Emotional Regulation of Displaced Aggression in Provocative Situations among Junior High School Students
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Experiment 1: The Effect of Displaced Aggression on Emotions after Provocation among Junior High School Students
2.1. Methods
2.1.1. Participants
2.1.2. Design
2.1.3. Materials
2.1.4. Measures
2.1.5. Procedure
2.2. Results
2.2.1. Manipulation Check
2.2.2. Effect of Displaced Aggression on Positive Emotion
2.2.3. Effect of Displaced Aggression on Negative Emotion
2.3. Discussion
3. Experiment 2. The Effect of the Intensity of Displaced Aggression on Emotions after Experiencing High Levels of Provocation among Junior High School Students
3.1. Methods
3.1.1. Participants
3.1.2. Design
3.1.3. Materials and Procedure
3.2. Results
3.2.1. Manipulation Check about Forced Choice
3.2.2. Effect of Displaced Aggressive Intensity on Positive Emotion
3.2.3. Effect of Displaced Aggressive Intensity on Negative Emotion
3.2.4. Mini Meta-Analysis
3.3. Discussion
4. General Discussion
4.1. Emotion-Regulating Effects of Displaced Aggression in Provocative Situations
4.2. Emotional Regulation Effects of Different Intensities of Displaced Aggression after High-Level Provocation
5. Limitations and Further Direction
6. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Anderson, C.A.; Bushman, B.J. Human Aggression. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2002, 53, 27–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stanford, M.S.; Houston, R.J.; Mathias, C.W.; Villemarette-Pittman, N.R.; Helfritz, L.E.; Conklin, S.M. Characterizing aggressive behavior. Assessment 2003, 10, 183–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Denson, T.F.; Pedersen, W.C.; Miller, N. The displaced aggression questionnaire. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2006, 90, 1032–1051. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kim, E.; Shin, Y.; Kim, J. Conflicts between work and family and displaced aggression with working parents in South Korea: An exploratory study. J. Comp. Fam. Stud. 2019, 50, 331–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reijntjes, A.; Thomaes, S.; Kamphuis, J.H.; Bushman, B.J.; Reitz, E.; Telch, M.J. Youths’ displaced aggression against in- and out-group peers: An experimental examination. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 2013, 115, 180–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liu, Y.; Wang, M.; Chang, C.H.; Shi, J.; Zhou, L.; Shao, R. Work-family conflict, emotional exhaustion, and displaced aggression toward others: The moderating roles of workplace interpersonal conflict and perceived managerial family support. J. Appl. Psychol. 2015, 100, 793–808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garcia-Sancho, E.; Martin, S.J.; Vasquez, E.A.; Fernandez-Berrocal, P. Validity and reliability of the spanish version of the displaced aggression questionnaire. Psicothema 2016, 28, 96–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Willoughby, T.; Good, M.; Adachi, P.J.; Hamza, C.; Tavernier, R. Examining the link between adolescent brain development and risk taking from a social-developmental perspective. Brain Cogn. 2014, 89, 70–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Barker, E.D.; Vitaro, F.; Lacourse, E.; Fontaine, N.M.; Carbonneau, R.; Tremblay, R.E. Testing the developmental distinctiveness of male proactive and reactive aggression with a nested longitudinal experimental intervention. Aggress. Behav. 2010, 36, 127–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Runions, K.C.; Salmivalli, C.; Shaw, T.; Burns, S.; Cross, D. Beyond the reactive-proactive dichotomy: Rage, revenge, reward, and recreational aggression predict early high school bully and bully/victim status. Aggress. Behav. 2018, 44, 501–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bushman, B.J.; Bonacci, A.M.; Pedersen, W.C.; Vasquez, E.A.; Miller, N. Chewing on It Can Chew You Up: Effects of Rumination on Triggered Displaced Aggression. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2005, 88, 969–983. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carre, J.M.; Gilchrist, J.D.; Morrissey, M.D.; McCormick, C.M. Motivational and situational factors and the relationship between testosterone dynamics and human aggression during competition. Biol. Psychol. 2010, 84, 346–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Denzler, M.; Förster, J. A goal model of catharsis. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 2012, 23, 107–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chester, D.S. The role of positive affect in aggression. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2017, 26, 366–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chester, D.S.; DeWall, C.N.; Enjaian, B. Sadism and aggressive behavior: Inflicting pain to feel pleasure. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2019, 45, 1252–1268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gollwitzer, M.; Meder, M.; Schmitt, M. What gives victims satisfaction when they seek revenge? Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 41, 364–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chester, D.S.; DeWall, C.N. The pleasure of revenge: Retaliatory aggression arises from a neural imbalance toward reward. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neur. 2016, 11, 1173–1182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fischer, P.; Greitemeyer, T. Music and aggression: The impact of sexual-aggressive song lyrics on aggression-related thoughts, emotions, and behavior toward the same and the opposite sex. Pers. Soc. Psychol. B 2006, 32, 1165–1176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McGlade, E.; Rogowska, J.; Yurgelun-Todd, D. Sex differences in orbitofrontal connectivity in male and female veterans with TBI. Brain Imaging. Behav. 2015, 9, 535–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liu, Y.; Zhou, B.; Yang, B. How does emotion shape aggressive behavior of violent offenders? An explanation based on emotion regulation theory. Acta. Psychol. Sin. 2022, 54, 270–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parlamis, J.D. Venting as emotion regulation: The influence of venting responses and respondent identity on anger and emotional tone. Int. J. Confl. Manag. 2012, 23, 77–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhan, J.; Xiu, H.; Ren, J.; Luo, J. Is catharsis beneficial or harmful? The psychological intervention effect and potential harm of catharsis. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 2020, 28, 22–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reijntjes, A.; Kamphuis, J.H.; Thomaes, S.; Bushman, B.J.; Telch, M.J. Too calloused to care: An experimental examination of factors influencing youths’ displaced aggression against their peers. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2013, 142, 28–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pajevic, M.; Vukosavljevic-Gvozden, T.; Stevanovic, N.; Neumann, C.S. The relationship between the Dark Tetrad and a two–dimensional view of empathy. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 2018, 123, 125–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- March, E.; Steele, G. High Esteem and hurting others online: Trait sadism moderates the relationship between self-esteem and internet trolling. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2020, 23, 441–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stadler, C.; Rohrmann, S.; Steuber, S.; Poustka, F. Effects of provocation on emotions and aggression: An experimental study with aggressive children. Swiss. J. Psychol. 2006, 65, 117–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chester, D.S.; DeWall, C.N. Combating the sting of rejection with the pleasure of revenge: A new look at how emotion shapes aggression. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2017, 112, 413–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vasquez, E.A.; Denson, T.F.; Pedersen, W.C.; Stenstrom, D.M.; Miller, N. The moderating effect of trigger intensity on triggered displaced aggression. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2005, 41, 61–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bernstein, A.M.; Visconti, K.J.; Csorba, J.; Radvanyi, K.; Rojahn, J. The relationship between challenging behaviours, mood and interest/pleasure in adults with severe and profound intellectual disabilities. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2015, 59, 1033–1041. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baumeister, R.F.; Campbell, W.K. The intrinsic appeal of evil: Sadism, sensational thrills, and threatened egotism. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 1999, 3, 210–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, Y.; Yang, Q.; Zhu, B.; Ye, S.; Tian, X.; Krueger, F. High levels of psychopathic traits increase the risk of transferring reactive aggression to innocent people after provocation: Evidence from an ERP study. Biol. Psychol. 2020, 153, 107891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beyens, U.; Yu, H.; Han, T.; Zhang, L.; Zhou, X. The strength of a remorseful heart: Psychological and neural basis of how apology emolliates reactive aggression and promotes forgiveness. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Song, P.; Zhang, Z.; Wang, B.; David, N.; Zhao, H.; Wang, Q.; Xiao, Y.; Yang, B. The influence of trait empathy on reactive aggression: An ERP study. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2018, 133, 102–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Warburton, W.A.; Bushman, B.J. The competitive reaction time task: The development and scientific utility of a flexible laboratory aggression paradigm. Aggress. Behav. 2019, 45, 389–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Quan, F. Psychological Pathway and Brain Association of Hostile Attribution Bias in Predicting Aggression. Ph.D. Thesis, Southwest University, Chongqing, China, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Watson, D.; Clark, L.A.; Tellegen, A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1988, 54, 1063–1070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, C.; Li, X.; Xia, L. Long-term effect of cybervictimization on displaced aggressive behavior across two years: Mutually predicting mediators of hostile emotion and moral disengagement. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2023, 141, 107611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eadeh, F.R.; Peak, S.A.; Lambert, A.J. The bittersweet taste of revenge: On the negative and positive consequences of retaliation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2017, 68, 27–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berceanu, A.I.; Matu, S.; Macavei, B.I. Emotional and Cognitive responses to theatrical representations of aggressive behavior. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1785. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rajchert, J.; Konopka, K.; Oreziak, H.; Dziechciarska, W. Direct and displaced aggression after exclusion: Role of gender differences. J. Soc. Psychol. 2023, 163, 126–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goh, J.X.; Hall, J.A.; Rosenthal, R. Mini meta-analysis of your own studies: Some arguments on why and a primer on how. Soc. Pers. Psychol. 2016, 10, 535–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buckels, E.E.; Jones, D.N.; Paulhus, D.L. Behavioral confirmation of everyday sadism. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 2201–2209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Porter, S.; Woodworth, M.; Earle, J.; Drugge, J.; Boer, D. Characteristics of sexual homicides committed by psychopathic and nonpsychopathic offenders. Law. Human. Behav. 2003, 27, 459–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Maheux-Caron, V.; Gamache, D.; Hétu, S. Dark tetrad and vulnerable dark triad traits: Identifying latent profiles from a person-centered approach. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 2024, 219, 112499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Larsen, R.J. Toward a science of mood regulation. Psychol. Inq. 2000, 11, 129–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bushman, B.J.; Baumeister, R.F.; Phillips, C.M. Do people aggress to improve their mood? Catharsis beliefs, affect regulation opportunity, and aggressive responding. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2001, 81, 17–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Threadgill, A.H.; Gable, P.A. Revenge is sweet: Investigation of the effects of Approach-Motivated anger on the RewP in the motivated anger delay (MAD) paradigm. Hum. Brain. Mapp. 2020, 41, 5032–5056. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yang, J.; Li, W.; Wang, W.; Gao, L.; Wang, X. Anger rumination and adolescents’ cyberbullying perpetration: Moral disengagement and callous-unemotional traits as moderators. J. Affect. Disord. 2021, 278, 397–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elbert, T.; Schauer, M.; Moran, J.K. Two pedals drive the bicycle of violence: Reactive and appetitive aggression. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2018, 19, 135–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ramírez, J.M.; Bonniot-Cabanac, M.C.; Cabanac, M. Can aggression provide pleasure. Eur. Psychol. 2005, 10, 136–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martens, A.; Kosloff, S.; Greenberg, J.; Landau, M.J.; Schmader, T. Killing begets killing: Evidence from a bug-killing paradigm that initial killing fuels subsequent killing. Pers. Soc. Psychol. B 2007, 33, 1251–1264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elbert, T.; Moran, J.K.; Schauer, M. Lust for violence: Appetitive aggression as a fundamental part of human nature. Neuroforum 2017, 23, 77–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhao, G.; Song, J.; Ge, Y.; Liu, Y.; Yao, L.; Wen, T. Advances in emotion recognition based on physiological big data. J. Comput. Res. Dev. 2016, 53, 80–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xiao, M.; Zhu, W.; Wei, J.; Lei, X.; Xia, L.X. The relationship among resting-state brain activity and connectivity, agreeableness and displaced aggression: Two possible mediation models. J. Affect. Disord. 2019, 256, 641–649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Drabant, E.M.; McRae, K.; Manuck, S.B.; Hariri, A.R.; Gross, J.J. Individual differences in typical reappraisal use predict amygdala and prefrontal responses. Biol. Psychiatry 2009, 65, 367–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aknin, L.B.; Whillans, A.V. Helping and happiness: A review and guide for public policy. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 2021, 15, 3–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Measurement Phase | |||
---|---|---|---|
Prior to Aggression | Post Aggression | ||
Provocation | Low | 2.96 (1.25) | 2.94 (1.28) |
High | 2.45 (1.22) | 3.12 (1.31) |
Measurement Phase | |||
---|---|---|---|
Prior to Aggression | Post Aggression | ||
Provocation | Low | 1.85 (0.58) | 1.72 (0.89) |
High | 2.32 (1.04) | 1.67 (0.95) |
Measurement Phase | |||
---|---|---|---|
Prior to Aggression | Post Aggression | ||
Intensity | Low | 2.38 (1.24) | 3.00 (1.41) |
High | 2.73 (1.2) | 2.65 (1.37) |
Measurement Phase | |||
---|---|---|---|
Prior to Aggression | Post Aggression | ||
Intensity | Low | 1.81 (0.15) | 1.41 (0.09) |
High | 1.87 (0.15) | 1.36 (0.06) |
Standardized Difference in Means (Cohen’s d) | 95% Confidence Interval | p | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Emotion | Lower limit | Upper limit | ||
Positive Emotion | 0.34 | 0.13 | 0.54 | 0.001 |
Negative Emotion | −0.59 | −0.81 | −0.37 | <0.001 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Lin, S.; Cheng, G.; Sun, S.; Feng, M.; Bai, X. Emotional Regulation of Displaced Aggression in Provocative Situations among Junior High School Students. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 500. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14060500
Lin S, Cheng G, Sun S, Feng M, Bai X. Emotional Regulation of Displaced Aggression in Provocative Situations among Junior High School Students. Behavioral Sciences. 2024; 14(6):500. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14060500
Chicago/Turabian StyleLin, Shuang, Gonglu Cheng, Shinan Sun, Mengmeng Feng, and Xuejun Bai. 2024. "Emotional Regulation of Displaced Aggression in Provocative Situations among Junior High School Students" Behavioral Sciences 14, no. 6: 500. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14060500
APA StyleLin, S., Cheng, G., Sun, S., Feng, M., & Bai, X. (2024). Emotional Regulation of Displaced Aggression in Provocative Situations among Junior High School Students. Behavioral Sciences, 14(6), 500. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14060500