Does the Cognitive Reflection Test Work with Chinese College Students? Evidence from a Time-Limited Study
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Experiment 1
2.1. Method
2.1.1. Participants
2.1.2. Materials
- A bat and a ball cost $1.10 in total. The bat costs $1.00 more than the ball. How much does the ball cost?
- If it takes 5 machines 5 min to make 5 widgets, how long would it take 100 machines to make 100 widgets?
- In a lake, there is a patch of lily pads. Every day, the patch doubles in size. If it takes 48 days for the patch to cover the entire lake, how long would it take for the patch to cover half the lake?
2.1.3. Procedure
2.2. Results
2.3. Discussion
3. Experiment 2
3.1. Method
3.1.1. Participants
3.1.2. Procedure
3.2. Results
3.3. Discussion
4. Experiment 3
4.1. Method
4.1.1. Participants
4.1.2. Materials
A bat and two balls cost $2.60 in total.The bat costs $2 more than two balls.How much does one ball cost?
In an office, there are 150 pens and pencils in total.There are 100 pens.How many pencils are there in the office?
In an office, there are 150 pens and pencils in total.There are 100 pens.How many kinds of stationery are there in the office?
4.1.3. Procedure
Pre-Experiment
Formal Experiment
4.2. Results
4.3. Discussion
5. General Discussion
6. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Frederick, S. Cognitive reflection and decision making. J. Econ. Perspect. 2005, 19, 25–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Toplak, M.E.; West, R.F.; Stanovich, K.E. The Cognitive Reflection Test as a predictor of performance on heuristics-and-biases tasks. Mem. Cogn. 2011, 39, 1275–1289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Evans, J.S.; Stanovich, K.E. Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 8, 223–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Campitelli, G.; Gerrans, P. Does the cognitive reflection test measure cognitive reflection? A mathematical modeling approach. Mem. Cogn. 2014, 42, 434–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pennycook, G.; Ross, R.M. Commentary: Cognitive reflection vs. calculation in decision making. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sirota, M.; Juanchich, M. Role of numeracy and cognitive reflection in Bayesian reasoning with natural frequencies. Stud. Psychol. 2011, 53, 151–161. [Google Scholar]
- De Neys, W. (Ed.) Dual Process Theory 2.0; Routledge: Oxon, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Evans, J.S.B. The heuristic-analytic theory of reasoning: Extension and evaluation. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2006, 13, 378–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Raoelison, M.; Thompson, V.A.; De Neys, W. The smart intuition: Cognitive capacity predicts intuitive rather than deliberate thinking. Cognition 2020, 204, 104381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Campitelli, G.; Labollita, M. Correlations of cognitive reflection with judgments and choices. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2010, 5, 182–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahneman, D. Thinking, Fast and Slow; MacMillan: London, UK, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Liberali, J.M.; Reyna, V.F.; Furlan, S.; Stein, L.M.; Pardo, S.T. Individual differences in numeracy and cognitive reflection, with implications for biases and fallacies in probability judgment. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2012, 25, 361–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weller, J.A.; Dieckmann, N.F.; Tusler, M.; Mertz, C.K.; Burns, W.J.; Peters, E. Development and testing of an abbreviated numeracy scale: A Rasch analysis approach. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2013, 26, 198–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Askew, M.; Hodgen, J.; Hossain, S.; Bretscher, N. Values and Variables: Mathematics Education in High-Performing Countries; Nuffield Foundation: London, UK, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Leung, K.S.F. Mathematics education in East Asia and the West: Does culture matter? In Mathematics Education in Different Cultural Traditions—A Comparative Study of East Asia and the West: The 13th ICMI Study; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 2006; pp. 21–46. [Google Scholar]
- Sturman, L. What is there to learn from international surveys of mathematical achievement. In Oxford Handbook of Numerical Cognition; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2015; pp. 430–444. [Google Scholar]
- PISA (Programme for International Student Assessment). PISA 2018 Results (Volume III): What School Life Means for Students’ Lives; OECD Publishing: Washington, DC, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Geary, D.C.; Salthouse, T.A.; Chen, G.P.; Fan, L. Are East Asian versus American differences in arithmetical ability a recent phenomenon? Dev. Psychol. 1996, 32, 254–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Di Paola, B. Why Asian children outperform students from other countries? Linguistic and parental influences comparing Chinese and Italian children in Preschool Education. Int. Electron. J. Math. Educ. 2016, 11, 3351–3359. [Google Scholar]
- Dowker, A.; Li, A.M. English and Chinese children’s performance on numerical tasks. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 2731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- De Neys, W.; Rossi, S.; Houdé, O. Bats, balls, and substitution sensitivity: Cognitive misers are no happy fools. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2013, 20, 269–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bago, B.; Raoelison, M.; De Neys, W. Second-guess: Testing the specificity of error detection in the bat-and-ball problem. Acta Psychol. 2019, 193, 214–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Boissin, E.; Caparos, S.; Raoelison, M.; De Neys, W. From bias to sound intuiting: Boosting correct intuitive reasoning. Cognition 2021, 211, 104645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brañas-Garza, P.; Kujal, P.; Lenkei, B. Cognitive reflection test: Whom, how, when. J. Behav. Exp. Econ. 2019, 82, 101455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Janssen, E.M.; Raoelison, M.; de Neys, W. ‘You’re wrong!’: The impact of accuracy feedback on the bat-and-ball problem. Acta Psychol. 2020, 206, 103042. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Raoelison, M.; Keime, M.; De Neys, W. Think slow, then fast: Does repeated deliberation boost correct intuitive responding? Mem. Cogn. 2021, 49, 873–883. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frey, D.; Johnson, E.D.; De Neys, W. Individual differences in conflict detection during reasoning. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2018, 71, 1188–1208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Johnson, E.D.; Tubau, E.; De Neys, W. The Doubting System 1: Evidence for automatic substitution sensitivity. Acta Psychol. 2016, 164, 56–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Travers, E.; Rolison, J.J.; Feeney, A. The time course of conflict on the Cognitive Reflection Test. Cognition 2016, 150, 109–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sirota, M.; Juanchich, M. Effect of response format on cognitive reflection: Validating a two and four-option multiple choice question version of the Cognitive Reflection Test. Behav. Res. Methods 2018, 50, 2511–2522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brown, S.D.; Heathcote, A. The simplest complete model of choice response time: Linear ballistic accumulation. Cogn. Psychol. 2008, 57, 153–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ratcliff, R.; Smith, P.L.; Brown, S.D.; McKoon, G. Diffusion decision model: Current issues and history. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2016, 20, 260–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paas, F.G.W.C.; Van Merriënboer, J.J.G. Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem-solving skills: A cognitive-load approach. J. Educ. Psychol. 1994, 86, 122–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Evans, J.S.B.T.; Curtis-Holmes, J. Rapid responding increases belief bias: Evidence for the dual-process theory of reasoning. Think. Reason. 2005, 11, 382–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boehm, U.; Van Maanen, L.; Forstmann, B.; van Rijn, H. Trial-by-trial fluctuations in CNV amplitude reflect anticipatory adjustment of response caution. Neuroimage 2014, 96, 95–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mulder, M.J.; van Maanen, L. Are accuracy and reaction time affected via different processes? PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e80222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rae, B.; Heathcote, A.; Donkin, C.; Averell, L.; Brown, S. The hare and the tortoise: Emphasizing speed can change the evidence used to make decisions. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2014, 40, 1226–1243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kemp, N.; Grieve, R. Face-to-face or face-to-screen? Undergraduates’ opinions and test performance in classroom vs. online learning. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 1278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Warschauer, M. Computer-mediated collaborative learning: Theory and practice. Mod. Lang. J. 1997, 8, 470–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hobbs, D. Constructivist approach to web course design: A review of the literature. Int. J. E-Learn. 2002, 1, 60–65. Available online: http://www.editlib.org/p/10821 (accessed on 6 March 2022).
- Oshri, A.; Cui, Z.; Owens, M.M.; Carvalho, C.A.; Sweet, L. Low-to-moderate level of perceived stress strengthens working memory: Testing the hormesis hypothesis through neural activation. Neuropsychologia 2022, 176, 108354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Oshri, A.; Cui, Z.; Carvalho, C.; Liu, S. Is perceived stress linked to enhanced cognitive functioning and reduced risk for psychopathology? Testing the hormesis hypothesis. Psychiatry Res. 2022, 314, 114644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bialek, M.; Pennycook, G. The cognitive reflection test is robust to multiple exposures. Behav. Res. Methods 2017, 50, 1953–1959. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Haigh, M. Has the standard cognitive reflection test become a victim of its own success? Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 2016, 12, 145–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stieger, S.; Reips, U.D. A limitation of the cognitive reflection test: Familiarity. Peer J. 2016, 4, e2395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoppe, E.I.; Kusterer, D.J. Behavioral biases and cognitive reflection. Econ. Lett. 2011, 110, 97–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spiegel, R.; Eacute. World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki. Gastroenterol. Jpn 1991, 26, 269–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hosseini, M.; Abidin, M.J.Z.; Baghdarnia, M. Comparability of test results of computer based tests (CBT) and paper and pencil tests (PPT) among English language learners in Iran. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 2014, 98, 659–667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jeong, H. A comparative study of scores on computer-based tests and paper-based tests. Behav. Inf. Technol. 2014, 33, 410–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sapolsky, R.M. Stress and the brain: Individual variability and the inverted-U. Nat. Neurosci. 2015, 18, 1344–1346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jamieson, J.P.; Koslov, K.R.; Nock, M.K.; Mendes, W.B. Experiences of discrimination increase risk taking. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 131–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Takemura, Y.; Kikuchi, S.; Inaba, Y. Does psychological stress improve physical performance? Tohoku J. Exp. Med. 1999, 187, 111–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cueva, C.; Iturbe-Ormaetxe, I.; Mata-Pérez, E.; Ponti, G.; Sartarelli, M.; Yu, H.; Zhukova, V. Cognitive (ir) reflection: New experimental evidence. J. Behav. Exp. Econ. 2016, 64, 81–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pennycook, G.; Cheyne, J.A.; Koehler, D.J.; Fugelsang, J.A. Is the cognitive reflection test a measure of both reflection and intuition? Behav. Res. Methods 2016, 48, 341–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Russo, J.E.; Dosher, B.A. Strategies for multiattribute binary choice. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1983, 9, 676–696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klein, G. The recognition-primed decision (RPD) model: Looking back, looking forward. Nat. Decis. Mak. 1997, 285, 292. [Google Scholar]
- Orasanu, J. Stress and naturalistic decision making: Strengthening the weak links. In Decision Making under Stress-Emerging Themes and Applications; Flin, R., Salas, E., Strubb, M., Matin, L., Eds.; Ashgate: Aldershot, UK, 1997; pp. 49–160. [Google Scholar]
- Orasanu, J.; Fischer, U. Finding decisions in natural environments: The view from the cockpit. In Naturalistic Decision Making; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 1997; pp. 343–357. [Google Scholar]
- Cokely, E.T.; Kelley, C.M. Cognitive abilities and superior decision making under risk: A protocol analysis and process model evaluation. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2009, 4, 20–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oechssler, J.; Roider, A.; Schmitz, P.W. Cognitive abilities and behavioral biases. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2009, 72, 147–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- d’Ailly, H.H. Asian mathematics superiority: A search for explanations. Educ. Psychol. 1992, 27, 243–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Geary, D.C.; Hamson, C.O.; Chen, G.-p.; Liu, F.; Hoard, M.K.; Salthouse, T.A. Computational and reasoning abilities in arithmetic: Cross-generational change in China and the United States. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1997, 4, 425–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Geary, D.C.; Liu, F.; Chen, G.-P.; Saults, S.J.; Hoard, M.K. Contributions of computational fluency to cross-national differences in arithmetical reasoning abilities. J. Educ. Psychol. 1999, 91, 716–719. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gerofsky, P.R. Why Asian Preschool Children Mathematically Outperform Preschool Children from Other Countries. West. Undergrad. Psychol. J. 2015, 3, 1–8. [Google Scholar]
- Di Paola, B.; Spagnolo, F. European and Chinese cognitive styles and their impact on teaching/learning Mathematics. J. Math. Educ. 2010, 3, 139–153. [Google Scholar]
- Jordan, K.E.; Baker, J. Multisensory information boosts numerical matching abilities in young children. Dev. Sci. 2011, 14, 205–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Di Paola, B. Can we learn from “outside”? A dialogue with a Chinese teacher: The “two basics” as a meaningful approach to mathematics teaching. Proc. CIEAEM 2015, 67, 579–585. [Google Scholar]
- Cheng, V.M. Progress from traditional to creativity education in Chinese societies. In Creativity: When East Meets West; World Scientific: Singapore, 2004; pp. 137–167. [Google Scholar]
- Wang, J.; Lin, E. Comparative studies on US and Chinese mathematics learning and the implications for standards-based mathematics teaching reform. Educ. Res. 2005, 34, 3–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Patel, N.; Baker, S.G.; Scherer, L.D. Evaluating the cognitive reflection test as a measure of intuition/reflection, numeracy, and insight problem solving, and the implications for understanding real-world judgments and beliefs. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2019, 148, 2129–2153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Evans, J.S.B.T. Dual-process theories of reasoning: Contemporary issues and developmental applications. Dev. Rev. 2011, 31, 86–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ma, C.E.; Rapee, R.M. Differences in mathematical performance, creativity potential, and need for cognitive closure between Chinese and Australian students. J. Creat. Behav. 2015, 49, 295–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pong, W.Y.; Chow, J.C.S. On the pedagogy of examinations in Hong Kong. Teach. Teach. Educ. 2002, 18, 139–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Number | Research | Participant (Female) | Task | Accuracy | Time limited |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | Boissin et al. [23] | 104 | Two-response paradigm (bat and ball) | 21.2% (pre-test) 17.2%/13.8% (study 1) 6.4%/15.3% (study 2) | Unlimited time to respond |
2 | Raoelison et al. [9] | 123 (79) | Two-response paradigm | 23.5% (slow)/19.9% (fast) | Intuition and 4 s/25 s |
3 | Janssen et al. [25] | 50 (30) | Two-response paradigm | 28.2%/27.7 (study 1) 15.1%/28.8% (study 2) | Intuition and 5 s |
4 | Raoelison et al. [26] | 100 | Two-response paradigm | 9.6%/13% (study 1) 12%/13.7% (study 2) | Intuition and 5 s |
5 | Bago et al. [22] | 231 (176) 143 (80) 140 (95) | 27.3% 17.8% 23.8 | Unlimited time to respond | |
6 | Frey et al. [27] | 248 | 21% | Unlimited time to respond | |
7 | Johnson et al. [28] | 313 (266) | 21.6% (no load) 15.9% (low load) 3.3% (high load) 3.3% (extra high load) | Unlimited time but load | |
8 | Pennycook et al. [5] | 372 (268) | Cognitive reflection test | 30.3% | / |
9 | Travers et al. [29] | 131 | Cognitive reflection test (3 item) | 36% | / |
10 | Sirota and Juanchich [30] | 452 (273) | Cognitive reflection test (7 item) | 39.5% | / |
11 | De Neys et al. [21] | 248 | Cognitive reflection test (3 item) | 21% | / |
Bat-and-Ball | Lily Pads | Machine | |
---|---|---|---|
Experiment 1: ACC | 0.685 (0.465) | 0.418 (0.498) | 0.610 (0.515) |
Experiment 2: ACC | 0.872 (0.349) | 0.846 (0.622) | 0.854 (0.353) |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Li, Z.; Yan, S.; Liu, J.; Bao, W.; Luo, J. Does the Cognitive Reflection Test Work with Chinese College Students? Evidence from a Time-Limited Study. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 348. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14040348
Li Z, Yan S, Liu J, Bao W, Luo J. Does the Cognitive Reflection Test Work with Chinese College Students? Evidence from a Time-Limited Study. Behavioral Sciences. 2024; 14(4):348. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14040348
Chicago/Turabian StyleLi, Zhaoxian, Shangsong Yan, Jie Liu, Wei Bao, and Junlong Luo. 2024. "Does the Cognitive Reflection Test Work with Chinese College Students? Evidence from a Time-Limited Study" Behavioral Sciences 14, no. 4: 348. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14040348
APA StyleLi, Z., Yan, S., Liu, J., Bao, W., & Luo, J. (2024). Does the Cognitive Reflection Test Work with Chinese College Students? Evidence from a Time-Limited Study. Behavioral Sciences, 14(4), 348. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14040348