Next Article in Journal
How Are Different Perfectionism Traits Related to Mental Health in Students?
Next Article in Special Issue
Social Understanding of Disability: Determinants and Levers for Action
Previous Article in Journal
Can Perceivers Differentiate Intense Facial Expressions? Eye Movement Patterns
Previous Article in Special Issue
I Do Not Want to Miss a Thing! Consequences of Employees’ Workplace Fear of Missing Out for ICT Use, Well-Being, and Recovery Experiences
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Measuring Attributions 50 Years on: From within-Country Poverty to Global Inequality

by
Franco Bastias
1,2,†,
Nadja Peter
3,†,
Aristobulo Goldstein
4,
Santiago Sánchez-Montañez
4,
Anette Rohmann
3 and
Helen Landmann
3,5,*
1
Cluster of Excellence “The Politics of Inequality”, University of Konstanz, 78464 Konstanz, Germany
2
National Scientific and Technical Research Council, Buenos Aires C1425FQB, Argentina
3
Faculty of Psychology, FernUniversität in Hagen, 58097 Hagen, Germany
4
Faculty of Philosophy and Humanities, Catholic University of Cuyo, San Juan 5400, Argentina
5
Department of Psychology, Universität Klagenfurt, 9020 Klagenfurt, Austria
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Behav. Sci. 2024, 14(3), 186; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14030186
Submission received: 31 October 2023 / Revised: 30 January 2024 / Accepted: 5 February 2024 / Published: 26 February 2024

Abstract

:
Fifty years after Feagin’s pioneering 1972 study, we present a systematic review of the measurement of attributions for poverty and economic inequality. We conducted a search for articles published from 1972 to 2023 in APA PsycArticles, Psychology and Behavioral Sciences Collection, APA PsycInfo, PSYNDEX Literature with PSYNDEX Tests, and Google Scholar. We used the following English keywords: “poor”, “poverty”, “inequality”, “attribution”, and “attributions” and their equivalents in Spanish. Applying our inclusion and exclusion criteria led to a final sample of 74 articles. We report three main findings. First, the majority of studies classify attributions on the dimensions of individualistic vs. structural. Second, there is a clear tendency to measure attributions for domestic poverty without considering supranational factors or poverty as a global challenge. Third, studies focus almost exclusively on poverty rather than (economic) inequality. We identify potential for future development within the literature, namely, from a domestic to a global perspective, from locus to controllability, and from poverty to inequality.

1. Introduction

According to the most recent human development report by the United Nations Development Programme, the number of people living in multidimensional poverty—that is, facing deprivations in health, educational opportunities and material standards of living—is as high as 1.3 billion today [1]. While the Human Development Index, measuring life expectancy, mean years of schooling, and gross national income per capita, dropped in most countries in the period from 2020 to 2021, countries with a lower index were more severely affected, highlighting the exacerbation of global inequalities due to recent crises such as climate change and the COVID-19 pandemic [2].
Psychological factors are relevant for the perpetuation of poverty and inequality since they influence (economic) behaviour and (political) preferences. Over the past decade, scholars have started examining the perceptions of global inequality, and in particular justice appraisals of global inequality [3,4]. Perceptions of global injustice have been found to be associated with behavioural intentions or actions directed at more global equality such as fair-trade consumption and environmental protection [5,6]. But when do people actually consider poverty and inequality as an injustice? One important factor influencing (in)justice perceptions is causal attribution.
In line with this reasoning, a wide array of research on poverty attributions demonstrates that the extent to which people support redistribution policies and welfare, engage in egalitarian action, or have intentions to help the poor very much depends on their causal explanations for poverty. In particular, those who perceive the causes of poverty to be rather structural, external to and uncontrollable by the poor embrace more egalitarian attitudes, as opposed to those who perceive the causes of poverty to be individualistic, internal to and controllable by the poor [7,8,9].

1.1. Measuring Attributions—A Brief History

One of the pioneering authors researching poverty attributions was Feagin [10], who classified explanations for poverty into three basic dimensions: (1) individualistic, including causes referring to dispositional and personal characteristics such as lack of will, motivation, or laziness; (2) structural, relating to environmental and societal factors like the job market, lack of opportunities, or discrimination; (3) fatalistic, covering explanations such as destiny, supernatural powers, and luck. During the 1970s, studies from India [11] and Australia [12] provided empirical evidence supporting Feagin’s claims and laying the foundations of the first taxonomy.
Over the next years, Weiner and collaborators further developed and extended attribution theory [13]. They studied how people’s causal explanations affect their behaviour and emotions and proposed a different taxonomy for causal attributions with the following dimensions: (a) locus, referring to the internal or external qualities of a cause; (b) stability , indicating whether causes are stable or unstable over time; (c) and controllability, related to a person’s experienced control or agency over the situation and ranging from controllable to uncontrollable [14,15]. However, while this approach has been influential in research on causal explanations for achievement in educational settings [for reviews, see [16,17]] and for the plights of different stigmatised groups [18,19,20,21], to date, it has received only little attention in the literature on attributions for economic outcomes [15,22,23]. These different paths of theoretical influences are outlined in Figure 1. (Following Weiner’s description of the dimensions of locus, stability, and controllability [13], Russell created a semantic differential scale called Causal Dimension Scale (CDS) [24]. A decade later, McAuley and colleagues presented a revised version of the scale, the Causal Dimension Scale II (CDSII) [25]. More than twenty years later, a revised version of the scale was used to study causal attributions of poverty by Osborne and Weiner [22]).
At the beginning of the millennium, Cozzarelli and colleagues validated a 22-item scale that included internal, external, and what they introduced as cultural attributions [26]. The authors questioned the relevance of fatalistic attributions (i.e., beliefs that outcomes are determined in advance and cannot be changed, e.g., fate). To account for this critique, they suggested a dimension referring to a culture of poverty with items such as “born in poverty” or “dysfunctional nuclear family”. This new proposal for measuring attributions was widely applied in many subsequent studies [27,28]. A couple of years later, Bullock and colleagues developed a scale called the Attributions for Poverty Questionnaire [29], a revised and improved version of her previous scale [30]. This new scale includes some items from Cozzarelli and colleagues and Furnham [26,31] and contains 45 items designed to assess individualistic, structural, and fatalistic attributions. Additionally, items referring to further structural causes were incorporated, e.g., the gap between men and women, the lack of adequate childcare, and (lack of) parental support.
While most research in the realm of poverty attributions so far has focused on domestic poverty, in the early 1990s, some scholars developed an interest in the global dynamics of poverty. Harper and colleagues designed the Causes of Third-World Poverty Scale [32], which was later on translated and validated in other languages [33]. Their principal component analysis indicated the following four factors: blame the poor, blame third-world governments, blame nature, and blame exploitation. The last two factors are unique compared to the studies by Feagin [10,34]. The dimension “blame nature” includes items such as “their land is not suitable for agriculture”, and “blame exploitation” includes items such as “the world economy and the banking system are against the poor”.
Figure 1. Theoretical influences of attribution measures [10,13,23,24,25,26,29,30,31,32,34,35,36,37].
Figure 1. Theoretical influences of attribution measures [10,13,23,24,25,26,29,30,31,32,34,35,36,37].
Behavsci 14 00186 g001

1.2. The Present Research

Over the past decades, different instruments with a variety of dimensions have been developed to measure poverty and inequality attributions. However, the study of attributions for poverty has been criticised for several reasons [38,39,40]. Firstly, most measures focus on domestic poverty and are based on data from countries with relatively low poverty rates [41,42,43,44]. Hence, they are developed from what has been called a WEIRD perspective (Western, Educated, Industrialised, Rich, Democratic [45]) and do not consider poverty as a global issue. Secondly, as Weiner [40] (p. 604) points out, “attribution theory has been built upon the idea that causal beliefs reside within (internal to) or outside (external to) the person”, even though some objects of inquiry may “fall[s] in the middle of a locus dimension anchored with internal and external”. That might apply to poverty and inequality, where it is difficult to locate the cause inside or outside a person if we conceptualise the phenomena as a relationship between the individual and their context. Relatedly, current studies emphasise poverty rather than the concept of inequality. More specifically, examining inequality as opposed to poverty considers the advantaged as well as the disadvantaged group or person—an important aspect given the historical and societal mechanisms through which social injustice is perpetuated. Finally, some authors have discussed the fact that the poverty attributions scales are based on items developed several decades ago [33].
To date, however, the criticism regarding research on poverty attributions has rarely been based on a systematic review of the literature [46,47]. In the present study, we intend to close this gap by conducting a systematic review of the different scales used to measure attributions for poverty and economic inequality between 1972 and 2023. In particular, we examine the following research questions:
  • On which domains do the scales focus (i.e., attributions of domestic poverty, global poverty, domestic inequality, or global inequality)?
  • On which theoretical approaches are the scales assessing poverty and inequality attributions based?
  • Which dimensions of attributions are covered?
  • In what countries were the samples collected?

2. Materials and Methods

A search was carried out for articles published in the period between 1972 and 2023 using the databases APA PsycArticles, Psychology and Behavioral Sciences Collection, APA PsycInfo, PSYNDEX Literature with PSYNDEX Tests, and Google Scholar. The final search was carried out in December 2023 (see Table A1, in the Appendix A). The keywords used were poor, poverty, inequality, attributions and attribution and their equivalents in Spanish: pobre, pobreza, desigualdad, atribuciones and atribución. These words were combined as follows: poor + attributions; poverty + attributions; inequality + attributions; poor + attribution; poverty + attribution; inequality + attribution. A filter was used such that the keywords had to appear in the title of the article.
During the selection process (see Figure 2), some articles were discarded in line with the following exclusion criteria: unpublished undergraduate and graduate theses, conference papers, qualitative studies and papers that did not use scales to measure the construct. For the search referring to inequality, we also excluded any articles referring to gender inequality and other types of inequality beyond economic, wealth, and income inequality. Finally, a list of 74 articles was obtained, of which 63 were written in English and 11 in Spanish.

3. Results

We analysed articles concerning the following information: author and year of publication, country of origin of the samples, scope of attribution, sample size and properties, theoretical reference for the scale employed, number of items in the scale and scale response anchors, scale dimensions and their respective reliability. Appendix A details the information collected.

3.1. Sample Characteristics

As shown in Appendix A, most of the identified articles were written in English, while 14.9% were written in Spanish. Regarding the continent of origin of the samples included, 27 studies used samples from North America (with only 2 studies from Mexico), 23 from Europe, 14 from Asia, including Turkey and the Middle East, 7 from South America, 5 from Oceania, 5 from Africa, and 2 from Central America. Hence, approximately 65% of the research collected data in Europe, Canada, or the US. A total of 29 studies surveyed university students. All articles except 12 used Likert-type scales to measure attributions (see Appendix A).

3.2. Theoretical Influences

The attributional processes involved in understanding poverty and inequality can be considered a phenomenon that is psychological, sociological, and political in nature. Consequently, over the last 50 years, authors from the fields of psychology (e.g., Adrian Furnham, Bernard Weiner, Catherine Cozzarelli, David J. Harper, Heather Bullock), sociology (e.g., James R. Kluegel, Joe Feagin), and political science (e.g., Kevin B. Smith, Wim van Oorschot) have engaged in dialogue with each other through their work to understand how people explain poverty and inequality. Thus, in our review, despite focusing mainly on psychological studies of attribution processes, the underlying dialogue with other disciplines was revealed in different ways. For instance, a few articles from other social science disciplines besides psychology emerged in the search [48,49], and we were able to identify influences from other disciplines in psychology studies, mainly political science [50,51] and sociology [10,35,36].
As depicted in Figure 3, a large majority of the studies were explicitly guided by Feagin’s theoretical proposition and employed his scale either in its original version [10] or retained an indirect influence by employing scales designed based on Feagin’s work, such as those by Cozzarelli et al. [26], Bullock et al. [29,30], and others (see Figure 1).
Only six studies took a more global perspective by employing the Causes of Third-World Poverty Questionnaire developed by Harper and colleagues [32], who also developed their items based on Feagin [10]. Eight studies employed the Attributions for Poverty Scale by Cozzarelli et al. [26], which was based on Smith and Stone [37]. This scale places particular emphasis on the cultural aspect of poverty. Another eight studies opted for the Attributions for Poverty Questionnaire [29,30], which also drew inspiration from Cozzarelli et al., and Furnham [26,31]. Furnham’s measure was employed six times by different authors [31]. Three works used the Attributions for Poverty Questionnaire by Kluegel and Smith [35,36]; they employed a version of Furnham’s questionnaire [31] in addition to Feagin’s scale [10]. Furthermore, another three studies employed the Beliefs About Poverty Scale designed by Smith and Stone [37] based on both Feagin [34] and Kluegel and Smith [35,36]. Finally, two studies opted for the scale designed by van Oorschot and Halman [50], who drew inspiration from Kluegel and Smith [35,36].

3.3. Dimensionality of Poverty and Inequality Attributions

Since the vast majority of studies and scales descended directly or indirectly from the work of Feagin [10,34], the dimensionality of the scales used follows the original three-category typology of poverty explanations. As shown in Appendix A, 40 studies employed individualistic and structural dimensions. (Note that among the studies reviewed, individualistic is sometimes called individual, individualism, and dispositional; while structural is at times renamed as structuralistic, sociostructural, societal, or situational. Despite the varying terminology, the original meaning of the two dimensions by Feagin [10] is largely preserved.) The fatalistic dimension was present in 30 articles as well. By contrast, the distinction between internal and external factors, mainly following Weiner’s approach [15,22,23], was employed in 22 studies. Furthermore, factor structures based on Harper [32,38] were found in four studies, with dimensions such as blame exploitation, blame the poor, blame conflict, blame nature, and blame the governments of third-world countries (see Appendix A). Finally, the cultural dimension was present in nine papers. Only a few studies contained other dimensions, such as guilt/fate [50]; motivation [51,52]; family and morality [53]; problems in romantic relationships and related to having children [54]; chance [55]; competition, social attractiveness, and physical attractiveness [56].

4. Discussion

Our systematic review provides an overview of measurements of attributions for poverty and economic inequality employed in psychological studies. It highlights the focus on domestic poverty compared to global poverty or global inequality as well as the dominance of specific theoretical approaches and WEIRD samples. We identify a potential for further development within the literature, namely, from a domestic to a global perspective, from locus to controllability, and from poverty to inequality, which are discussed in the following sections.

4.1. From A Domestic to A Global Perspective

Most of the studies that resulted from our search focused on domestic or within-country poverty. In fact, several of the cross-national studies found are concerned with comparing how the populations of different countries explain internal economic problems within their own borders [57,58], rather than examining how global poverty and inequality between countries are explained. Of the few exceptions we identified as taking a global perspective, all were based on the Causes of Third-World Poverty Scale developed by Harper et al. [32], who in turn had based their items on Feagin [10]. Among those, Hine and Montiel are the only ones to have added a few items to the scale based on actual qualitative data, namely interviews with NGOs in Canada and the Philippines [7]. Moreover, except Vázquez and colleagues [59], all articles were published more than ten years ago. Thus, it can be assumed that the respective scales do not accurately reflect the current political and societal context. This becomes even more clear when looking at the actual items of the Causes of Third-World Poverty Questionnaire [7,32] in more detail, some of which use language that can be considered offensive from today’s perspective. The same is true for the very name of the scale, referring to the “third world”, which is not considered appropriate any longer. These results point to the need for an up-to-date scale.
The lack of a global perspective in studies on poverty and inequality attributions is evident in two additional aspects. First, supranational factors are scarce to non-existent among the most common causes suggested by researchers and listed in their scales for respondents to explain their domestic economic issues (for the list of causes, see [23]). As Baute & Pellegata [60] show in their study, in multi-level governance systems, citizens can attribute responsibility for economic results to supranational institutions, such as the European Union. Second, we found that WEIRD samples are overrepresented in inequality and poverty attribution studies, at least among top journals. About 65% of the research reviewed was based on data from Europe, Canada, or the US. There was a particularly strong bias in favour of US samples, at 23 out of the 74 articles resulting from our search. In contrast, Asia, Latin America, Oceania, and Africa were represented with only a small number of studies each.
This is in line with previous criticism of psychological research overall and research on poverty attributions in particular. With regard to the former, analyses of publications in six top psychological journals from 2003 to 2007 by Arnett [61] and from 2014 to 2018 by Thalmayer and colleagues [62] have shown that even though the percentage of studies based on US samples has decreased over the past decade or so, this is mainly due to an increasing number of studies using samples from other English-speaking countries and Western Europe. The majority of the world, on the other hand, is still severely underrepresented. Only 4–5% of the studies in the journals analysed were based on samples from non-WEIRD countries, even though they represent 89% of the world’s population [62].
These numbers are especially concerning with regard to the topic of poverty and inequality attributions. The parts of the world which, in global comparison, are most affected by the issues of poverty and most disadvantaged by global inequality are the ones least represented in research on this topic. This has previously been discussed concerning Asian countries [41,43] and Latin America [42,44] and holds equally true for the African continent. Overall, global poverty and inequality are urgent matters that deserve to receive more attention from attribution researchers. Our results highlight the need for an up-to-date scale grounded in the current societal context and taking into account perspectives from different parts of the world. Future research should further investigate whether the dimensions of poverty and inequality attributions replicate across different countries and cultures with scales adapted for the respective context.

4.2. From Locus to Controllability

Our findings show that 50 years after Feagin’s pioneering study on poverty attributions [10], his approach still remains very influential in the field. The vast majority of the reviewed studies are based on his tripartite operationalisation of attributions with the individualistic, structural, and fatalistic dimensions. However, we point out that this original three-part typology has its shortcomings. On the one hand, the fatalistic dimension does not seem to be as relevant as the individualistic and structural ones [35,36,63,64,65,66,67] and has been criticised for its low reliability [8,29,67,68,69,70,71]. (In particular, when “the poor” are referred to in more specific categories or characterised in certain ways (e.g., immigrants, families with children, the elderly), the distinction between individualistic and structural explanations remains for the different groups, while it is rare for the fatalistic dimension to emerge [39].) On the other hand, the individualistic dimension conflates causes that could be the individual’s fault and others that are not, despite this being a fundamental nuance to make when it comes to understanding people’s reactions to poverty [23,72]. Why do physical disability, laziness, and drug use, all internal causes of poverty, give rise to very different reactions [72]? We develop this point below.
Many authors stick to Heider’s [73] original proposal calling the individualistic–structural pair internal–external [48,74] and employing them practically as synonyms [75]. Nevertheless, following Weiner [22,23], we believe that talking about internal–external represents a new configuration of causal beliefs, more complex than Feagin’s proposal, since it implies defining internal or external to what. That is, if the cause of poverty is internal or external to the person, one is referring to locus; if the cause of poverty is internal or external to the person’s agency, one is referring to controllability. (In his 2 × 2 × 2 taxonomy, in addition to locus and controllability, Weiner proposes a third dimension: the stability of the causes. The stability of poverty over time and generations may suggest that the issue goes beyond the control of the individual, as Osborne and Weiner showed by reporting a negative correlation between stability and personal control [22]. However, this study also indicated the scarce relevance of this dimension as a predictor of personal helping behaviour.) Beyond individualistic–structural differentiation, this classification of locus and control effectively opens new possibilities to explore how people (including researchers) understand poverty as a highly complex phenomenon, where it is common to erroneously consider correlates as causes of poverty and where in certain conceptual definitions of the phenomenon, such as multidimensional poverty [76,77], the correlates may be better described as dimensions of poverty.
In particular, Weiner’s taxonomy allows us to explore explanations of poverty that conceive that the causes are located within the person, but are external in their controllability, such as lack of education, child malnutrition, disabilities, etc. This differentiation is especially important for capturing intergenerational explanations of poverty. My parents’ deprivation might result in my poor access to education, leading to my impoverishment. My own poverty might then be attributed internally to my lack of education, while in fact the cause—my parents’ poverty—is external and controllability lies within external factors like the school system. As in this example, the causes of poverty are often located within the poor when they are confused with the traces poverty leaves on the individual. In fact, epigenetic studies show that people’s genetics can be predicted by their ancestors’ exposure to sustained poverty and social inequality [78]. In this case, claiming that “poverty is in genetics” is not saying that there is a genetic cause but rather that these particular genetics could also be the consequence or correlation of ancestral poverty. In the public debate around poverty, there is not always a clear distinction between causes, correlates, and consequences.
We believe that bringing the aspect of controllability into the discussion helps shift the focus from where the cause of poverty lies (within or outside the person) to who is responsible and who can take action (internal or external controllability). Leaving aside the discussions of internal–external locus seems reasonable in the explanation of a phenomenon that emerges from the interaction of person–situation or person–society and is thus an inherently social issue. Simultaneously, as the internal-or-external locus discussion sometimes leads to a mere description of reality, foregrounding debates on controllability and responsibility can be a first step to engaging in actions to actually reduce poverty.

4.3. From Poverty to Inequality

In the social sciences, the phenomenon of poverty has acquired multiple conceptualisations [79]. Some studies, for example, rely on the concept of poverty as a lack of the financial and material goods necessary to live in a certain society, while others adopt a multidimensional approach, defining poverty as an unjust deprivation of social, economic, and cultural rights (including housing, work, food security, clothing, education, and healthcare, among others) affecting the development of human capacities and social integration [76,77]. Not only in academia but also for research participants, poverty is far from an unequivocal concept. Research has shown that “poverty” and “the poor” employed as a general stimulus can bring to mind images and associations ranging from people suffering from hunger to homeless people, from people experiencing a lack of money to a lack of rights [80]. Thus, it is worth asking: attributions of what kind of poverty are we investigating?
In fact, a critique of mainstream research on poverty attributions is that studies have almost exclusively relied on a generic conceptualisation of poverty and “the poor” as a homogeneous group [39,64,81,82], failing to acknowledge that different definitions and types of poverty might trigger different causal interpretations [39]. In our review, when reading full texts, we also often found it difficult to identify attributions of what type of poverty were being captured since, in most cases, participants were asked to explain the causes of poverty without further specification (e.g., Please rate the importance of each of those factors as causes of poverty from 1 (not at all important) to 5 (extremely important) [26]). However, we assume that many studies implicitly deal with monetary poverty attributions, since when listing the causes of poverty in their measurements, they largely mention economic aspects (“lack of thrift and proper money management”, “prejudice and discrimination in promotion and wages” [26]).
In public opinion research, where opinions (in this case, attributions) can change easily depending on how survey questions are framed [83], we believe that the use of “poverty” as a prompt to explore explanations for economic outcomes may have downsides. These are not only due to its polysemic connotation as explained above, but also to its framing (as in the case of monetary poverty) of economic suffering. Accordingly, we instead see potential in focusing on the concept of economic inequality in the study of attributions, which may bring in a more process-oriented and relational understanding of individuals’ economic plight. While poverty tends to be understood as a descriptive concept of the state of lack experienced by a person or group, focusing on who suffers from poverty [80], inequality is by definition a relational concept with a focus on the dynamic between the disadvantaged and the advantaged. In that sense, since inequality always implies a comparison group, it does not allow the system of relationships from which poverty arises to remain invisible. By asking respondents to explain economic (pay, income, or wealth) differences, it is possible to explore attributions of economic suffering without neglecting the social background in which it occurs.
Our review identifies attributions for inequality as a major gap in this field of study. The results show a strikingly small number of studies in this regard and, in our review, no scale measuring attributions for economic inequality was found. Considering the arguments outlined above, the field would benefit from incorporating the concept of inequality in future research to be able to adequately address the societal challenges posed by it today (see [84] for a recent qualitative study on global inequality attributions).

4.4. Limitations

The main limitation of our review is that, due to the search criteria employed, we might not have covered the entire landscape of studies on attributions for poverty and economic inequality. Since we restricted the results to articles mentioning the keywords in the title, we might have missed out on publications which, for example, did not mention attributions explicitly in the title, but rather referred to attitudes or explanations. However, the current study still presents an extensive overview of the literature, and the trends we identified, namely a WEIRD bias in samples, the preponderance of certain theoretical approaches, and a lack of scales focusing on the majority world and the concept of inequality, are unlikely to be affected by the studies that were potentially omitted.

5. Conclusions

In Weiner’s article “Wither attribution theory?”, the author points out that the field needs to tap into new areas and expand to new phenomena the theory originally did not address [40]. He calls for the introduction of new theoretical challenges that force reconceptualisation and rethinking in the realm of attribution research. The current systematic review makes a substantial contribution to this by pointing to various gaps and limitations of the instruments employed to measure poverty and inequality attributions over the years. It also serves as a call for researchers to critically reflect on how we understand and study economic plights within countries and globally.
Our review analyses what, how, and who is being measured in the literature on poverty and inequality attribution. We found that the current scales focus on poverty rather than inequality and therefore, given that poverty is not always understood as a relational concept, run the risk of making the relationship between the rich and the poor as well as the dynamics leading to the perpetuation of economic plights invisible. Our results further show that most scales to date are based on the structural and individualistic dimensions introduced by Feagin [10] and suggest that the field could benefit from a stronger focus on controllability and responsibility as in the taxonomy by Weiner [22,23]. Finally, we identified that most scales were employed to measure domestic poverty, developed from a WEIRD perspective and applied to WEIRD populations [45]. There is a lack of consideration of poverty and inequality as a global issue, especially global inequality. This seems particularly relevant today—even though global poverty has decreased overall since the early 1990s [85], economic inequality is at a historical peak [86]. Furthermore, as Piketty [87] points out, inequalities have actually widened in times of global economic growth.
On a different note, the importance of controllability and responsibility as well as the concept of inequality as discussed here come with certain implications for policymakers. Focusing on those in control and considering both sides of the divide between the advantaged and the disadvantaged makes the systematic nature in which economic and in particular global inequality is maintained and reproduced clearly visible. To break the cycle in which this inequality is reproduced over and over again would mean shifting from charity to justice, as Hickel [88] puts it. Charity and aid cannot alleviate poverty since the financial resources for such assistance stem from the very processes leading to poverty in the first place [88]. Importantly, they also do not put the disadvantaged back in control of their situation but instead reproduce and stabilise existing hierarchies and dependencies. Hence, Hickel [88] calls for changing the rules that perpetuate poverty by implementing measures such as abolishing debt burdens, democratising global institutions, and introducing a universal basic income, amongst others. However, these truly empowering measures will likely only come onto the table once politicians as well as the broader public cease to attribute responsibility to the poor and poor countries themselves.
The current human development report by the United Nations Development Programme refers to a new uncertainty complex that we live in today, with humanity facing a variety of crises, some of them very much related to (global) inequality [1]. However, these uncertainties also constitute a chance to reimagine, renew, and adapt to today’s challenges. The research on inequality attributions holds the potential to be part of this path forward.

Author Contributions

Conceptualisation, F.B., N.P., A.G., S.S.-M., A.R. and H.L.; methodology, F.B., N.P., A.G. and S.S.-M.; formal analysis, F.B. and N.P.; writing—original draft preparation, F.B. and N.P.; writing—review and editing, F.B., N.P., A.R. and H.L.; visualisation, F.B. and A.G.; supervision, A.R. and H.L.; All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The first author received funding from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG—German Research Foundation) under the German Excellence Strategy—EXC-2035/1–390681379.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Acknowledgments

We want to thank Jascha Naumann for support with formatting the manuscript and Leonard Harmening for support with the literature research.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Searches in free and paid databases.
Table A1. Searches in free and paid databases.
Search Engine and DatabasesKeyword CombinationResults
Google Scholarpoverty AND attributions113
poverty AND attribution40
poor AND attributions50
poor AND attribution44
inequality AND attributions23
inequality AND attribution16
pobreza AND atribuciones28
pobreza AND atribución3
pobre AND atribuciones1
pobre AND atribución0
desigualdad AND atribuciones1
desigualdad AND atribución1
Total320
EBSCO host
(including APA PsycArticles, Psychology and Behavioral Sciences Collection, APA PsycInfo, PSYNDEX Literature with PSYNDEX Tests)
poverty AND attributions73
poverty AND attribution73
poor AND attributions31
poor AND attribution31
inequality AND attributions14
inequality AND attribution14
pobreza AND atribuciones4
pobreza AND atribución0
pobre AND atribuciones1
pobre AND atribución0
desigualdad AND atribuciones0
desigualdad AND atribución0
Total241
Table A2. Studies measuring poverty attributions from 1972 to 2023.
Table A2. Studies measuring poverty attributions from 1972 to 2023.
Authors and Year of PublicationScope of AttributionsSample CountrySample Size and ProfileTheoretical Reference№ of Items and Scale Response AnchorsDimensions and Their Reliability *
1Abouchedid and Nasser (2002) [89]Domestic poverty or without specificationLebanon and Portugal372 university studentsFeagin (1972; 1975) [10,34]15 items, 1–5 (disagree–agree)Lebanon: structural (α = 0.63), individualist (α = 0.67), fatalist (α = 0.67);
Portugal: structural (α = 0.54), individualist (α = 0.70), fatalist (α = 0.77)
2Alcañiz-Colomer, Moya, and Valor-Segura (2023) [90]Domestic poverty or without specificationSpain484 (study 1, survey respondents); 256 (study 2, undergraduate students); 358 (study 3, survey respondents)Furnham, (1982) [31], Weiner et al., (2011) [23]20 items, 1–5 (strongly disagree–strongly agree) + 4 items for the Spanish contextIndividualistic (α = 0.80/0.83/0.71), structural (α = 0.81/0.81)
3Bai, Xu, Yang, and Guo (2023) [91]Domestic poverty or without specificationChina448 (study 1)Li (2014) [92]16 items, 1–7 (totally disagree–totally agree)Internal (α = 0.79), external (α = 0.76)
4Bennett, Raiz, and Davis (2016) [27]Domestic poverty or without specificationUSA209 social workersBullock (2004) [68], Bullock et al., (2003a) [29], Weiss-Gal and Gal (2007) [93]33 items, 1–6 (fully agree–fully disagree)Individual (α = 0.94), structural (α = 0.88), cultural (α = 0.77)
5Bergmann and Todd (2019) [94]Domestic poverty or without specificationUSA189 (study 1) and 646 (study 2) university studentsCozzarelli et al., (2001) [26]13 items, 1–5 (not important at all–extremely important)Internal (α = 0.83), external (α = 0.80)
6Bobbio, Canova, and Manganelli (2010) [95]Domestic poverty or without specificationItaly181 university studentsFeagin (1972) [10], Smith and Stone (1989) [37]12 items, 1–5 (not important at all–extremely important)Internal/individualistic (α = 0.82), external/structuralistic (α = 0.74)
7Bradley and Cole (2002) [96]Domestic poverty or without specificationCanada and the USA714 survey respondents aged 18 or olderFeagin (1975) [34]11 items, 1–3 (very important–not important at all)Internal (α = 0.60), external (α = 0.62)
8Bullock (1999) [30]Domestic poverty or without specificationUSA236 survey respondentsFurnham (1982) [31]16 items, 1–7 (strongly disagree–strongly agree)Individualistic, structural, structural–fatalistic
9Bullock, Williams, and Limbert (2003) [29]Domestic poverty or without specificationUSA131 university studentsBullock (1999) [30], Cozzarelli et al., (2001) [26], Furnham (1982) [31]45 items, 1–7 (disagree–agree)Structural (α = 0.91), individualistic/poverty culture (α = 0.91), fatalistic/structural (α = 0.72)
10Canto, Perles, and San Martín (2012) [97]Global povertySpain300 university studentsHine and Montiel (1999) [7], adapted by Betancor et al., (2002) [98]22 items, 1–6 (fully disagree–fully agree)Structural, personal, fatalistic
11Carr, Taef, De M.S. Ribeiro and MacLachlan (1998) [99]Global povertyAustralia and Brazil100 textile workersHarper et al., (1990) [32]16 items, 1–5 (disagree–agree)Nature, the poor, local governments, exploitation
12Carr and MacLachlan (1998) [100]Global povertyAustralia and Malawi582 university studentsHarper et al., (1990) [32]20 items, 1–5 (disagree–agree)Blame the poor, blame international exploitation, blame nature, blame third-world governments
13Castillo and Rivera-Gutiérrez (2018) [74]Domestic poverty or without specificationChile1245 survey respondents aged 18 or olderFeagin (1972) [10]5 items, 1–5 (never–always)Internal/individual, external/sociocultural
14Cheng and Ngok (2023) [101]Domestic poverty or without specificationChina10,855/10,678 survey respondentsFeagin (1972) [10]5 items (dummy variables)Individualistic, structural, fatalistic
15Cojanu and Stroe (2017) [102]Domestic poverty or without specificationRomania600 beneficiaries of guaranteed minimum incomeN/A10 items, 1–2 (ordinal scale)Individual, structural/societal, fatalistic
16Cozzarelli, Wilkinson, and Tagler (2001) [26]Domestic poverty or without specificationUSA209 university studentsFeagin (1972) [10], Smith and Stone (1989) [37]22 items, 1–5 (not important at all–very important)Internal (α = 0.75), external (α = 0.79), cultural (α = 0.65)
17da Costa and Dias (2013) [57]Domestic poverty or without specification13 European countries15,504 respondents above age 15N/A11 items (dummy variables)Individualistic/internal, structural, Fatalist
18Davidai (2018) [103]Domestic poverty or without specificationUSA397 survey respondentsKluegel and Smith (1986) [36]7 items, 1–7 (not so important–extremely important)Internal, external
19Engler, Strassle, and Steck (2019) [104]Domestic poverty or without specificationUSA161 primary and secondary education administrative employeesCozzarelli et al., (2001) [26]22 itemsInternal, external
20Frei, Castillo, Herrera, and Suárez (2020) [105]Domestic poverty or without specificationChile2954 survey respondents aged 18 or olderN/A10 items, 1–2 (ordinal scale)Internal, external, ambivalent
21Gatica and Navarro-Lashayas (2019) [106]Domestic poverty or without specificationSpain184 university studentsBullock et al., (2003) [29]32 items, 1–5 (not important–very important)Sociostructural (α = 0.91), individual (α = 0.91), fatalistic (α = 0.72)
22Generalao (2005) [107]Domestic poverty or without specificationPhilippines373 housewives (145 from rural areas and 228 from urban areas)Weiner (1985) [14]7 items, 1–5 (semantic differential scale)Locus, controllability, Stability
23Gonzalez, Macchia, and Whillans (2022) [108]Domestic inequality or without specificationUSA200 survey respondentsHussak and Cimpian (2015) [109]3 items (forced choice)Uncontrollable dispositional/controllable dispositional/uncontrollable situational
24Griffin and Oheneba-Sakyi (1993) [110]Domestic poverty or without specificationUSA207 undergraduate studentsN/A1 item (dummy variable)Individual
25Habibov, Cheung, Auchynnikava, and Fan (2017) [58]Domestic poverty or without specification24 European and Asian countries37,307 survey respondents aged 17 or olderN/A1 item (dummy variable)Structural
26Halik, Malek, Bahari, Matshah, and Webley (2012) [111]Domestic poverty or without specificationMalaysia124 university studentsFeagin (1972) [10], Furnham (1982) [31]16 items, 1–5 (strongly disagree–strongly agree)Individualistic (α = 0.71), structural (α = 0.63), fatalistic (α = 0.58)
27Heaven (1989) [112]Domestic poverty or without specificationAustralia285 survey respondents aged 18 or olderFeagin (1972) [10], Furnham (1982) [31]11 itemsSocietal (α = 0.72), negative individualistic (α = 0.75), characterological (α = 0.66)
28Hill, Toft, Garrett, Ferguson, and Kuechler (2016) [113]Domestic poverty or without specificationUSA337 university studentsFeagin (1972; 1975) [10,34]7 items, 1–5 (agree–disagree)Individual (α = 0.66), structural (α = 0.70)
29Husz, Kopasz, and Medgyesi (2022) [114]Domestic poverty or without specificationHungary600 social workersFeagin (1972) [10]10 items, 1–4 (strongly agree–strongly disagree)Structural, individualistic
30Ige and Nekhwevha (2012) [115]Domestic poverty or without specificationSouth Africa383 survey respondentsFeagin (1972) [10]38 items, 1–5 (strongly disagree–strongly agree)Structural (α = 0.86), individual (α = 0.91), fatalistic (α = 0.85)
31Kafetsios and Kateri (2022) [116]Domestic inequality or without specificationGreece846 survey respondentsN/A12 itemsDispositional, contextual
32Kitchens, Ricks, and Hannor-Walker (2020) [117]Domestic poverty or without specificationUSA91 university studentsBullock (1999) [30]36 items, 1–5 (not at all important as a cause of poverty–extremely important as a cause of poverty)Individualistic (α = 0.91), structuralistic (α = 0.91), fatalistic (α = 0.72)
33Landmane and Reņģe (2010) [118]Domestic poverty or without specificationLatvia202 womenBullock et al., (2003) [29], Bullock (2004) [68]30 items, 1–5 (strong disagreement–strong agreement)Family/fatalistic (α = 0.86), individualistic (α = 0.79), structural (α = 0.77)
34Lee, Park, Rhee, Kim, Lee, Ha, Baik, and Ahn (2021) [119]Domestic poverty or without specificationSouth Korea, Japan, USA2213 survey respondents representative of each country’s populationFeagin (1972, 1975) [10,34]8 items, 1–5 (strongly disagree–strongly agree)Individualistic (α = 0.79), societal (α = 0.64)
35Ljubotina and Ljubotina (2007) [120]Domestic poverty or without specificationCroatia365 university studentsFeagin (1975) [34]24 items, 1–5 (completely disagree–completely agree)Individual (α = 0.76), structural (α = 0.79), fatalistic (α = 0.70), micro-environmental/cultural (α = 0.65)
36McWha and Carr (2009) [121]Global povertyNew Zealand171 university studentsHarper et al., (1990) [32]17 items, 1–5 (strongly disagree–strongly agree)Blame the poor (α = 0.77), blame third-world governments (α = 0.70), blame nature (α = 0.56)
37Mickelson and Hazlett (2014) [54]Domestic poverty or without specificationUSA66 low-income women with at least one child aged 1–6 yearsBullock et al., (2003) [29]37 items, 1–5 (did not contribute at all–contributed a lot)Structural (α = 0.90), individualistic (α = 0.76), children (α = 0.71), romantic relationships (α = 0.66), fatalistic (α = 0.65)
38Murry, Brody, Brown, Wisenbaker, Cutrona, and Simons (2002) [122]Domestic poverty or without specificationUSA96 single mothers who receive government welfareConger (1995) [123]16 items, 1–4 (agree–disagree)External (α total = 0.76)
39Nasser (2007) [124]Domestic poverty or without specificationLebanon242 high-school studentsFeagin (1972) [10]N/AStructuralistic, individualistic, fatalistic
40Nasser and Abouchedid (2001) [125]Domestic poverty or without specificationLebanon232 university studentsFeagin (1972) [10], Hunt (1996) [65], Morcol (1997) [126], Griffin and Oheneba-Sakyi (1993) [110], Williamson (1974) [127]15 items, 1–5 (strongly agree–strongly disagree)Structuralist (α = 0.70), individualist status quo (α = 0.60), fatalist (α = 0.70), individual blaming the poor/societal (α = 0.50)
41Nasser and Abouchedid (2006) [128]Domestic poverty or without specificationLebanon, South Africa443 university studentsFeagin (1972) [10]15 items, 1–5 (strongly agree–strongly disagree)Individualism (α = 0.71), fatalism (α = 0.62), structuralism (α = 0.50)
42Nasser, Singhal, and Abouchedid (2005) [129]Domestic poverty or without specificationIndia365 high-school and university studentsFeagin (1972) [10], adapted by Nasser and Abouchedid (2001) [125]17 items, 1–5 (fully agree–fully disagree)Individualistic, structural, fatalistic (α total = 0.63)
43Nelson and Joselus (2023) [130]Domestic inequality or without specificationUSA448 survey respondentsPeffley, Hurwitz, and Mondak (2017) [131]7 items, 1–6 (not important at all–extremely important)Internal (α = 0.915), cultural (α = 0.906), external
44Niemelä (2011) [39]Domestic poverty or without specificationFinland2006 social security officials and other citizensFeagin (1972) [10], van Oorschot and Halman (2000) [50], Saunders (2003) [132]11 items, 1–5 (strongly agree–strongly disagree)Individual, individual–structural, structural, fatalistic
45Norcia, Castellani, and Rissotto (2010) [133]Domestic poverty or without specification Italy1914 survey respondentsN/A7 items, 1–5 (never–very often)Internal, external, fatalism
46Norcia and Risotto (2011) [134]Domestic poverty or without specificationItaly1914 survey respondentsN/A7 items, 1–5Powerful others, chance, internal
47Norcia and Rissotto (2015) [55]Domestic poverty or without specificationItaly992 survey respondentsN/A8 items, 1–5Internal (α = 0.57), powerful other (α = 0.66), chance (α = 0.63)
48Osborne and Weiner (2015) [22]Domestic poverty or without specificationNew Zealand and the USA315 university studentsMcAuley et al., (1992) [25]12 items, 1–7 (semantic differential scale)Locus (α = 0.79), stability (α = 0.65), personal control (α = 0.83), other control (α = 0.71)
49Özpinar and Akdede (2022) [135]Domestic poverty or without specificationTurkey1110 participantsFeagin (1972) [10]7 items (dummy variables)Individualistic, Structural, Fatalistic
50Pandey, Sinha, Prakash, and Tripathi (1982) [136]Domestic poverty or without specificationIndia90 university studentsSinha et al., (1980) [137]8 items, 1–5 (completely disagree–completely agree)Self, fate, governmental policies, economic dominance
51Piff, Wiwad, Robinson, Aknin, Mercier, and Shariff (2020) [8]Domestic poverty or without specificationUSA602 survey respondentsFeagin (1972) [10], Kluegel and Smith (1986) [36]12 items, 1–5 (not so important–extremely important)Situational attributions (α = 0.85), dispositional attributions (α = 0.79)
52Reutter, Veenstra, Stewart, Raphael, Love, Makwarimba, and McMurray (2006) [138]Domestic poverty or without specificationCanada1671 survey respondentsvan Oorschot and Halman (2000) [50]5 items, 1–5 (strongly disagree–strongly agree)Structural, sociocultural, individualistic, fatalistic
53Reyna, Acosta, Saavedra, and Correa (2018) [139]Domestic poverty or without specificationArgentina280 survey respondentsCozzarelli et al., (2001) [26]23 items, 1–5 (not important for poverty–extremely important as a poverty cause)Internal (α = 0.77), sociostructural (α = 0.76), fatalistic (α = 0.69)
54Reyna and Reparaz (2014) [28]Domestic poverty or without specificationArgentina177 university studentsCozzarelli et al., (2001) [26]23 items, 1–5 (not important for poverty–extremely important as a poverty cause)Internal (α = 0.74), external (α = 0.73), cultural (α = 0.79)
55Ríos-Rodríguez, Moreno-Jiménez, and Vallejo Martín (2022) [140]Global povertySpain720 survey respondentsN/A17 itemsCultural learning (α = 0.80), factic, (α = 0.82), deterministic (α = 0.71)
56Robinson (2011) [53]Domestic poverty or without specificationUSA839 survey respondents (431 social workers and 408 school teachers)Feagin (1975) [34], Kluegel and Smith (1982) [36]11 itemsIndividual (α = 0.70), structural (α = 0.72), psycho/medical (α = 0.63), family/morals (α = 0.68)
57Sainz, García-Castro, Jiménez-Moya, and Lobato (2023) [141]Domestic poverty or without specificationMexico523 survey respondentsCozzarelli et al., (2001) [26]11 items, 1–7 (not at all–completely)Internal (α = 0.88), external (α = 0.80)
58Schneider and Castillo (2015) [48]Domestic poverty or without specificationGermany3059 survey respondents (715 from East Germany and 2344 from West Germany)N/A5 items, 1–5 (very often–never)Internal, external
59Segretin, Reyna, and Lipina (2022) [142]Domestic poverty or without specificationArgentina1659 survey respondentsBolitho et al., (2007) [143], Cozzarelli et al., (2001) [26],
Ige and Nekhwevha, (2014) [144], Reyna and Reparaz, (2014) [28], Vázquez et al., (2010) [33], Weiss-Gal et al., (2009) [52]
32 items, 1–5 (not important for poverty–extremely important as a poverty cause)Internal or individualistic (α = 0.90), external or structural (α = 0.90)
60Sigelman (2012) [56]Domestic poverty or without specificationUSA88 primary education studentsN/A9 items, 1–5 (no–yes)Competence (α = 0.79), social attractiveness (α = 0.81), physical attractiveness (α = 0.68)
61Smith and Kluegel (1979) [49]Domestic poverty or without specificationUSA175 respondents aged 18 or olderFeagin (1972) [10]N/AStructural (α = 0.62), individual (α = 0.77)
62Stoeffler, Kauffman, and Joseph (2021) [145]Domestic poverty or without specificationUSA1037 social work educatorsBullock et al., (2003) [29]41 items, 1–7 (strong agreement–strong disagreement)Structural, individual, fatalistic
63Swami, Voracek, Furnham, Robinson and Tran (2023) [146]Domestic poverty or without specificationUK392 respondentsYun and Weaver (2010) [147]21 items, 1–5 (fully disagree–fully agree)Individualistic; discriminatory; structural (reliability across subscales, McDonald’s ω = 0.91)
64Terol-Cantero, Martin-Aragón Gelabert, Costa-López,
Manchón López, and Vázquez-Rodríguez (2023) [148]
Domestic poverty or without specificationSpain278 university studentsCozzarelli et al., (2001) [26], Reyna and Reparaz (2014) [28]23 items, 1–5 (not at all important–extremely important)Cultural (α = 0.69), internal (α = 0.73), external (α = 0.77)
65Toporek and Pope-Davis (2005) [66]Domestic poverty or without specificationUSA158 master’s studentsSmith and Stone (1989) [37]19 items, 1–3 (is not important–is very important)Individualism (α = 77), structuralism/situationalism (α = 72).
66Vázquez and Panadero (2007) [149]Global povertySpain and Nicaragua294 university studentsHarper et al., (1990) [32]25 items, 1–5 (fully disagree–fully agree)Dispositional, situational
67Vázquez and Panadero (2009) [150]Global povertySpain and Nicaragua294 university studentsHarper et al., (1990) [32]25 items, 1–5 (fully disagree–fully agree)Dispositional, situational
68Vázquez, Panadero, Pascual, and Ordoñez (2017) [59]Global povertySpain1092 university studentsHarper (2002) [32], Hine et al., (2005) [151], Vázquez and Panadero (2009) [150]50 items, –2–+2 (fully disagree–fully agree)Fault of the world economic structure; fault of fate, nature, cultural habits, and political misconduct; fault of the developing countries’ population
69Vilchis Carrillo (2022) [152]Domestic poverty or without specificationMexico1403 survey respondentsN/A3 items (dichotomous response)Structural, individualistic, fatalistic
70Waddell, Wright, Mendel, Dys-Steenbergen, and Bahrami (2023) [9]Domestic poverty or without specificationCanada337 undergraduate students (study 1), 203 undergraduate students (study 2)Furnham (1982) [31]9/10 items, 1–7 (strongly disagree–strongly agree)Internal (α = 0.77), external (α = 0.65)
71Weiss-Gal, Gal, Benyamini, Ginzburg, Savaya, and Peled (2009) [52]Domestic poverty or without specificationIsrael811 survey respondents (401 clients and 410 social workers)Weiss-Gal (2005) [51], Weiss-Gal et al., (2003) [153], Bullock et al., (2003) [29]25 items, 1–5 (strongly disagree–strongly agree)Psychological (α = 0.89), motivational (α = 0.87), sociostructural (α = 0.82), fatalistic (α = 0.78)
72Wollie (2009) [154]Domestic poverty or without specificationEthiopia460 high-school and university studentsFeagin (1972) [10], Nasser and Abouchedid (2001) [125], Nasser et al., (2005) [129]39 items, 1–5 (strongly disagree–strongly agree)Individualistic, structural, fatalistic
73Yeboah and Ernest (2012) [155]Domestic poverty or without specificationGhana147 university studentsN/AN/AIndividual, structural, fatalistic
74Yúdica, Bastias, and Etchezahar (2021) [44]Domestic poverty or without specificationArgentina331 secondary school studentsGatica et al., (2017) [156], based on Bullock et al., (2003) [29]32 items; 1–5 (not at all important–very important)Individualistic (α = 0.81); structural (α = 0.80); fatalistic (α = 0.61)
Note. * Reliabilities were not available for all scales.

References

  1. United Nations Development Programme. Human Development Report 2021–2022. Uncertain Times, Unsettled Lives: Shaping Our Future in a Transforming World; United Nations Development Programme: New York, NY, USA, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  2. Berkhout, E.; Galasso, N.; Lawson, M.; Rivero Morales, P.A.; Taneja, A.; Vázquez Pimentel, D.A. The Inequality Virus: Bringing Together a World Torn Apart by Coronavirus through a Fair, Just and Sustainable Economy; OXFAM: Oxford, UK, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  3. Reese, G.; Berthold, A.; Steffens, M.C. We are the world—and they are not: Prototypicality for the world community, legitimacy, and responses to global inequality. Polit. Psychol. 2012, 33, 683–700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Reese, G.; Proch, J.; Cohrs, J.C. Individual differences in responses to global inequality. Anal. Soc. Issues Public Policy 2014, 14, 217–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Reese, G.; Jacob, L. Principles of environmental justice and pro-environmental action: A two-step process model of moral anger and responsibility to act. Environ. Sci. Policy 2015, 51, 88–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Reese, G.; Kohlmann, F. Feeling global, acting ethically: Global identification and fairtrade consumption. J. Soc. Psychol. 2015, 155, 98–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Hine, D.W.; Montiel, C.J. Poverty in developing nations: A cross-cultural attributional analysis. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 29, 943–959. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Piff, P.K.; Wiwad, D.; Robinson, A.R.; Aknin, L.B.; Mercier, B.; Shariff, A. Shifting attributions for poverty motivates opposition to inequality and enhances egalitarianism. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2020, 4, 496–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Waddell, M.W.; Wright, S.C.; Mendel, J.; Dys-Steenbergen, O.; Bahrami, M. From passerby to ally: Testing an intervention to challenge attributions for poverty and generate support for poverty-reducing policies and allyship. Anal. Soc. Issues Public Policy 2023, 23, 334–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Feagin, J.R. Poverty: We still believe that God helps those who help themselves. Psychol. Today 1972, 6, 101–110. [Google Scholar]
  11. Singh, S.; Vasudeva, P.N. A factorial study of the perceived reasons for poverty. Asian J. Psychol. Educ. 1977, 2, 51–56. [Google Scholar]
  12. Feather, N.T. Explanations of poverty in Australian and American samples: The person, society, or fate? Aust. J. Psychol. 1974, 26, 199–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Weiner, B. A Theory of motivation for some classroom experiences. J. Educ. Psychol. 1979, 71, 3–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Weiner, B. An attributional theory of achievement, motivation and emotion. Psychol. Rev. 1985, 92, 548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Zucker, G.S.; Weiner, B. Conservatism and perceptions of poverty: An attributional analysis. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1993, 23, 925–943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Graham, S.; Williams, C. An Attributional Approach to Motivation in School. In Handbook of Motivation at School; Wentzel, C., Wigfield, A., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 2009; pp. 11–33. [Google Scholar]
  17. Wang, H.; Hall, N.C. A systematic review of teachers’ causal attributions: Prevalence, correlates, and consequences. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 2305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Marichal, F.; Quiles, M.N. El estudio del estigma desde la atribución causal. Rev. Psicol. Social 1998, 13, 503–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Menec, V.H.; Perry, R.P. Reactions to stigmas among Canadian students: Testing an attribution-affect-help judgment model. J. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 138, 443–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Lyndon, S. Troubling discourses of poverty in early childhood in the UK. Child. Soc. 2019, 33, 602–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Tscharaktschiew, N.; Rudolph, U. The who and whom of help giving: An attributional model integrating the help giver and the help recipient. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2016, 46, 90–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Osborne, D.; Weiner, B. A latent profile analysis of attributions for poverty: Identifying response patterns underlying people’s willingness to help the poor. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2015, 85, 149–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Weiner, B.; Osborne, D.; Rudolph, U. An attributional analysis of reactions to poverty: The political ideology of the giver and the perceived morality of the receiver. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2011, 15, 199–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Russell, D.W. The Causal Dimension Scale: A measure of how individuals perceive. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1982, 42, 1137–1145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. McAuley, E.; Duncan, T.E.; Russell, D.W. Measuring causal attributions: The revised causal dimension scale (CDSII). Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1992, 18, 566–573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Cozzarelli, C.; Wilkinson, A.V.; Tagler, M.J. Attitudes toward the poor and attributions for poverty. J. Soc. Issues 2001, 57, 207–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Bennett, R.M.; Raiz, L.; Davis, T.S. Development and validation of the Poverty Attributions Survey. J. Soc. Work Educ. 2016, 52, 347–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Reyna, C.; Reparaz, M. Propiedades psicométricas de las escalas de atribuciones sobre las causas de la pobreza y actitudes hacia los pobres. Actual. Psicol. 2014, 28, 55–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Bullock, H.E.; Williams, W.R.; Limbert, W.M. Predicting support for welfare policies: The impact of attributions and beliefs about inequality. J. Poverty 2003, 7, 35–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Bullock, H.E. Attributions for poverty: A comparison of middle class and welfare recipient attitudes. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 29, 2059–2082. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Furnham, A. Why are the poor always with us? Explanations for poverty in Britain. Britain. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 1982, 21, 311–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Harper, D.J.; Wagstaff, G.F.; Newton, J.T.; Harrison, K.R. Lay causal perceptions of third world poverty and the just world. Soc. Behav. Pers. 1990, 18, 235–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Vázquez Cabrera, J.J.; Pascual, I.; Panadero Herrero, S. Developing the “Causes of Poverty in Developing Countries Questionnaire (CPDCQ)” in a Spanish-speaking population. Soc. Behav. Pers. 2010, 38, 1167–1172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Feagin, J.R. Subordinating Poor Persons: Welfare and American Beliefs; Prentice-Hall: Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA, 1975. [Google Scholar]
  35. Kluegel, J.R.; Smith, E.R. Whites’ beliefs about blacks’ opportunity. Am. Sociol. Rev. 1982, 47, 518–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Kluegel, J.R.; Smith, E.R. Beliefs About Inequality: Americans’ Views of What Is and What Ought to Be; Aldine De Gruyter: New York, NY, USA, 1986. [Google Scholar]
  37. Smith, K.B.; Stone, L. Rags, riches, and bootstraps: Beliefs about the causes of wealth and poverty. Sociol. Q. 1989, 30, 93–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Harper, D.J. Poverty and Discourse. In Poverty and Psychology: From Global Perspective to Local Practice; Carr, S.C., Sloan, T.S., Eds.; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 2003; pp. 185–203. [Google Scholar]
  39. Niemelä, M. Attributions for poverty in Finland: A non-generic approach. Res. Finn. Soc. 2011, 4, 17–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Weiner, B. Wither attribution theory? J. Organ. Behav. 2019, 40, 603–604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Çoban, I.A.; Şahin, F.; Uluocak, G.P.; Büyüköztürk, Ş. Turkish adaptation of the Chinese Perceived Causes of Poverty Scale. Eur. J. Soc. Sci. 2010, 15, 564–572. [Google Scholar]
  42. Dakduk, S.; González, M.; Malavé, J. Percepciones acerca de los pobres y la pobreza: Una revisión. Rev. Latinoam. Psicol. 2010, 42, 413–425. [Google Scholar]
  43. Shek, D.T. Chinese people’s explanations of poverty: The Perceived Causes of Poverty Scale. Res. Soc. Work Pract. 2003, 13, 622–640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Yúdica, L.; Bastias, F.; Etchezahar, E. Poverty attributions and emotions associated with willingness to help and government aid. Psihol. Teme 2021, 30, 509–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Henrich, J.; Heine, S.J.; Norenzayan, A. The weirdest people in the world? Behav. Brain Sci. 2010, 33, 61–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Lepianka, D.; Van Oorschot, W.; Gelissen, J. Popular explanations of poverty: A critical discussion of empirical research. J. Soc. Policy 2009, 38, 421–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Babjaková, J.; Džuka, J.; Gresty, J. Perceived causes of poverty and subjective aspirations of the poor: A literature review. Ceskoslovenska Psychol. 2019, 63, 325–336. [Google Scholar]
  48. Schneider, S.M.; Castillo, J.C. Poverty attributions and the perceived justice of income inequality: A comparison of East and West Germany. Soc. Psychol. Q. 2015, 78, 263–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Smith, E.R.; Kluegel, J.R. Causal attributions outside the laboratory: Explaining poverty. In Proceedings of the American Sociological Association Session on Attribution, Cognitive, and Related Processes, Boston, MA, USA, 27–31 August 1979; pp. 1–15. [Google Scholar]
  50. Van Oorschot, W.; Halman, L. Blame or fate, individual or social? An international comparison of popular explanations of poverty. Eur. Soc. 2000, 2, 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Weiss-Gal, I. Is there a global common core to social work? A cross-national comparative study of BSW graduate students. Soc. Work 2005, 50, 101–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Weiss-Gal, I.; Gal, J.; Benyamini, Y.; Ginzburg, K.; Savaya, R.; Peled, E. Social workers’ and service users’ causal attributions for poverty. Soc. Work. 2009, 54, 125–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Robinson, G. The contradictions of caring: Social workers, teachers, and attributions for poverty and welfare reform. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 41, 2374–2404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Mickelson, K.D.; Hazlett, E. “Why me?”: Low-income women’s poverty attributions, mental health, and social class perceptions. Sex Roles 2014, 71, 319–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Norcia, M.; Rissotto, A. Causal attributions for poverty in Italy: What do people think about impoverishment? OIDA Int. J. Sustain. Dev. 2015, 8, 59–70. [Google Scholar]
  56. Sigelman, C.K. Rich man, poor man: Developmental differences in attributions and perceptions. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2012, 113, 415–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Da Costa, L.P.; Dias, J.G. Perceptions of poverty attributions in Europe: A multilevel mixture model approach. Qual. Quant. 2013, 48, 1409–1419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Habibov, N.; Cheung, A.; Auchynnikava, A.; Fan, L. Explaining support for structural attribution of poverty in post-communist countries: Multilevel analysis of repeated cross-sectional data. J. Soc. Soc. Welf. 2017, 44, 173–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Vázquez Cabrera, J.J.; Panadero Herrero, S.; Pascual, I.; Ordoñez, X.G. Causal attributions of poverty in less developed countries: Comparing among undergraduates from nations with different development levels. Interam. J. Psychol. 2017, 51, 29–43. [Google Scholar]
  60. Baute, S.; Pellegata, A. Multi-level blame attribution and public support for EU welfare policies. West Eur. Polit. 2023, 46, 1369–1395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Arnett, J.J. The neglected 95%: Why American psychology needs to become less American. Am. Psychol. 2008, 63, 602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Thalmayer, A.G.; Toscanelli, C.; Arnett, J.J. The neglected 95% revisited: Is American psychology becoming less American? Am. Psychol. 2021, 76, 116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Smith, K.B. I made it because of me: Beliefs about the causes of wealth and poverty. Sociol. Spectr. 1985, 5, 255–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Wilson, G. Toward a revised framework for examining beliefs about the causes of poverty. Sociol. Q. 1996, 37, 413–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Hunt, M. The individual, society, or both? A comparison of Black, Latino and White beliefs about the causes of poverty. Soc. Forces 1996, 75, 293–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Toporek, R.L.; Pope-Davis, D.B. Exploring the relationships between multicultural training, racial attitudes, and attributions of poverty among graduate counselling trainees. Cult. Divers. Ethn. Min. Psychol. 2005, 11, 259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Bastias, F.; Cañadas, B.; Figueroa, M.C.; Sosa, V.; Moya, M.J. Explanations about poverty origin according to professional training area. J. Educ. Psychol. Prop. Y Repres. 2019, 7, 121–133. [Google Scholar]
  68. Bullock, H.E. From the front lines of welfare reform: An analysis of social worker and welfare recipient attitudes. J. Soc. Psychol. 2004, 144, 571–590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Bullock, H.E.; Limbert, W.M. Scaling the socioeconomic ladder: Low-income women’s perceptions. J. Soc. Issues 2003, 59, 693–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Delavega, E.; Kindle, P.A.; Peterson, S.; Schwartz, C. The blame index: Exploring the change in social work students’ perceptions of poverty. J. Soc. Work Educ. 2017, 53, 664–675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Toikko, T.; Rantanen, T. How does the welfare state model influence social political attitudes? An analysis of citizens’ concrete and abstract attitudes toward poverty. J. Int. Comp. Soc. Policy 2017, 33, 201–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Weiner, B. Judgments of Responsibility: A Foundation for a Theory of Social Conduct; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  73. Heider, F. The Psychology of Interpersonal Relations; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1958. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Castillo, J.C.; Rivera-Gutiérrez, M. Dimensiones comunes a las atribuciones de pobreza y riqueza. Psykhe 2018, 27, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. González, R.; Lay, S. Sense of Responsibility and Empathy: Bridging the Gap between Attributions and Helping Behaviours. In Intergroup Helping; van Leeuwen, E., Zagefka, H., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2017; pp. 331–347. [Google Scholar]
  76. Alkire, S.; Roche, J.M.; Ballon, P.; Foster, J.; Santos, M.E.; Seth, S. Multidimensional Poverty Measurement and Analysis; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  77. Sen, A. Sobre conceptos y medidas de pobreza. Comer. Exter. 1992, 42, 310–322. [Google Scholar]
  78. Shanahan, M.J.; Cole, S.W.; Ravi, S.; Chumbley, J.; Xu, W.; Potente, C.; Levitt, B.; Bodeleta, J.; Aiellod, A.; Gaydoshmiy, L.; et al. Socioeconomic inequalities in molecular risk for chronic diseases observed in young adulthood. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2022, 119, e2103088119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Spicker, P. Definiciones de Pobreza: Doce Grupos de Significados. In Pobreza: Un Glosario Internacional; Spicker, P., Álvarez, S., Gordon, D., Eds.; Consejo Latinoamericano de Ciencias Sociales CLACSO: Buenos Aires, Argentina, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  80. Bastias, F.; Barreiro, A. Who is poor? Analysis of social representations in an Argentine sample. Psico-USF 2023, 28, 67–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Lee, B.A.; Jones, S.H.; Lewis, D.W. Public beliefs about the causes of homelessness. Soc. Forces 1990, 69, 253–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Lepianka, D. Are the Poor to be Blamed or Pitied? A Comparative Study of Popular Poverty Attributions in Europe; Tilburg University: Tilburg, The Netherlands, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  83. Kangas, O. Self-interest and the common good: The impact of norms, selfishness and context in social policy opinions. J. Socio-Econ. 1997, 26, 475–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Peter, N.; Landmann, H.; Cañadas, B.; Bastias, F.; Rohmann, A. What are the causes of global inequality? An exploration of global inequality attributions in Germany and Argentina. Community Psychol. Glob. Perspect. 2024, 10, 49–64. [Google Scholar]
  85. Berry, A.; Serieux, J. All about the giants: Probing the influences on world growth and income inequality at the end of the 20th century. CESifo Econ. Stud. 2004, 50, 133–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Milanovic, B. Capitalism, Alone: The Future of the System That Rules the World; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  87. Piketty, T. El Capital en el Siglo XXI; Fondo de Cultura Económica: Mexico City, Mexico, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  88. Hickel, J. The Divide: A Brief Guide to Global Inequality and its Solutions; William Heinemann: London, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  89. Abouchedid, K.; Nasser, R. Attributions of responsibility for poverty among Lebanese and Portuguese university students: A cross-cultural comparison. Soc. Behav. Pers. Int. J. 2002, 30, 25–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Alcañiz-Colomer, J.; Moya, M.; Valor-Segura, I. Not all poor are equal: The perpetuation of poverty through blaming those who have been poor all their lives. Curr. Psychol. 2023, 42, 26928–26944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  91. Bai, J.; Xu, B.-X.; Yang, S.-L.; Guo, Y.-Y. Why are higher-class individuals less supportive of redistribution? The mediating role of attributions for rich-poor gap. Curr. Psychol. 2023, 42, 16883–16893. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Li, J. Study on the Tendency of Attribution on the Gap between the Rich and the Poor in Different Social Classes; World Publishing Corporation: Guangzhou, China, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  93. Weiss-Gal, I.; Gal, J. Poverty in the eyes of the beholder: Social workers compared to other middle-class professionals. British J. Soc. W. 2007, 37, 893–908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Bergmann, B.A.; Todd, N.R. Religious and spiritual beliefs uniquely predict poverty attributions. Soc. Justice Res. 2019, 32, 459–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Bobbio, A.; Canova, L.; Manganelli, A.M. Conservative ideology, economic conservatism, and causal attributions for poverty and wealth. Curr. Psychol. 2010, 29, 222–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Bradley, C.; Cole, D.J. Causal attributions and the significance of self-efficacy in predicting solutions to poverty. Sociol. Focus 2002, 35, 381–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Canto, J.M.; Perles, F.; San Martín, J. Racismo, dominancia social y atribuciones causales de la pobreza de los inmigrantes magrebíes. Bol. Psicol. 2012, 104, 73–86. [Google Scholar]
  98. Betancor, V.; Quiles, M.N.; Morera, D.; Rodríguez, R.; Rodríguez, A.; Delgado, N.; Acosta, V. Creencias sobre las causas de la pobreza y su influencia sobre el prejuicio hacia los inmigrantes. R. Psicol. Soc. Apl. 2002, 12, 5–20. [Google Scholar]
  99. Carr, S.C.; Taef, H.; Ribeiro, R.D.M.S.; MacLachlan, M. Attributions for “Third World” poverty: Contextual factors in Australia and Brazil. Psychol. Dev. Soc. 1998, 10, 103–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Carr, S.C.; Maclachlan, M. Actors, observers, and attributions for Third World poverty: Contrasting perspectives from Malawi and Australia. J. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 138, 189–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Cheng, Q.; Ngok, K. Does the Dibao Program improve citizens’ life satisfaction in China? Perceptions of pathways of poverty attribution and income inequality. Appl. Res. Qual. Life 2023, 18, 975–995. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Cojanu, S.; Stroe, C. Causes of Poverty—What do the poor think? Poverty attribution and its behavioural effects. Actas LUMEN 2017, 1, 186–197. [Google Scholar]
  103. Davidai, S. Why do Americans believe in economic mobility? Economic inequality, external attributions of wealth and poverty, and the belief in economic mobility. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2018, 79, 138–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Engler, J.N.; Strassle, C.G.; Steck, L.W. The impact of a poverty simulation on educators’ attributions and behaviors. Clear. House J. Educ. Strateg. Issues Ideas 2019, 92, 174–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Frei, R.; Castillo, J.C.; Herrera, R.; Suárez, J.I. ¿Fruto del esfuerzo? Los cambios en las atribuciones sobre pobreza y riqueza en Chile entre 1996 y 2015. Lat. Am. Res. Rev. 2020, 55, 477–495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Gatica, L.; Navarro-Lashayas, M.A. Ideología política, actitudes hacia la inmigración y atribuciones causales sobre la pobreza en una muestra universitaria. Rev. Serv. Soc. 2019, 69, 87–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Generalao, F.G. Causal attributions for poverty and their correlates. Philipp. J. Psychol. 2005, 38, 39–51. [Google Scholar]
  108. Gonzalez, A.M.; Macchia, L.; Whillans, A.V. The developmental origins and behavioral consequences of attributions for inequality. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2022, 101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Hussak, L.J.; Cimpian, A. An early-emerging explanatory heuristic promotes support for the status quo. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2015, 109, 739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  110. Griffin, W.E.; Oheneba-Sakyi, Y. Sociodemographic and political correlates of university students’ causal attributions for poverty. Psychol. Rep. 1993, 73, 795–800. [Google Scholar]
  111. Halik, M.; Malek, M.D.A.; Bahari, F.; Matshah, N.; Webley, P. Attribution of poverty among Malaysian students in the United Kingdom. Southeast Asia Psychol. J. 2012, 1, 22–30. [Google Scholar]
  112. Heaven, P.C.L. Economic locus of control beliefs and lay attributions of poverty. Aust. J. Psychol. 1989, 41, 315–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Hill, K.M.; Toft, J.E.; Garrett, K.J.; Ferguson, S.M.; Kuechler, C.F. Assessing clinical MSW students’ attitudes, attributions, and responses to poverty. J. Poverty 2016, 20, 396–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Husz, I.; Kopasz, M.; Medgyesi, M. Social workers’ causal attributions for poverty: Does the level of spatial concentration of disadvantages matter? Soc. Indic. Res. 2022, 162, 1069–1091. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Ige, K.D.; Nekhwevha, F.H. Poverty attribution in the developing world: A critical discussion on aspects of split consciousness among low income urban slum dwellers in Lagos. J. Soc. Sci. 2012, 33, 213–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Kafetsios, K.; Kateri, E.; Sloam, J.; Flanagan, C.; Hayward, B.; Kalogeraki, S.; Kousis, M. Youths’ Cultural Orientations, Attributions to Inequality and Political Engagement Attitudes and Intentions. In Youth Political Participation in Greece: A Multiple Methods Perspective; Kalogeraki, S., Kousis, M., Eds.; Palgrave Macmillan: London, UK, 2022; pp. 99–118. [Google Scholar]
  117. Kitchens, S.; Ricks, L.; Hannor-Walker, T. Self-efficacy as it relates to attributions and attitudes towards poverty among school counselors-in-training. Ga. Sch. Couns. Assoc. J. 2020, 27, 35–50. [Google Scholar]
  118. Landmane, D.; Reņģe, V. Attributions for poverty, attitudes toward the poor and identification with the poor among social workers and poor people. Balt. J. Psychol. 2010, 11, 37–50. [Google Scholar]
  119. Lee, H.; Park, C.H.K.; Rhee, S.J.; Kim, B.; Lee, S.S.; Ha, K.; Baik, C.J.; Ahn, Y.M. The influence of poverty attribution on attitudes toward suicide and suicidal thought: A cross-national comparison between South Korean, Japanese, and American populations. Compr. Psych. 2021, 109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  120. Ljubotina, O.D.; Ljubotina, D. Attributions of poverty among social work and non-social work students in Croatia. Croat. Med. J. 2007, 48, 741. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  121. McWha, I.; Carr, S.C. Images of poverty and attributions for poverty: Does higher education moderate the linkage? J. Philanthr. Market. 2009, 14, 101–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Murry, V.M.; Brody, G.H.; Brown, A.; Wisenbaker, J.; Cutrona, C.E.; Simons, R.L. Linking employment status, maternal psychological well-being, parenting, and children’s attributions about poverty in families receiving government assistance. Fam. Relat. 2002, 51, 112–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Conger, R.D. Family and Community Health Study; Unpublished manuscript; University of Iowa: Des Moines, IA, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  124. Nasser, R. Does subjective class predict the causal attribution for poverty? J. Soc. Sci. 2007, 3, 197–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Nasser, R.; Abouchedid, K. Causal attribution of poverty among Lebanese university students. Curr. Res. Soc. Psychol. 2001, 6, 205–220. [Google Scholar]
  126. Morçöl, G. Lay explanations for poverty in Turkey and their determinants. J. Soc. Psychol. 1997, 137, 728–738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Williamson, J. Beliefs about the motivation of poor persons and attitudes toward poverty policy. Soc. Probl. 1974, 21, 634–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Nasser, R.; Abouchedid, K. Locus of control and the attribution for poverty: Comparing Lebanese and South African university students. Soc. Behav. Personal. Int. J. 2006, 34, 777–796. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Nasser, R.; Singhal, S.; Abouchedid, K. Causal attributions for poverty among Indian youth. Curr. Res. Soc. Psychol. 2005, 11, 1–13. [Google Scholar]
  130. Nelson, T.E.; Joselus, D. Cultural attributions for racial inequality. Pol. Gr. Id. 2023, 11, 876–898. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Peffley, M.; Hurwitz, J.; Mondak, J. Racial attributions in the justice system and support for punitive crime policies. American Pol. Res. 2017, 45, 1032–1058. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  132. Saunders, P. Stability and change in community perceptions of poverty: Evidence from Australia. J. Pov. 2003, 7, 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Norcia, M.; Castellani, A.; Rissotto, A. The process of causal attribution of poverty: Preliminary results of a survey in Italy. OIDA Int. J. Sustain. Dev. 2010, 1, 85–97. [Google Scholar]
  134. Norcia, M.; Rissotto, A. How does poverty work? Representations and causal attributions for poverty and wealth. Int. J. Soc. Sci. Humanit. Stud. 2011, 3, 259–269. [Google Scholar]
  135. Özpinar, Ş.; Akdede, S.H. Determinants of the attribution of poverty in Turkey: An empirical analysis. Soc. Indic. Res. 2022, 164, 949–967. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  136. Pandey, J.; Sinha, Y.; Prakash, A.; Tripathi, R.C. Right–left political ideologies and attribution of the causes of poverty. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 1982, 12, 327–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Sinha, Y.; Jain, U.; Pandey, J. Attribution of causality to poverty. J. Soc. Econ. Studies 1980, 8, 349–359. [Google Scholar]
  138. Reutter, L.I.; Veenstra, G.; Stewart, M.J.; Raphael, D.; Love, R.; Makwarimba, E.; Mcmurray, S. Public attributions for poverty in Canada. Can. Rev. Sociol./Rev. Can. Sociol. 2006, 43, 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  139. Reyna, C.; Acosta, C.; Saavedra, B.A.; Correa, P.S. Atribuciones causales de la pobreza en ciudadano/as cordobeses. Rev. Argent. Cienc. Comport. 2018, 10, 193–194. [Google Scholar]
  140. Ríos-Rodríguez, M.L.; Moreno-Jiménez, P.; Vallejo Martín, M. Atribuciones de la pobreza: Efectos en la comunidad. Psicumex 2022, 12, 1–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  141. Sainz, M.; García-Castro, J.D.; Jiménez-Moya, G.; Lobato, R.M. How do people understand the causes of poverty and wealth? A revised structural dimensionality of the attributions about poverty and wealth scales. J. Poverty Soc. Justice 2023, 31, 81–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  142. Segretin, M.S.; Reyna, C.; Lipina, S.J. Atribuciones sobre las causas de la pobreza general e infantil en Argentina. Interdisciplinaria 2022, 39, 293–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  143. Bolitho, F.H.; Carr, S.C.; Fletcher, R.B. Public thinking about poverty: Why it matters and how to measure it. Int. J. Nonprofit Vol. Sec. Mark. 2007, 12, 13–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  144. Ige, K.D.; Nekhwevha, F.H. Causal attributions for poverty among low income communities of Badia, Nigeria. J. Soc. Sci. 2014, 38, 205–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  145. Stoeffler, S.W.; Kauffman, S.E.; Joseph, R. Causal attributions of poverty among social work faculty: A regression analysis. Soc. Work Educ. 2021, 42, 1198–1214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  146. Swami, V.; Voracek, M.; Furnham, A.; Robinson, C.; Tran, U.S. Support for weight-related anti-discrimination laws and policies: Modelling the role of attitudes toward poverty alongside weight stigma, causal attributions about weight, and prejudice. Body Image 2023, 45, 391–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  147. Yun, S.H.; Weaver, R.D. Development and validation of a short form of the attitude toward poverty scale. Adv. Soc. Work 2010, 11, 174–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  148. Terol-Cantero, M.C.; Martín-Aragón Gelabert, M.; Costa-López, B.; Manchón López, J.; Vázquez-Rodríguez, C. Causal attribution for poverty in young people: Sociodemographic characteristics, religious and political beliefs. Soc. Sci. 2023, 12, 308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  149. Vázquez Cabrera, J.J.; Panadero Herrero, S. Pobreza en los estados menos desarrollados y sus atribuciones de causalidad: Estudio transcultural en estados latinoamericanos con diferentes niveles de desarrollo. Santiago Rev. Univ. Oriente 2007, 115, 364–379. [Google Scholar]
  150. Vázquez Cabrera, J.J.; Panadero Herrero, S. Atribuciones causales de la pobreza en los países menos desarrollados. Perfiles Latinoam. 2009, 17, 125–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  151. Hine, D.W.; Montiel, C.J.; Cooksey, R.W.; Lewko, J.H. Mental models of poverty in developing nations: A causal mapping analysis using a Canada-Philippines contrast. J. Cross-Cultural Psychol. 2005, 36, 283–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  152. Vilchis Carrillo, D.E. Pobreza, desigualdad y religión: Creencias religiosas y atribuciones causales de la pobreza en México. Rev. Temas Sociol. 2022, 30, 253–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  153. Weiss-Gal, I.; Gal, J. Israel. In Professional Ideologies and Preferences in Social Work: A Global Study; Weiss-Gal, I., Gal, J., Dixon, J., Eds.; Praeger: Westport, CT, USA, 2003; pp. 141–156. [Google Scholar]
  154. Wollie, C.W. Causal attributions for poverty among youths in Bahir Dar, Amhara region, Ethiopia. J. Soc. Evol. Cult. Psychol. 2009, 3, 251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  155. Yeboah, S.A.; Ernest, K. Attributions for poverty: A survey of student’s perception. Int. Rev. Manag. Market. 2012, 2, 83–91. [Google Scholar]
  156. Gatica, L.; Martini, J.P.; Dreizik, M.; Imhoff, D. Psychosocial and psycho-political predictors of social inequality justification. R. Psicol. 2017, 35, 279–310. [Google Scholar]
Figure 2. PRISMA flow diagram of the systematic literature search.
Figure 2. PRISMA flow diagram of the systematic literature search.
Behavsci 14 00186 g002
Figure 3. Occurrence of different theoretical perspectives among the reviewed articles [10,26,29,30,31,32,34,35,36,37,38].
Figure 3. Occurrence of different theoretical perspectives among the reviewed articles [10,26,29,30,31,32,34,35,36,37,38].
Behavsci 14 00186 g003
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Bastias, F.; Peter, N.; Goldstein, A.; Sánchez-Montañez, S.; Rohmann, A.; Landmann, H. Measuring Attributions 50 Years on: From within-Country Poverty to Global Inequality. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 186. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14030186

AMA Style

Bastias F, Peter N, Goldstein A, Sánchez-Montañez S, Rohmann A, Landmann H. Measuring Attributions 50 Years on: From within-Country Poverty to Global Inequality. Behavioral Sciences. 2024; 14(3):186. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14030186

Chicago/Turabian Style

Bastias, Franco, Nadja Peter, Aristobulo Goldstein, Santiago Sánchez-Montañez, Anette Rohmann, and Helen Landmann. 2024. "Measuring Attributions 50 Years on: From within-Country Poverty to Global Inequality" Behavioral Sciences 14, no. 3: 186. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14030186

APA Style

Bastias, F., Peter, N., Goldstein, A., Sánchez-Montañez, S., Rohmann, A., & Landmann, H. (2024). Measuring Attributions 50 Years on: From within-Country Poverty to Global Inequality. Behavioral Sciences, 14(3), 186. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14030186

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop