Next Article in Journal
Influence of an Esports Program on Problematic Gaming in Children with Autistic Spectrum Disorder: A Pilot Study
Previous Article in Journal
Trust in Institutions, Not in Political Leaders, Determines Compliance in COVID-19 Prevention Measures within Societies across the Globe
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Effects of Early Weaning Associated with Alimentary Stress on Emotional and Feeding Behavior of Female Adult Wistar Rats

by
Víctor Isaac Meléndez Díaz
1,*,
Julliet Araújo de Souza
1 and
Sandra Lopes de Sousa
1,2
1
Graduate Program of Neuropsychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Federal University of Pernambuco, Recife 50740-600, Brazil
2
Graduate Program of Nutrition, Federal University of Pernambuco, Recife 50740-600, Brazil
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2022, 12(6), 171; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12060171
Submission received: 12 January 2022 / Revised: 3 February 2022 / Accepted: 9 February 2022 / Published: 31 May 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Experimental and Clinical Neurosciences)

Abstract

:
Maternal lactation proves crucial for mammals’ nutrition during their early development, influencing the development of adult physiological mechanisms. Its premature termination has been associated with several disorders, but these have been primarily documented in males, when they are most prevalent in women. Therefore, we subjected adult female Wistar rats to Early Weaning through maternal separation at age 15 days to acute alimentary stress in the form of visual and olfactory exposition to a cafeteria diet sans consumption for 22 days. We measured standard diet intake and water intake daily and cafeteria diet intake every 7 days. Additionally, we evaluated anxiety using the elevated plus maze and measured body weight in similar intervals. Results showed less consumption of the cafeteria diet among Early Weaning rats on day 2 and more time spent in the maze’s central area by the Early Weaning rats during the basal evaluation and in the maze’s open arms by control rats on day 7 when compared to the same group’s basal time. No other significant differences were found. These results show the importance of determining the impact that female steroidal gonadal hormones such as estradiol have upon feeding behavior and anxiety and determining to what degree these parameters are influenced by hormonal action.

1. Introduction

Knowledge of breastfeeding’s importance has evolved considerably over the last few decades. Initially dismissed as a physiological mechanism rendered irrelevant by worldwide advances in food production, sanitation, and industrialization [1], this attitude changed considerably once scientific insights revealed how much influence breastfeeding has in modulating mechanisms, both physiological [2] and otherwise [3]. Breastfeeding, or lack thereof, can influence human physiology to such a degree that infant mortality rates directly correlate with its administration [4]. Some of the mechanisms involved are those that regulate intake and food preference, with this particular pairing having important implications for adult life [5,6]. As such, there is great interest in parsing out whether breastfeeding has an influence over other behaviors that affect reward seeking.
One such behavior is anxious behavior. It is one of the most prevalent mental disorders during childhood, and its impact can extend into adulthood [7]. It is a dysfunction in an organism’s stress reactivity that is in part mediated by the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis. Both early life stress [8] and Early Weaning [9] (EW) are directly correlated with anxious-like behavior in rats. Furthermore, early life adverse conditions in rats have a direct effect on food preference [10] and food intake [11], while enhanced maternal care with increased suckling reduces anxious behavior [12]. This posits the question of how much of this effect is due to early or insufficient weaning.
Both anxiety disorders and obesity are more prevalent in women than in men. It is possible that steroidal gonadal hormones play a role in this discrepancy. However, female steroidal hormones’ effects on both types of behaviors are paradoxical, with 17β-Estradiol (E2 for short) administration having anxiolytic effects and reducing food intake in ovariectomized rats. Furthermore, female animals are oftentimes excluded from studies to eliminate the confounding effect that hormonal differences have. Therefore, there is a great need to research the interactions between environmental and nutritional stressors such as EW and their effects on a female population. As such, we set out to determine the effects of EW and an acute alimentary stressor on the feeding behavior of female Wistar rats, along with testing to see if there is an increase in their anxious behaviors.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design

This study was a prospective, randomized, controlled, and blinded animal model study based on the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and the Animal Research: Reporting of In Vivo Experiments (ARRIVE) guidelines [8].

2.2. Animals and Experimental Protocol

2.2.1. Animals

Albino Wistar rats of reproductive age approximately 200–250 g in weight were used. They were provided by the Breeding Farm of the Department of Nutrition of the Federal University of Pernambuco. Rats were paired in the ratio of two females for each male. Gestation was confirmed through weight change verification. Once gestation was confirmed, rats were transferred to individual cages and received a standard vivarium diet (Labina, Presence). After the pups’ birth, sexing was undertaken with the goal of forming litters of right pups per mother, with male and female pups being distributed per the same ratio (4:4) whenever possible. Two female pups per litter were randomly picked to form the experimental groups, each of which was comprised of eight rats. During the entire experiment, the animals were kept in standard vivarium conditions, at a 22 °C (±1) temperature, under an inverted 12 h light–dark cycle (lights on at 18 h), and with food and water ad libitum. Care and management were undertaken following the recommendations of the Brazilian College of Animal Experimentation (COBEA) and were approved by the ethics committee on animal experimentation of the Federal University of Pernambuco (UFPE) under the license number 0020/2018 on 7 April 2018.

2.2.2. Early Weaning and Experimental Groups

Experimental groups were determined according to whether the animals were weaned early or not. Early weaning consisted of the early separation of pups from their mothers on a permanent basis starting from the 15th post-natal day, with control animals being weaned on the 30th post-natal day. Early-weaned pups received a shredded standard diet until 21 days of age. From the 21st day of age onwards all groups received a standard diet. Early-weaned and control groups were thus formed. After weaning, only females were used in this study. They were grouped in pairs in separate cages until experiments began at around 170 days of age. Each subgroup was composed of a total of eight animals. Therefore, the following experimental groups were made:
  • Control Group—weaned on a period natural for its species (30th post-natal day, n = 8);
  • Early Weaning Group—weaned on the 15th post-natal day (n = 8).

2.3. Behavioral Procedures

2.3.1. Alimentary Stress

Alimentary stress started between 166 and 168 days of age, between 12 h and 12h20. It consisted of visual and olfactory exposure to the cafeteria diet (Bauducco chocolate cookies) for twenty minutes, over 22 consecutive days. The food was wrapped in galvanic mesh (15 cm × 15 cm) that kept animals from consuming it.

2.3.2. Cafeteria Diet Intake before and after Stress

Intake measurements were carried out during five different moments: basal (sans alimentary stress) and on the 2nd, 8th, 15th, and 22nd days of alimentary stress. During basal intake measurement, animals were not exposed previously to cafeteria food wrapped in galvanic mesh; they had 1 h of free access to it. During stress days, animals had access to the cookies for 1 h after 20 min of alimentary stress. Food intake (g) was quantified through the difference between the amount offered and the remaining amount. During this experiment, the animals had free access to water.

2.3.3. Body Weight

Body weight was evaluated at the start of the experiment and after that, on a weekly basis, on days 2, 8, 15 and 22 of alimentary stress. A digital weighing balance with a sensibility of 0.001 g was used.

2.3.4. Experimental Anxiety before and after Alimentary Stress

Anxiety assessment was carried out using the elevated plus maze model. This test model lasts for 5 min and is carried out inside a maze, which consists of two open arms (50 × 10 cm) and two closed ones (50 × 10 × 40 cm) that extend from a central platform (10 × 10 cm), with everything at a height of 50 cm above ground level. The coating of the floor, walls, and central region is a dark anti-slip material. During the test, the maze was illuminated using low voltage red light (15 W). This experimental model was based on rodents’ innate fear of open elevated spaces and preference for closed arms. The test itself consisted of placing the animals individually on the device’s central space, with the head facing towards the open arm opposite from the evaluator and registering data during the following 5 min. Time spent in and number of entries into each arm were registered per protocol. An entry was only registered once an animal placed all four paws in one of the maze’s arms. Each animal’s time was measured using a digital chronometer. Time spent in the central platform was calculated using the following formula: TC= TT − (TBF + TBA), where TC is the time spent in the central platform, TT is the test’s total time, TBF is time spent in the closed arm and TBA is time spent in the open arm.
Using the elevated plus maze test, experimental anxiety has a basal assessment, with stress response being later assessed in four distinct periods during the 22 days of alimentary stress, these being days 1, 7, 14, and 21. Animals were tested for 5 min after being subjected to 20 min of alimentary stress during these days. The testing period went on from 12h00 to 15h00.

2.3.5. Standard Diet Intake after Alimentary Stress

During the 22 days of alimentary stress, standard diet intake was measured every 24 h. A measurement of 50 g of standard diet was offered once the 20 min alimentary stress period was over. After 24 h, the remaining food was weighed. This procedure was repeated until the alimentary stress period’s end, meaning until the 22nd day. Food intake (g) was quantified as the difference between the amount offered and the remaining amount.

2.3.6. Water Intake after Alimentary Stress

During the 22 days of alimentary stress, water intake was measured every 24 h. Bottles full of water weighing approximately 900 g were offered. They were weighed once the 20-min alimentary stress period was over and weighed once again after 24 h. This procedure was repeated until the alimentary stress period’s end, meaning until the 22nd day. Water intake was quantified as the difference between amount offered and remaining amount in terms of grams of weight, equivalent to milliliters of volume.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

Data were presented as median and standard deviations. Clinical analyses were performed by a two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) and Test-t. The significance value used in every assessment was p < 0.05. Statistical analyses were carried out using GraphPad Prism 5 (GraphPad Inc., La Jolla, CA, USA).

3. Results

3.1. Body Weight

There were no differences in body weight between the control and EW groups at any of the ages assessed (Figure 1).

3.2. Elevated plus Maze

There were no differences in time spent in all four arms between the control and early-weaned group. However, an analysis of the time spent in the maze’s central platform showed that weaned animals spent more time there in the day of basal analyses, before they were exposed to alimentary stress. (Control: 25.5 ± 12.7 vs. Weaned: 47.9 ± 22.4; p < 0.05) (Figure 2).
An analysis comparing different days showed that the control group spent more time in the open arms on the 7th day than compared to the basal analysis (Basal: 36.5 ± 15.5 vs. Day 7: 79.2 ± 38.0; p < 0.05) (Figure 3 top). There were no differences in the number of entries into each of the four arms between the control group and the weaned group (Figure 3 bottom).

3.3. Cafeteria Diet Intake

The analysis of chocolate cookie (Bauducco) intake showed an increase in the intake of the weaned group on the first day after alimentary stress started (control: 3.4 ± 1.5 vs. weaned: 1.8 ± 1.3; p < 0.05) (Figure 4). There were no differences in intake between groups during basal analyses nor on day 7 of alimentary stress.

3.4. Standard Diet Intake

There were no differences in standard diet intake between the control and weaned groups on any of the days assessed. However, when an analysis of the data from all the days was carried out, a similar pattern emerged in both groups, where 2 days of lower intake were followed by 5 days with higher intake during the second and third weeks of the 22 days of alimentary stress, as pictured in Figure 5.

3.5. Water Intake

There was no difference in water intake between the control and weaned groups during any of the days assessed. However, when an analysis of the data from all the days was carried out, a similar pattern emerged in both groups. Intake peaked every 7 days during the 22 days of alimentary stress, as pictured in Figure 6.

4. Discussion

In the present study, we assessed the feeding and anxious patterns of female Wistar rats subjected to Early Weaning. Compared to control females, they showed no differences in body weight, in an anxiety test after basal assessments, and in standard diet and water intake. There was only a difference in cafeteria diet intake on the second day of acute alimentary stress.
Early Weaning (EW) is a procedure that allows us to evaluate the impact that the absence of maternal milk has on diet and maternal care during a critical moment of a mammal’s development: the neonatal period. In male rats, EW initially causes weight loss during the neonatal period; however, this result reverts during adulthood [9]. This effect is in part mediated by the metabolic programming of leptin, with low levels during infancy conditioning leptin resistance during adulthood [9]. Leptin acts on a variety of neuroanatomical structures, chief among them the hypothalamus’s archeate nucleus, where it inhibits NPY/AgRp/GABA neurons through ATP-sensitive potassium channels [10].
The actions of E2 are probably responsible for the difference in results we had when compared to similar studies carried out in males. Male rats subjected to EW show increased body weight during adult age [9], along with resistance to leptin and higher levels of this hormone [9]. Curiously enough, when subjected to EW, along with alimentary stress, they do not express this increase. Male rats subjected to EW show an increased intake of both standard and cafeteria diets [11].
Estrogens are a critical factor in body weight regulation. Everything points to food intake being controlled cyclically in a manner correlated with estrogen levels [12,13]. Studies carried out with early-weaned female rats show an increase in weight in teenage rats, something that we did not observe in our study [14]. This is possibly due to serum estrogen levels varying according to the rat’s age, these being lower in younger rats [15]. Lower levels of circulating estrogen promote higher body weight, as demonstrated in ovariectomized rats [16,17]. Classic studies have demonstrated that estrogen treatment directly results in weight loss and reduced food intake in ovariectomized rats [18]. It could be that in the present study, the similarity in body weight between both groups was related to a similar lack of change in standard diet intake. Estrogen has an important role in mediating energy balance, reducing food intake, and increasing energy expenditure [19]. Early Weaning promotes weight gain in male animals but does not have a similar effect in females, probably due to estrogen’s actions in weight reduction, meaning that estrogen did not allow for EW-induced weight gain to take place, as it has an antagonistic effect on this variable. It is worth considering that apart from its effects on energy intake, estrogen also stimulates brown adipose tissue thermogenesis through its actions on the ERα receptor [20].
Intragroup standard diet intake results did, however, vary significantly between peaks of consumption that took place on days 14 and 21 and low points on days 2, 3, 4, 8, 9, 15, 26, and 22. We posit that this was a result of cafeteria diet administration on days 2, 8, 15, and 22, as cafeteria diet intake can reduce standard diet intake [21]. Despite this, standard diet intake did not differ between both groups. However, in males subjected to EW, there is an important increase in standard diet intake, especially during the dark cycle [9]. It is precisely during the dark period of the circadian cycle that females reduce meal size during estrus [14], a phase that is preceded by a peak in serum estrogen levels. Females have demonstrated markedly rhythmic behavior during the ovarian cycle [12]. Reduced food intake during estrus involves cyclical alterations in the neurobiology of meal size control [12]. Additionally, rats show low calorie efficiency [18]. In female rats’ case, leptin and estrogens interact in such a way that ovariectomized rats show high levels of leptin, along with increased body weight [22]. Leptin is an anorexigenic hormone. Among estrogens, 17β-Estradiol (E2 for short) has considerable influence over feeding [23]. It plays a role in the homeostatic control of feeding behavior by exerting both tonic [24] and phasic [25] anorexigenic effects. The former is evident in rats without ovaries, which are the main source of estrogens in females, which subsequently show an increased intake [24]. Phasic inhibition is evident through the fluctuations in intake that happen throughout the estrous cycle [25]. Along with a decrease in food intake during estrus comes a decrease in meal size. E2′s actions over feeding behavior are attributed to that hormone’s actions in the hypothalamus and nucleus tractus solitarius (NTS) [20]. Direct administration of E2 into the arcuate nucleus of the hypothalamus directly inhibits NPY/AgRP neurons, which are orexigenic. This results in reduced food intake and body weight, as well as an increase in physical activity [26]. This action is mediated by the Erα receptor, which acts both through nuclear signaling [27] and through membrane signaling in mice and guinea pigs [28].
We know that estrogens reduce food intake; however, their effects on food reward are much less clear, specifically when it comes to food’s motivational properties. Among male rats, it has been observed that EW increases cafeteria diet intake [14]. They show a higher increase of dopaminergic and serotoninergic receptors associated with higher cafeteria diet consumption [14]. However, in this study, we did not observe changes in female rats’ intake, regardless of what kind of diet they were offered, whether it was standard or cafeteria. Rats are more motivated to work for cocaine during estrus when compared to other phases of the cycle [29,30]. This is relevant, as cerebral circuitry of food motivation overlaps with that of drug addiction. [31] However, in females, it is probable that reward mechanisms for drugs and food are more distinct from each other than in males [20]. Cafeteria diet intake in females is due more to its high caloric content than to its palatable properties [32]. Estrogens decrease food intake [16], while the use of recreational drugs increases serum estrogen levels [33]. Food motivation varies along the estrous cycle, with the least amount of food motivation registered during estrus [23]. During this same experiment, the removal of the ovaries increased food-motivational behavior. It also demonstrated that estrogens act directly on the mesolimbic reward of circuits that reduce food reward, as direct E2 microinjections in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) reduced food reward behavior [23]. Additionally, E2 decreases sucrose motivation in rats [34] while not increasing the effects of food reward [35]. This is mediated by a mGluR5-dependent mechanism that selectively increases the motivation for cocaine but not for sucrose [35]. Going from there, we may conjecture that the presence of E2 bestows a protective effect against acute alimentary stress. On top of this, orexins have differing effects on sucrose intake. [36]
The instance of a significantly reduced cafeteria diet we observed among EW rats on day 2 could be explained by a higher susceptibility to stress among female EW rats that is nevertheless not strong enough to be expressed significantly in the other parameters we measured. Nevertheless, lower cafeteria diet, standard diet, and water intakes were consistently lower in the EW group relative to controls, just not in significant enough magnitude. Some studies have shown an increase in anxious behaviors among EW rodents, particularly male ones [6,37,38]. Perhaps a more sensible way of measuring the way stress affects feeding, such as the Novelty Suppressed Feeding test, which measures latency to feeding in an unfamiliar environment, could parse this out [39]. Evaluating the effect that an anxiolytic agent would have over this behavior could help clarify this possibility, too.
Within the control group, we observed a significant increase in time spent in the open arms of the EPM during day 7 when compared to the same group’s basal assesment. This is in line with a previous study carried out to measure variability in repeated EPM assesments in non-stressed male rats, where significant changes were observed towards the fourth repeated session while still remaining below a preference threshold [40]. However, there were no differences in the time spent in the open arms of the elevated plus maze between both groups. Our results are consistent with results from previous studies carried out with EW rats, where male rats spent less time in the open arms and entered them less frequently, while there were no differences in either time spent or frequency entering between both female groups [37]. Additionally, there were similarities with studies carried out in ovariectomized mice where doses of E2 had an anxiolytic effect, increasing the time spent in the open arms of the maze during the elevated plus maze test [41]. Based on that, we may conjecture that these results are the product of the anxiolytic property of estrogens, E2 being chief among them. These actions vary according to which receptor they activate. For example, studies carried out in knockout mice found that the ERα receptor regulates the hormone’s anxiogenic effect, while ERβ knockouts and agonists, such as diarylpropionitrile and WAY-200070, increase anxious symptoms in tests such as the elevated plus maze in both rats and mice. Furthermore, the GPR30 membrane receptor is involved in the stress response since it shows increased immunoreactivity in the basal amygdala in ovariectomized mice subjected to stress. This results in a decrease in time spent in the EPM’s open arms [42]. The increase in time spent in the maze’s central area that we observed among EW rats during basal testing is a curious datum that could point towards changes in unknown behavioral parameters caused by EW. Until now, time spent in that area has been correlated with increased impulsivity during operant conditioning tasks [43]. Impulsivity has been linked to anxiety-like behavior both in rats [44] and, more controversially, in humans [45].
As for water intake, we know that it is prandial in rats, with 70–85% of spontaneous intake directly related to feeding [46]. This could be the reason why there were no differences in water intake between groups during our experiment since water intake closely mirrored food intake. We did observe peaks on days 7, 14, and 21, but we believe these were caused by additional time during which both groups had access to the water bottles as they awaited behavioral testing (EPM) compared to the rest of the days. Nevertheless, E2 is also involved in the regulation of this behavior. It has an anti-dipsogenic effect, even though that effect seems to be mediated more by organizational differences since hormone injections do not have the same effect on male rats. The structures that regulate this effect are mainly concentrated in the hypothalamus. These include the Subfornical Organ, the Lateral Hypothalamus, the Paraventricular Nucleus, and the Median Preoptic Nucleus [47]. Even though water intake in rats is prandial, E2′s anti-dipsogenic effects do not result in a decrease in food intake. Central hormone injections result in a decrease in water intake in rats, especially when administered in the lateral hypothalamus, [48], while chronic subcutaneous administration increases water intake [49].
Our study shows what we believe to be a protective effect of gonadal steroidal hormones against feeding behavior dysregulation and anxiety-like symptoms among female rats subjected to Early Weaning and alimentary stress. While the results are consistent with previous literature, they appear to be contradictory with the reality of the higher prevalence of feeding and anxiety disorders among women. Our results show the importance of continued research into the complex interactions between Early Weaning, diverse diets, and hormonal states. The use of ovariectomization, specific estrogenic receptor antagonism at different ages, and anxiolytic agents could help parse out these interactions and possibly bring about new dietary interventions and pharmacological treatments to combat the illnesses that result from their imbalance.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, V.I.M.D., J.A.d.S. and S.L.d.S.; Formal analysis, V.I.M.D. and J.A.d.S.; Funding acquisition, S.L.d.S.; Investigation, V.I.M.D.; Methodology, V.I.M.D., J.A.d.S. and S.L.d.S.; Project administration, J.A.d.S. and S.L.d.S.; Resources, S.L.d.S.; Software, J.A.d.S.; Supervision, J.A.d.S. and S.L.d.S.; Validation, S.L.d.S.; Visualization, S.L.d.S.; Writing—original draft, V.I.M.D.; Writing—review & editing, V.I.M.D., J.A.d.S. and S.L.d.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior-Brasil (CAPES) finance code 001 and supported by Pró-Reitoria de Pesquisa e Pós-Graduação da UFPE (PROPESP/UFPE, Brazil).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the CEUA of UFPE (protocol code 0020/2018 06/06/2018).

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to express their gratitude to the Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES), the Empresa Brasileira de Pesquisa Agropecuária (EMBRAPA Amazônia Oriental), and the Brazilian National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Abbreviations

EWEarly Weaning
E217β-Estradiol
EPMElevated Plus Maze
NPYNeuropeptide Y
AgRpAgouti-Related Protein
GABAγ-aminobutyric Acid
mGluR5Metabotropic Glutamatergic Receptor 5
ERαEstrogen Receptor Alpha
OECDOrganization for Economic Cooperation and Development
UFPEFederal University of Pernambuco
CEUAEthics Committee on the Use of Animals
CAPESCoordination for Improvement of Higher Education Personnel
CNPqBrazilian National Council for Scientific and Technological Development
PROCADNational Academic Cooperation Program

References

  1. Report of the Task Force on the Assessment of the Scientific Evidence Relating to Infant-Feeding Practices and Infant Health. Pediatrics 1984, 74, 579–762.
  2. Giugliani, E.R.J.; Horta, B.L.; Loret de Mola, C.; Lisboa, B.O.; Victora, C.G. Effect of breastfeeding promotion interventions on child growth: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Acta Paediatr. 2015, 104, 20–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Bowatte, G.; Tham, R.; Allen, K.J.; Tan, D.J.; Lau, M.; Dai, X.; Lodge, C.J. Breastfeeding and childhood acute otitis media: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Acta Paediatr. 2015, 104, 85–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Sankar, M.J.; Sinha, B.; Chowdhury, R.; Bhandari, N.; Taneja, S.; Martines, J.; Bahl, R. Optimal breastfeeding practices and infant and child mortality: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Acta Paediatr. 2015, 104, 3–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Ventura, A.K. Does Breastfeeding Shape Food Preferences? Links to Obesity. Ann. Nutr. Metab. 2017, 70 (Suppl. S3), 8–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Kanari, K.; Kikusui, T.; Takeuchi, Y.; Mori, Y. Multidimensional structure of anxiety-related behavior in early-weaned rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2005, 156, 45–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Lähdepuro, A.; Savolainen, K.; Lahti-Pulkkinen, M.; Eriksson, J.G.; Lahti, J.; Tuovinen, S.; Kajantie, E.; Pesonen, A.-K.; Heinonen, K.; Räikkönen, K. The Impact of Early Life Stress on Anxiety Symptoms in Late Adulthood. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 4395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Du Percie Sert, N.; Hurst, V.; Ahluwalia, A.; Alam, S.; Avey, M.T.; Baker, M.; Browne, W.J.; Clark, A.; Cuthill, I.C.; Dirnagl, U.; et al. The ARRIVE guidelines 2.0: Updated guidelines for reporting animal research. PLoS Biol. 2020, 18, e3000410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Lima, N.d.S.; Moura, E.G.d.; Passos, M.C.F.; Nogueira Neto, F.J.; Reis, A.M.; Oliveira, E.d.; Lisboa, P.C. Early weaning causes undernutrition for a short period and programmes some metabolic syndrome components and leptin resistance in adult rat offspring. Br. J. Nutr. 2011, 105, 1405–1413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Baquero, A.F.; Solis, A.J.d.; Lindsley, S.R.; Kirigiti, M.A.; Smith, M.S.; Cowley, M.A.; Zeltser, L.M.; Grove, K.L. Developmental switch of leptin signaling in arcuate nucleus neurons. J. Neurosci. 2014, 34, 9982–9994. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Tavares, G.A.; do Amaral Almeida, L.C.; Souza, J.A.d.; Farias, V.V.d.; Souza, F.L.d.; Andrade Silva, S.C.d.; Lagranha, C.J.; Kaeffer, B.; Souza, S.L.d. Early weaning leads to disruption of homeostatic and hedonic eating behaviors and modulates serotonin (5HT) and dopamine (DA) systems in male adult rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2020, 383, 112531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Tarttelin, M.F.; Gorski, R.A. The effects of ovarian steroids on food and water intake and body weight in the female rat. Acta Endocrinol. 1973, 72, 551–568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Tarttelin, M.F. Cyclical variations in food and water intakes in ewes. J. Physiol. 1968, 195, 29P–31P. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  14. Tavares, G.A.; Almeida, L.C.d.A.; Souza, J.A.d.; Braz, G.R.F.; da Silva, M.C.; Lagranha, C.J.; do Nascimento, E.; Souza, S.L.d. Early weaning disrupts feeding patterns in female juvenile rats through 5HT-system modulations. Behav. Processes 2020, 170, 103981. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Steger, R.W.; Peluso, J.J. Effects of age on hormone levels and in vitro steroidogenesis by rat ovary and adrenal. Exp. Aging Res. 1982, 8, 203–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Asarian, L.; Geary, N. Cyclic estradiol treatment normalizes body weight and restores physiological patterns of spontaneous feeding and sexual receptivity in ovariectomized rats. Horm. Behav. 2002, 42, 461–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Wallen, W.J.; Belanger, M.P.; Wittnich, C. Sex hormones and the selective estrogen receptor modulator tamoxifen modulate weekly body weights and food intakes in adolescent and adult rats. J. Nutr. 2001, 131, 2351–2357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Wade, G.N. Some effects of ovarian hormones on food intake and body weight in female rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1975, 88, 183–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Eckel, L.A.; Langhans, W.; Kahler, A.; Campfield, L.A.; Smith, F.J.; Geary, N. Chronic administration of OB protein decreases food intake by selectively reducing meal size in female rats. Am. J. Physiol. 1998, 275, R186–R193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Martínez de Morentin, P.B.; González-García, I.; Martins, L.; Lage, R.; Fernández-Mallo, D.; Martínez-Sánchez, N.; Ruíz-Pino, F.; Liu, J.; Morgan, D.A.; Pinilla, L.; et al. Estradiol regulates brown adipose tissue thermogenesis via hypothalamic AMPK. Cell Metab. 2014, 20, 41–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. South, T.; Holmes, N.M.; Martire, S.I.; Westbrook, R.F.; Morris, M.J. Rats eat a cafeteria-style diet to excess but eat smaller amounts and less frequently when tested with chow. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e93506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Chu, S.-C.; Chou, Y.-C.; Liu, J.-Y.; Chen, C.-H.; Shyu, J.-C.; Chou, F.-P. Fluctuation of serum leptin level in rats after ovariectomy and the influence of estrogen supplement. Life Sci. 1999, 64, 2299–2306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Rivera, H.M.; Stincic, T.L. Estradiol and the control of feeding behavior. Steroids 2018, 133, 44–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Drewett, R.F. Oestrous and dioestrous components of the ovarian inhibition on hunger in the rat. Anim. Behav. 1973, 21, 772–780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Eckel, L. Spontaneous meal patterns in female rats with and without access to running wheels. Physiol. Behav. 2000, 70, 397–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Ahdieh, H.B.; Wade, G.N. Effects of hysterectomy on sexual receptivity, food intake, running wheel activity, and hypothalamic estrogen and progestin receptors in rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1982, 96, 886–892. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Gruber, C.J.; Gruber, D.M.; Gruber, I.M.L.; Wieser, F.; Huber, J.C. Anatomy of the estrogen response element. Trends Endocrinol. Metab. 2004, 15, 73–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Qiu, J.; Bosch, M.A.; Tobias, S.C.; Krust, A.; Graham, S.M.; Murphy, S.J.; Korach, K.S.; Chambon, P.; Scanlan, T.S.; Rønnekleiv, O.K.; et al. A G-protein-coupled estrogen receptor is involved in hypothalamic control of energy homeostasis. J. Neurosci. 2006, 26, 5649–5655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Carroll, M.E.; Morgan, A.D.; Lynch, W.J.; Campbell, U.C.; Dess, N.K. Intravenous cocaine and heroin self-administration in rats selectively bred for differential saccharin intake: Phenotype and sex differences. Psychopharmacology 2002, 161, 304–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Roberts, D.C.; Bennett, S.A.; Vickers, G.J. The estrous cycle affects cocaine self-administration on a progressive ratio schedule in rats. Psychopharmacology 1989, 98, 408–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Volkow, N.D.; Wang, G.-J.; Baler, R.D. Reward, dopamine and the control of food intake: Implications for obesity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011, 15, 37–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  32. Galea, L. High levels of estradiol disrupt conditioned place preference learning, stimulus response learning and reference memory but have limited effects on working memory. Behav. Brain Res. 2001, 126, 115–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Becker, J.B.; Hu, M. Sex differences in drug abuse. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 2008, 29, 36–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Richard, J.E.; López-Ferreras, L.; Anderberg, R.H.; Olandersson, K.; Skibicka, K.P. Estradiol is a critical regulator of food-reward behavior. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2017, 78, 193–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Martinez, L.A.; Gross, K.S.; Himmler, B.T.; Emmitt, N.L.; Peterson, B.M.; Zlebnik, N.E.; Foster Olive, M.; Carroll, M.E.; Meisel, R.L.; Mermelstein, P.G. Estradiol Facilitation of Cocaine Self-Administration in Female Rats Requires Activation of mGluR5. Eneuro 2016, 3, 0140-16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Cason, A.M.; Aston-Jones, G. Role of orexin/hypocretin in conditioned sucrose-seeking in female rats. Neuropharmacology 2014, 86, 97–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Ito, A.; Kikusui, T.; Takeuchi, Y.; Mori, Y. Effects of early weaning on anxiety and autonomic responses to stress in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2006, 171, 87–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Shimozuru, M.; Kodama, Y.; Iwasa, T.; Kikusui, T.; Takeuchi, Y.; Mori, Y. Early weaning decreases play-fighting behavior during the postweaning developmental period of Wistar rats. Dev. Psychobiol. 2007, 49, 343–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Campos, A.C.; Fogaça, M.V.; Aguiar, D.C.; Guimarães, F.S. Animal models of anxiety disorders and stress. Braz. J. Psychiatry 2013, 35 (Suppl. S2), S101–S111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Schrader, A.J.; Taylor, R.M.; Lowery-Gionta, E.G.; Moore, N.L.T. Repeated elevated plus maze trials as a measure for tracking within-subjects behavioral performance in rats (Rattus norvegicus). PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0207804. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Kastenberger, I.; Lutsch, C.; Schwarzer, C. Activation of the G-protein-coupled receptor GPR30 induces anxiogenic effects in mice, similar to oestradiol. Psychopharmacology 2012, 221, 527–535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  42. Borrow, A.P.; Handa, R.J. Estrogen Receptors Modulation of Anxiety-Like Behavior. Vitam. Horm. 2017, 103, 27–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Rico, J.L.; Hurtado-Parrado, C.; Vásquez-Sepúlveda, J.; Fonseca, J.; Cardona, Á. El tiempo en el área central del laberinto en cruz elevado correlaciona con medidas relacionadas a impulsividad durante una tarea operante. Univ. Psychol. 2017, 15, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Stein, J.S.; Renda, C.R.; Barker, S.M.; Liston, K.J.; Shahan, T.A.; Madden, G.J. Impulsive choice predicts anxiety-like behavior, but not alcohol or sucrose consumption, in male Long-Evans rats. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2015, 39, 932–940. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Jakuszkowiak-Wojten, K.; Landowski, J.; Wiglusz, M.S.; Cubała, W.J. Impulsivity in anxiety disorders. A critical review. Psychiatr. Danub. 2015, 27 (Suppl. S1), S452–S455. [Google Scholar]
  46. Fitzsimons, T.J.; Le Magnen, J. Eating as a regulatory control of drinking in the rat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1969, 67, 273–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Shughrue, P.J.; Lane, M.V.; Merchenthaler, I. Comparative distribution of estrogen receptor-α and-β mRNA in the rat central nervous system. J. Comp. Neurol. 1997, 388, 507–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Suzuki, T.; Kohno, H.; Sakurada, T.; Tadano, T.; Kisara, K. Intracranial injection of thyrotropin releasing hormone (TRH) suppresses starvation-induced feeding and drinking in rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1982, 17, 249–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Choi, Y.-H.; Hartzell, D.; Azain, M.J.; Baile, C.A. TRH decreases food intake and increases water intake and body temperature in rats. Physiol. Behav. 2002, 77, 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Effect of Early Weaning associated with chronic stress on body weight of adult rats. The animals’ body weight was obtained at ages 166–168 days (basal analysis) and on days 2 (169–171 days of age), 8 (175–177 days of age), 15 (182–184 days of age), and 22 (188–190 days of age) of alimentary stress. Two-way ANOVA and Bonferroni’s post-hoc test n = 8. Data expressed as mean ± SD.
Figure 1. Effect of Early Weaning associated with chronic stress on body weight of adult rats. The animals’ body weight was obtained at ages 166–168 days (basal analysis) and on days 2 (169–171 days of age), 8 (175–177 days of age), 15 (182–184 days of age), and 22 (188–190 days of age) of alimentary stress. Two-way ANOVA and Bonferroni’s post-hoc test n = 8. Data expressed as mean ± SD.
Behavsci 12 00171 g001
Figure 2. Effect of Early Weaning associated to chronic stress over time spent in central platform of the elevated plus maze. Data were obtained from animals during their 166–168 days of age (basal analysis) and during days 1 (168–170 days of age), 7 (174–176 days of age), 14 (181–183 days of age) and 21 (187–189 days of age) of alimentary stress. t-test n = 8. Data expressed as mean ± SD. * p < 0.05 vs. controls.
Figure 2. Effect of Early Weaning associated to chronic stress over time spent in central platform of the elevated plus maze. Data were obtained from animals during their 166–168 days of age (basal analysis) and during days 1 (168–170 days of age), 7 (174–176 days of age), 14 (181–183 days of age) and 21 (187–189 days of age) of alimentary stress. t-test n = 8. Data expressed as mean ± SD. * p < 0.05 vs. controls.
Behavsci 12 00171 g002
Figure 3. Effect of Early Weaning associated with chronic stress over time spent in open and closed arms (two upper figures) and over number of entries into open and closed arms (two lower figures) of the elevated plus maze. Data were obtained from animals during their 166–168 days of age (basal analysis) and on days 1 (168–170 days of age), 7 (174–176 days of age), 14 (181–183 days of age), and 21 (187–189 days of age) of alimentary stress. Two-way ANOVA and Bonferroni’s post hoc test n = 8. Data expressed as mean ± SD. * p < 0.05 vs. Basal.
Figure 3. Effect of Early Weaning associated with chronic stress over time spent in open and closed arms (two upper figures) and over number of entries into open and closed arms (two lower figures) of the elevated plus maze. Data were obtained from animals during their 166–168 days of age (basal analysis) and on days 1 (168–170 days of age), 7 (174–176 days of age), 14 (181–183 days of age), and 21 (187–189 days of age) of alimentary stress. Two-way ANOVA and Bonferroni’s post hoc test n = 8. Data expressed as mean ± SD. * p < 0.05 vs. Basal.
Behavsci 12 00171 g003
Figure 4. Effect of Early Weaning associated with chronic stress on chocolate cookie (Bauducco) intake. Data were obtained from animals during their 166–168 days of age (basal analysis) and on days 2 (169–171 days of age), 8 (175–177 days of age), 15 (182–184 days of age), and 22 (188–190 days of age) of alimentary stress. t-test n = 8. Data expressed as mean ± SD. * p < 0.05 vs. controls.
Figure 4. Effect of Early Weaning associated with chronic stress on chocolate cookie (Bauducco) intake. Data were obtained from animals during their 166–168 days of age (basal analysis) and on days 2 (169–171 days of age), 8 (175–177 days of age), 15 (182–184 days of age), and 22 (188–190 days of age) of alimentary stress. t-test n = 8. Data expressed as mean ± SD. * p < 0.05 vs. controls.
Behavsci 12 00171 g004
Figure 5. Effect of Early Weaning associated with chronic stress over standard diet intake. Data were obtained every 24 hrs between days 2 and 22 of alimentary stress. Two-way ANOVA. Bonferroni’s post-hoc test n = 8. Data expressed as mean ± SD. * Shows the points where both groups showed statistically lower values when compared to the highest levels.
Figure 5. Effect of Early Weaning associated with chronic stress over standard diet intake. Data were obtained every 24 hrs between days 2 and 22 of alimentary stress. Two-way ANOVA. Bonferroni’s post-hoc test n = 8. Data expressed as mean ± SD. * Shows the points where both groups showed statistically lower values when compared to the highest levels.
Behavsci 12 00171 g005
Figure 6. Effect of Early Weaning associated with chronic stress over water intake. Data were obtained every 24 hrs between days 2 and 7 of alimentary stress. Two-way ANOVA and Bonferroni’s post-hoc test n = 8. Data expressed as mean ± SD. * Shows the points at which both groups showed statistically higher values when compared to the lowest values, indicating peaks.
Figure 6. Effect of Early Weaning associated with chronic stress over water intake. Data were obtained every 24 hrs between days 2 and 7 of alimentary stress. Two-way ANOVA and Bonferroni’s post-hoc test n = 8. Data expressed as mean ± SD. * Shows the points at which both groups showed statistically higher values when compared to the lowest values, indicating peaks.
Behavsci 12 00171 g006
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Díaz, V.I.M.; de Souza, J.A.; de Sousa, S.L. Effects of Early Weaning Associated with Alimentary Stress on Emotional and Feeding Behavior of Female Adult Wistar Rats. Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 171. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12060171

AMA Style

Díaz VIM, de Souza JA, de Sousa SL. Effects of Early Weaning Associated with Alimentary Stress on Emotional and Feeding Behavior of Female Adult Wistar Rats. Behavioral Sciences. 2022; 12(6):171. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12060171

Chicago/Turabian Style

Díaz, Víctor Isaac Meléndez, Julliet Araújo de Souza, and Sandra Lopes de Sousa. 2022. "Effects of Early Weaning Associated with Alimentary Stress on Emotional and Feeding Behavior of Female Adult Wistar Rats" Behavioral Sciences 12, no. 6: 171. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12060171

APA Style

Díaz, V. I. M., de Souza, J. A., & de Sousa, S. L. (2022). Effects of Early Weaning Associated with Alimentary Stress on Emotional and Feeding Behavior of Female Adult Wistar Rats. Behavioral Sciences, 12(6), 171. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12060171

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop