The Consequences of Anthropomorphic and Teleological Beliefs in a Global Pandemic
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Current Study
2.1. Validating a Short Form of the Teleological Beliefs Scale
2.2. Consequences of Anthropomorphic and Teleological Beliefs
3. Methods
Participants
4. Materials
4.1. Teleological and Causal Beliefs
4.1.1. Teleological Beliefs Scale
4.1.2. Beliefs about COVID-19
4.2. Predictors of Teleological Beliefs
4.2.1. Anthropomorphism
4.2.2. Individual Differences in Anthropomorphism Questionnaire
4.2.3. Anthropomorphism Questionnaire
4.2.4. Religious Belief
4.2.5. Inhibition of Intuitions
4.3. Consequences for the Pandemic
4.3.1. Threat
4.3.2. Uncertainty
4.3.3. Behavioural Change
5. Procedure
6. Results
6.1. Scale Validation
6.2. Consequences of Anthropomorphic and Teleological Beliefs
7. Discussion
7.1. Foundations of Teleology and Validation of the s-TBS
7.2. Anthropomorphism, Teleology, and Perceptions of the Pandemic
8. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- World Health Organization. Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19). 2020. Available online: https://www.who.int/emergencies/diseases/novel-coronavirus-2019 (accessed on 2 February 2021).
- Dennett, D.C. From Bacteria to Bach and Back; W. W. Norton & Company: New York, NY, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Hempel, C.G.; Oppenheim, P. Studies in the logic of explanation. Philos. Sci. 1948, 15, 135–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bloom, P. Intention, history, and artifact concepts. Cognition 1996, 60, 1–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kelemen, D. Beliefs about purpose: On the origins of teleological thought. In The Descent of Mind: Psychological Perspectives on Hominid Evolution; Corballis, M.C., Lea, S.E.G., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1999; pp. 278–310. [Google Scholar]
- Wright, L. Teleological Explanations; University of California Press: Berkeley, CA, USA, 1976. [Google Scholar]
- Lombrozo, T.; Carey, S. Functional explanation and the function of explanation. Cognition 2006, 99, 167–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brumby, M.N. Misconceptions about the concept of natural selection by medical biology students. Sci. Educ. 1984, 68, 493–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roberts, A.J.; Wastell, C.A.; Polito, V. Teleology and the intentions of supernatural agents. Conscious. Cogn. 2020, 80, 102905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kelemen, D. The scope of teleological thinking in preschool children. Cognition 1999, 70, 241–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Greif, M.L.; Kemler Nelson, D.G.; Keil, F.C.; Gutierrez, F. What do children want to know about animals and artifacts? Domain-specific requests for information. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 17, 455–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keil, F.C. The origins of an autonomous biology. In Modularity and Constraints in Language and Cognition; Gunnar, M.R., Maratsos, M., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1992; pp. 103–137. [Google Scholar]
- Kelemen, D.; Rottman, J.; Seston, R. Professional physical scientists display tenacious teleological tendencies: Purpose-based reasoning as a cognitive default. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2013, 142, 1074–1083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Willard, A.K.; Norenzayan, A. Cognitive biases explain religious belief, paranormal belief, and belief in life’s purpose. Cognition 2013, 129, 379–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kelemen, D.; Rosset, E. The Human function compunction: Teleological explanation in adults. Cognition 2009, 111, 138–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mills, R.; Frowley, J. Promiscuous Teleology and the effect of Locus of Control. Ir. J. Psychol. 2014, 35, 121–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Casler, K.; Kelemen, D. Developmental continuity in teleo-functional explanation: Reasoning about nature among Romanian Romani adults. J. Cogn. Dev. 2008, 9, 340–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lombrozo, T.; Kelemen, D.; Zaitchik, D. Inferring design: Evidence for a preference for teleological explanations in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Psychol. Sci. 2007, 18, 999–1007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Roberts, A.J.; Handley, S.J.; Polito, V. The design stance, intentional stance, and teleological beliefs about biological and non-biological natural entites. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2021, 120, 1720–1748. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zemla, J.C.; Steiner, S.M.; Sloman, S. Analytical Thinking Predicts Less Teleological Reasoning and Religious Belief. In Proceedings of the 38th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Philadelphia, PA, USA, 10–13 August 2012; pp. 1217–1222. [Google Scholar]
- Dennett, D.C. The Intentional Stance; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Willard, A.K.; Cingl, L.; Norenzayan, A. Cognitive biases and religious belief: A path model replication in the Czech Republic and Slovakia with a focus on anthropomorphism. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 2020, 11, 97–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Epley, N.; Waytz, A.; Cacioppo, J.T. On seeing human: A three-factor theory of anthropomorphism. Psychol. Rev. 2007, 114, 864–886. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Waytz, A.; Morewedge, C.K.; Epley, N.; Monteleone, G.; Gao, J.; Cacioppo, J.T. Making sense by making sentient: Effectance motivation increases anthropomorphism. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2010, 99, 410–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Waytz, A.; Cacioppo, J.T.; Epley, N. Who sees human? The stability and importance of individual differences in anthropomorphism. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2010, 5, 219–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Neave, N.; Jackson, R.; Saxton, T.; Hönekopp, J. The influence of anthropomorphic tendencies on human hoarding behaviours. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2015, 72, 214–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Landau, M.J.; Kay, A.C.; Whitson, J.A. Compensatory control and the appeal of a structured world. Psychol. Bull. 2015, 141, 694–722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Laurin, K.; Kay, A.C.; Moscovitch, D.A. On the belief in God: Towards an understanding of the emotional substrates of compensatory control. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 44, 1559–1562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ma, A.; Kay, A.C. Compensatory control and ambiguity intolerance. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 2017, 140, 46–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Erdfelder, E.; FAul, F.; Buchner, A.; Lang, A.G. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 2009, 41, 1149–1160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Lang, A.G.; Buchner, A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 2007, 39, 175–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Huber, S.; Huber, O.W. The centrality of religiosity scale (CRS). Religions 2012, 3, 710–724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frederick, S. Cognitive reflection and decision making. J. Econ. Perspect. 2005, 19, 25–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thomson, K.S.; Oppenheimer, D.M. Investigating an alternate form of the cognitive reflection test. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2016, 11, 99–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haigh, M. Has the standard cognitive reflection test become a victim of its own success? Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 2016, 12, 145–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows; Version 27.0; IBM Corp.: Armonk, NY, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software: Release 17; StataCorp LLC.: College Station, TX, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Banerjee, K.; Bloom, P. Why did this happen to me? Religious believers’ and non-believers’ teleological reasoning about life events. Cognition 2014, 133, 277–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heywood, B.T.; Bering, J.M. “Meant to be”: How religious beliefs and cultural religiosity affect the implicit bias to think teleologically. Relig. Brain Behav. 2014, 4, 183–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kelemen, D.; Diyanni, C. Intuitions about origins: Purpose and intelligent design in children’s reasoning about nature. J. Cogn. Dev. 2005, 6, 3–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kay, A.C.; Gaucher, D.; Napier, J.L.; Callan, M.J.; Laurin, K. God and the government: Testing a compensatory control mechanism for the support of external systems. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 95, 18–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kay, A.C.; Whitson, J.A.; Gaucher, D.; Galinsky, A.D. Compensatory control: Achieving order through the mind, our institutions, and the heavens. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2009, 18, 264–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Teleological Test Statements | False Control Statements | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Beta | SE | t | p | 95%CI | Beta | SE | t | p | 95%CI | |
IDAQ | −0.24 | <0.01 | −4.36 | <0.001 | −0.34, −0.13 | 0.02 | <0.01 | 0.39 | 0.699 | −0.10, 0.14 |
r-CRS | −0.11 | <0.01 | −1.94 | 0.053 | −0.22, <0.01 | 0.98 | <0.01 | 0.03 | 0.980 | −0.12, 0.12 |
CRT | 0.34 | 0.01 | 6.10 | <0.001 | 0.23, 0.44 | 0.09 | <0.01 | 1.68 | 0.094 | −0.02, 0.22 |
Teleological Test Statements | False Control Statements | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Beta | SE | t | p | 95%CI | Beta | SE | t | p | 95%CI | |
AQ | −0.22 | <0.01 | −3.84 | <0.0001 | −0.33, −0.11 | <0.01 | <0.01 | 0.01 | 0.990 | −0.12, 0.12 |
r-CRS | −0.09 | <0.01 | −1.60 | 0.110 | −0.20, 0.02 | 0.01 | <0.01 | 0.10 | 0.923 | −0.12, 0.13 |
CRT | 0.32 | 0.01 | 5.93 | <0.0001 | −0.33, −0.11 | 0.10 | <0.01 | 1.68 | 0.095 | −0.02, 0.22 |
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. IDAQ | - | ||||||||
- | |||||||||
2. AQ | 0.551 *** | - | |||||||
[0.47, 0.63] | - | ||||||||
3. r-CRS | 0.200 *** | 0.297 *** | - | ||||||
[0.09, 0.31] | [0.19, 0.40] | - | |||||||
4. CRT | −0.009 | −0.098 | −0.061 | - | |||||
[−0.12, 0.11] | [−0.21, 0.02] | [−0.18, 0.06] | - | ||||||
5. s-TBS | 0.260 *** | 0.274 *** | 0.173 ** | −0.345 *** | - | ||||
[0.15, 0.37] | [0.16, 0.38] | [0.06, 0.28] | [−0.44, −0.24] | - | |||||
6. COVID-T | 0.135 * | 0.186 *** | 0.102 | −0.191 ** | 0.291 *** | - | |||
[0.02, 0.25] | [0.07, 0.30] | [−0.01, 0.22] | [−0.30, −0.08] | [0.18, 0.39] | - | ||||
7. Uncertainty | −0.160 ** | −0.193 *** | −0.195 *** | 0.213 *** | -0.231 *** | −0.018 | - | ||
[−0.27, −0.05] | [−0.30, −0.08] | [−0.30, −0.08] | [0.10, 0.32] | [−0.34, −0.12] | [−0.13, 0.10] | - | |||
8. Threat | −0.166 ** | −0.157 ** | −0.181 ** | 0.025 | −0.164 ** | 0.043 | 0.488 *** | - | |
[−0.28, −0.05] | [−0.27, −0.04] | [−0.29, −0.07] | [−0.09, 0.14] | [−0.28, −0.05] | [−0.07, 0.16] | [0.40, 0.57] | - | ||
9. Behaviour | 0.125 * | 0.253 *** | 0.208 *** | −0.170 ** | 0.219 *** | 0.304 *** | −0.034 | 0.139 * | - |
[0.01, 0.24] | [0.14, 0.36] | [0.10, 0.32] | [−0.28, −0.06] | [0.11, 0.33] | [0.20, 0.41] | [−0.15, 0.08] | [0.02, 0.25] | - |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Roberts, A.J.; Handley, S.; Polito, V. The Consequences of Anthropomorphic and Teleological Beliefs in a Global Pandemic. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 146. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14020146
Roberts AJ, Handley S, Polito V. The Consequences of Anthropomorphic and Teleological Beliefs in a Global Pandemic. Behavioral Sciences. 2024; 14(2):146. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14020146
Chicago/Turabian StyleRoberts, Andrew J., Simon Handley, and Vince Polito. 2024. "The Consequences of Anthropomorphic and Teleological Beliefs in a Global Pandemic" Behavioral Sciences 14, no. 2: 146. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14020146
APA StyleRoberts, A. J., Handley, S., & Polito, V. (2024). The Consequences of Anthropomorphic and Teleological Beliefs in a Global Pandemic. Behavioral Sciences, 14(2), 146. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14020146