, L.; Ames, M.; Demler, O.; Faraone, S.; Hiripi, E.; Howes, M.J.; Jin, R.; Secnik, K.; Spencer, T.; et al. The World Health Organization Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS): A short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychol. Med. 2005, 35, 245–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Adler, L.A.; Spencer, T.; Faraone, S.V.; Kessler, R.C.; Howes, M.J.; Biederman, J.; Secnik, K. Validity of pilot Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS) to rate adult ADHD symptoms. Ann. Clin. Psychiatry 2006, 18, 145–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wechsler, D. The Wechsler Objective Reading Dimensions; The Psychological Corporation: London, UK, 1993; ISBN 978-074-910-293-7. [Google Scholar]
- Fawcett, A.J.; Nicolson, R.I. The Dyslexia Adult Screening Test (DAST); The Psychological Corporation: London, UK, 1998; ISBN 978-074-911-321-6. [Google Scholar]
- Raven, J.; Raven, J.C.; Court, J.H. Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales; Oxford Psychologists Press: Oxford, UK, 1998; ISBN 978-185-639-027-9. [Google Scholar]
- Bird, H.; Franklin, S.; Howard, D. Age of acquisition and imageability ratings for a large set of words, including verbs and function words. Beh. Res. Meth. Instr. Comp. 2001, 33, 73–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Coltheart, M. The MRC Psycholinguistic Database. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Sect. A 1981, 33, 497–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zeno, S.M.; Ivens, S.H.; Millard, R.T.; Duvvuri, R. The Educator’s Word Frequency Guide; Touchstone Applied Science Associates: Brewster, NY, USA, 1995; ISBN 978-156-497-021-3. [Google Scholar]
- Baayen, R.H.; Piepenbrock, R.; Gulikers, L. The CELEX Lexical Database (CD-ROM); Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Stanovich, K.E.; West, R.F. Individual differences in framing and conjunction effects. Think. Reason. 1998, 4, 289–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stanovich, K.E.; West, R.F. Who uses base rates and P(D/~H)? An analysis of individual differences. Mem. Cogn. 1998, 26, 161–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- King, P.M.; Kitchener, K.S. The Reflective Judgment Model: Twenty years of research on epistemic cognition. In Personal Epistemology: The Psychology of Beliefs about Knowledge and Knowing; Hofer, B.K., Pintrich, P.R., Eds.; Laurence Erlbaum: Mahweh, NJ, USA, 2002; pp. 40–62. ISBN 978-020-342-496-4. [Google Scholar]
- Vukman, K.B. Developmental differences in metacognition and their connections with cognitive development in adulthood. J. Adult Dev. 2005, 12, 211–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hsu, J.C. Multiple Comparisons: Theory and Methods; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2023; ISBN 978-103-247-802-9. [Google Scholar]
- Bacon, A.M.; Handley, S.J.; Newstead, S.E. Individual differences in strategies for syllogistic reasoning. Think. Reason. 2003, 9, 133–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Word Term Characteristic and Database Source | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Imageability | Word Frequency | |||
Word List | Bird et al. [64] Imageability | MRC Psycholinguistic Database Imageability [65] | CELEX Written Frequency [67] | Standard Frequency Index [66] |
High imageability and average word frequency | 582.66 (42.94) | 561.20 (33.02) | 1.30 (0.54) | 52.23 (7.10) |
Low imageability and average word frequency | 162.94 (148.90) | 323.24 (28.61) | 1.41 (0.77) | 53.99 (8.44) |
Average imageability and high word frequency | 397.44 (103.06) | 422.31 (60.52) | 2.72 (0.37) | 66.12 (4.40) |
Average imageability and low word frequency | 420.30 (82.13) | 402.50 (48.79) | −0.46 (0.73) | 28.13 (5.06) |
Rule Type | Mean Accuracy (SD) |
---|---|
If p, then q | 0.74 (1.28) |
If not p, then q | 1.23 (1.15) |
If p, then not q | 2.84 (1.86) |
If not p, then not q | 1.37 (1.37) |
Rule Type | Mean Completion Time (s) |
---|---|
If p then q | 55.95 (18.48) |
If not p then q | 58.88 (22.16) |
If p then not q | 51.53 (20.48) |
If not p then not q | 58.67 (19.88) |
Word Type | Mean Accuracy (SD) |
---|---|
High imageability–average frequency | 1.98 (1.29) |
Low imageability–average frequency | 1.33 (1.31) |
High frequency–average imageability | 1.32 (1.12) |
Low frequency–average imageability | 1.54 (1.28) |
Word Type | Mean Completion Time (s) |
---|---|
High imageability–average frequency | 55.00 (20.79) |
Low imageability–average frequency | 55.17 (21.97) |
Average imageability–high frequency | 61.07 (21.09) |
Average imageability–low frequency | 53.77 (19.75) |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Jacobs, J.B.; Smith-Spark, J.H.; Newton, E.J. The Roles of Rule Type and Word Term in the Deductive Reasoning of Adults with and without Dyslexia. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 635. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14080635
Jacobs JB, Smith-Spark JH, Newton EJ. The Roles of Rule Type and Word Term in the Deductive Reasoning of Adults with and without Dyslexia. Behavioral Sciences. 2024; 14(8):635. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14080635
Chicago/Turabian StyleJacobs, Janette B., James H. Smith-Spark, and Elizabeth J. Newton. 2024. "The Roles of Rule Type and Word Term in the Deductive Reasoning of Adults with and without Dyslexia" Behavioral Sciences 14, no. 8: 635. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14080635
APA StyleJacobs, J. B., Smith-Spark, J. H., & Newton, E. J. (2024). The Roles of Rule Type and Word Term in the Deductive Reasoning of Adults with and without Dyslexia. Behavioral Sciences, 14(8), 635. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14080635