Next Article in Journal
Return to the New Normal: Empirical Analysis of Changes in E-Consumer Behavior during the COVID-19 Pandemic
Previous Article in Journal
COVID-19-Related Knowledge and Anxiety Response among Physical Education Teachers during Practical In-Person Lessons: Effects of Potential Moderators
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

From Childhood Residential Green space to Adult Mental Wellbeing: A Pathway Analysis among Chinese Adults

1
Institute of Sports Science, College of Physical Education, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China
2
Department of Park, Recreation and Tourism Management, Clemson University, Clemson, SC 29634, USA
3
Clinical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, School of Medical Sciences, Örebro University, Orebro 70182, Sweden
4
Unit of Integrative Epidemiology, Institute of Environmental Medicine, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm 1317177, Sweden
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2022, 12(3), 84; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12030084
Submission received: 14 February 2022 / Revised: 2 March 2022 / Accepted: 14 March 2022 / Published: 17 March 2022

Abstract

:
Residential green spaces, arguably the most accessible type of urban green space, may have lasting impacts on children and even change their lives later in adulthood. However, the potential pathways from childhood residential green space to adulthood mental wellbeing are not well understood. Therefore, we conducted a questionnaire survey among Chinese adults (N = 770) in September 2021 to capture data on subjective measures of residential green space and nature contact during childhood, and nature connectedness, nature contact, and mental wellbeing during adulthood. Structural equation modeling (SEM) was employed to examine theoretical pathways between childhood residential green space and adult mental wellbeing. The results suggest that childhood residential green space positively predicts childhood nature contact and also has direct and indirect positive impacts on nature contact, nature connectedness, and mental wellbeing during adulthood. These findings advance understanding of the long-term impacts of childhood residential green space. Policymakers are advised to prioritize residential greening as well as other recreational facilities for children when planning health-promoting environments in urban spaces. Due to limitations in our study design, we also advise future studies to re-examine and extend the framework documented here.

1. Introduction

Contacting with nature is known to benefit mental and physical health. However, owing to global urbanization, more than half of the world’s population lives in cities, and this proportion is projected to reach nearly 70% by 2050 [1]. People today are constantly staying indoors, especially children, and are less active in natural environments than previous generations, bringing up concerns about a growing “disconnection” from nature [2,3].
In this context, urban green spaces are valuable for maintaining connections between people and nature. Urban green spaces can offer less polluted and attractive environments for urban residents to carry out various activities, thus enabling them to interact with natural elements more physically than an entirely “gray” urban environment [4,5]. Among the various types of urban green spaces, residential (near/at home) green spaces may be popular because travel distance is a critical factor that affects green space utilization, and a closer green space usually means more frequent visits [6,7].
So far, the health benefits of residential green space have been widely documented, such as lower risk of depression and higher levels of wellbeing [8,9]. Studies have also explored the pathways underneath these health benefits [10,11,12]. Notably, some recent studies have identified the impact of childhood residential green space on mental health issues in adulthood [13,14], implying a long-term impact of residential green space. These findings collectively enrich the previous knowledge concerning the impacts of childhood experiences and environments on mental health in later life [15,16,17]. Nevertheless, since the long-term impact of green space remains an emerging topic, the potential pathways are not fully understood, and the impact across time requires theoretical clarification.

1.1. Role of Residential Green Space on Children’s Nature Contact

Residential environments are known to affect children’s health and behavior [18,19,20,21]. For example, higher levels of residential green space have been associated with lower problematic behaviors, higher intelligence, and better neurodevelopment [21,22,23]. However, these studies have concentrated on the conditions of residential green space, while children’s physical contact with nature has not been fully considered. Theoretically, green space may motivate children to perform activities inside by sheltering them from heat and urban pollution [4,24]. According to a Chinese report, children in Shang, China, mainly perform outdoor activities in residential green space, indicating the role of residential green space in providing children places for nature contact [25]. Based on these clues, we hypothesized a pathway from residential green space to children’s contact with nature.

1.2. From Childhood Nature Contact to Adult Mental Wellbeing

Though we have identified limited evidence for a pathway linking childhood nature contact and adult mental wellbeing, we posited two indirect pathways based on existing clues.
The first indirect pathway is mediated by adult nature contact. As described above, childhood experiences may affect adult behaviors. Similarly, childhood experiences in nature may affect behavior that spurs nature contact. For example, Thompson, et al. [26] found that frequent visits to green spaces in childhood were strongly associated with being prepared to visit green spaces alone as an adult. Holt, et al. [27] found that daily interactions with green space in childhood were associated with frequent green space use as a university student. Similar pathways linking childhood and adulthood were confirmed in pathway analyses [28,29]. Hence, we established a pathway between childhood nature contact and adulthood nature contact. Since contact with nature is well-known to benefit mental wellbeing, we further established a pathway between adult nature contact and adult mental wellbeing [30,31].
The second indirect pathway is mediated by nature connectedness. Nature connectedness refers to an emotional attachment to the natural world [32]. According to previous research, nature connectedness can be developed through nature exposure [33,34], and childhood is a critical time window for cultivating nature connectedness [2]. Therefore, children’s nature contact may help them gain a stronger “connectedness” with nature. In addition, as nature connectedness may not readily decay over time [35], children who developed nature connectedness may be able to maintain this emotional attachment when they grow up. For these reasons, we established a pathway between childhood nature contact and adult nature connectedness. According to the biophilia hypothesis, individuals’ psychological health is associated with their relationship to nature [36]. Some previous studies have suggested potential impacts of nature connectedness on wellbeing [37,38]. Accordingly, we established a pathway between nature connectedness and mental wellbeing.

1.3. Conceptual Model

Based on the above-hypothesized pathways, we developed our conceptual framework that links residential green space in childhood and mental wellbeing in adulthood (Figure 1). Several demographic variables such as gender, age, and income are also considered in the framework. These variables have been reported to impact mental health [39,40,41], as well as psychological traits, such as nature connectedness [42]. Regarding gender, there is an assumption that women tend to be more sensitive to aspects related to the natural world [43]. Gender is also associated with certain social roles, which may influence their time at home or visiting outdoor environments and, therefore, chances for nature contact [44]. In addition, gender may influence willingness to engage in outdoor physical activities and chances for nature contact [45]. Regarding age, we can expect a similar impact of social roles, life stages, and sensitivity to aspects of the natural world to vary by age. Regarding income, family-level socioeconomic status (SES) may impact the health benefits of exposure to green spaces [46]. Last, regarding urbanization, population density can affect residents’ chances for green space access and associated health benefits [24,47,48]. Based on the framework, we hypothesized the following:
(1)
Childhood residential green space may affect adult nature contact and nature connectedness through childhood nature contact;
(2)
Childhood residential green space may affect adult mental wellbeing through childhood nature contact, adult nature contact, and adult nature connectedness.
Figure 1. Conceptual framework linking childhood residential green space to adult mental wellbeing.
Figure 1. Conceptual framework linking childhood residential green space to adult mental wellbeing.
Behavsci 12 00084 g001

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design and Population

We conducted a cross-sectional investigation during September 2021. The target population was Chinese citizens aged 18 years or older without restriction for the size of cities that respondents lived in. The study was approved and supervised by the Ethics Review Board of Southwest University, China.

2.2. Procedure

Our research team distributed messages via online social media including WeChat and Tencent QQ to recruit participants. We described the study’s goal as examining the relationship between green space in urban environments and the public health of urban residents. The details of the research questions were not disclosed during the recruitment. We offered a compensation of RMB 4 per participant for completing the online questionnaire to maximize the response rate.
The participants used an identity authentication platform to complete the questionnaire. They were asked to sign a consent form for the investigation and finish the questionnaire within 24 h. In total, 752 successfully submitted questionnaires were collected and included for analysis.

2.3. Measurements

2.3.1. Childhood Residential Green Space

Given the difficulties of recalling childhood, we used a single question to investigate the greenness of childhood residential green space, which was used in previous studies to capture perceived green space or nature exposure [10]. We investigated nature exposure during childhood within a predefined timeframe [49], which aligns with the age between 5 and 10 years old. The specific statement was:
“Please indicate the degree of greenness in your residential neighborhood when you were between 5 and 10 years old. Greenness refers to trees or any plants; the residential neighborhood refers to the area around your residences that is within a very few minutes walk”.
We used a 5-level response scale to collect answers, where 1 = extremely low (almost no greenness), 2 = low, 3 = moderate, 4 = high, and 5 = extremely high (the open space is basically full of greenness).

2.3.2. Childhood Nature Contact

We followed the approach by Larson, et al. [50] and Rosa, et al. [28] to investigate the contact with nature during childhood. We defined contact as intentional activities in a natural environment, including physically visiting, playing, and/or engaging in sport. In line with the above time frame, we asked the following question:
“Between 5 and 10 years old, how frequently did you participate in leisure activities in contact with nature? Leisure activities in nature may include visiting natural places, playing soccer or volleyball at the beach, swimming, surfing, camping, hiking, etc.”.
Answers were obtained on a 5-level response scale (1 = never, 2 = a few days, 3 = half of days, 4 = most of days, 5 = almost every day).

2.3.3. Adult Nature Contact

Following the approach for childhood nature contact, a similar question was deployed to investigate respondents’ current contact with nature:
“Currently, how frequently do you participate in leisure activities in contact with nature? Leisure activities in nature may include visiting natural places, playing soccer or volleyball at the beach, swimming, surfing, camping, hiking, etc.”. Answers were obtained on a 5-level response scale (1 = never, 2 = a few days, 3 = half of days, 4 = most of days, 5 = almost every day).

2.3.4. Adult Nature Connectedness

We employed a short version of the Connectedness to Nature Scale (CNS-7) to measure nature connectedness [51]. Participants rated seven items on their affective connections with nature, such as “I often feel a kinship with animals and plants”. Answers were obtained on a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree). The validity and reliability of the CNS-7 have been confirmed in several studies [51,52]. The CNS-7 showed good internal consistency in the current study (Cronbach α > 0.9).

2.3.5. Adult Mental Wellbeing

The WHO-5 wellbeing index was used as a reliable and validated scale to measure mental wellbeing [53]. This scale consists of seven items, such as “I have felt calm and relaxed” and “I woke up feeling fresh and rested”. Answers were obtained on a 5-point Likert-type scale (0 = never, 4 = every time). The WHO-5 has been previously employed to investigate mental wellbeing under the impacts of green space exposure [54]. To better associate mental wellbeing with green exercise levels, we used “over the last month” as the time frame for these questions. We used a Chinese version of WHO-5, and it showed good internal consistency in the current study (Cronbach α > 0.9).

2.3.6. Sociodemographic Characteristics

We controlled for three sociodemographic characteristics at the individual-level: age (in years), gender (male = 1, female = 2), and monthly income (1 = no income, 2 = income ≤ RMB 3000, 3 = RMB 3001–5000, 4 = RMB 5001–7000, 5 = RMB 7001–9000, 6 = RMB 9001–11,000, 7 = RMB 11,001–13,000, 8 = RMB 13,001–15,000, 9 = more than RMB 15,000). One RMB is approximately 0.8 USD or 0.7 EUR.

2.3.7. Urbanization Rate

The urbanization rate was obtained from the National Bureau of Statistics of China. It is the ratio of urban populations divided by the total populations of a specific province. For example, an urbanization rate of 0.5 indicates that 50% of the population is settled in urban areas. Our data was calculated based on the national investigation at the end of 2020.

3. Analysis

3.1. Internal Reliability of Employed Instrument and Bivariate Correlations

Internal reliability levels of the CNS-7 and WHO-5 were analyzed using Cronbach’s alpha. Spearman and point-biserial correlations (for gender) were employed to detect general patterns of associations within the data.

3.2. Application of Structural Equations Modeling

Structural equations modeling (SEM) was employed to investigate the hypothesized directional pathways in the presented hypothesized framework. The initial model (M0) was constructed as shown in Figure 2.
According to Bagozzi and Yi [55], the sample size for SEM should be twice the number of model parameters. Our sample size was adequate based on the parameters required to estimate in our framework (n = 37). Guided by previous pathway analysis [11,12], nature connectedness and mental wellbeing were processed as continuous summary scores. Therefore, no latent variable was involved in the current study. A Variance Inflation Factor (VIF) value smaller than 5.0 was considered evidence for the absence of multicollinearity. Based on this rule, no multicollinearity was observed for the independent variables (VIF < 3.0) [56].
We used a maximum likelihood estimator with the bootstrap generated (10,000 samples) confidence intervals, standard errors, and significant levels for all pathways [57,58,59]. The goodness of fit was assessed using the following indices: χ2/df < 5.00; root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) < 0.08; goodness-of-fit index (GFI) > 0.90; adjusted goodness of fit index (AGFI) > 0.90; Bentler’s comparative fit index (CFI) > 0.90; and Bentler–Bonett normed fit index (NFI) > 0.90.

3.3. Model Adjustment

The initial model (M0) showed a poor fit to the data (χ2/df = 14.17, GFI = 0.95, AGFI = 0.83, NFI = 0.81, CFI = 0.82, RMSEA = 0.13). After that, we modified the model by establishing new pathways between the core variables according to modification indices and potential causalities. The modified model (M1) showed an acceptable fit to the data (χ2/df = 2.87, NFI = 0.97, GFI = 0.99, AGFI = 0.96, CFI = 0.98, RMSEA = 0.50). Thus, this model was selected as the final model, and non-significant pathways were retained.

3.4. Sensitivity Analysis

We established competing models by altering theoretical causalities of the final model and comparing them with the final model using Akaike information criterion (AIC) and Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) values [60]. In addition, we conducted a multigroup pathway analysis to examine if the final model applied differentially to men and women. Given the generation difference, a multigroup pathway analysis was also conducted for data stratified by the median age (28 years).

4. Results

4.1. Characteristics of the Participants

The average age of the participants was 31.15 years, and 43.62% were females. More than half of the participants reported a high or extremely high level of greenness in their childhood residential neighborhood. Additionally, most of the participants (28.86%) reported that they contacted nature almost every day during their childhood. However, only 10.11% of the participants reported nature contact almost every day as an adult (Table 1).

4.2. Correlations between the Measurements

Table 2 presents the bivariate correlations between all measured variables. Childhood residential green space and nature connectedness were positively correlated with all core variables. Females tended to have higher nature connectedness than males. Regarding control variables, age was positively correlated with childhood nature contact, adult nature contact, and nature connectedness. Income was positively correlated with all core variables. Urbanization rate was positively correlated with adult nature contact.

4.3. SEM Model

In the final model, all the hypothesized direct pathways in the conceptual framework (Figure 1) were statistically significant (Figure 3). In addition, the new pathways established during the model adjustment were found to be statistically significant. Specifically, we found direct pathways between childhood residential green space and adult nature contact (β = 0.16, p < 0.001), adult nature connectedness (β = 0.13, p = 0.001), and adult mental wellbeing (β = 0.09, p = 0.001). We also identified a direct pathway between adult nature contact and adult nature connectedness (β = 0.40, p < 0.001).
We found that childhood residential green space had significant total impacts on childhood nature contact (β = 0.55, p < 0.001), adult nature contact (β = 0.30, p < 0.001), adult nature connectedness (β = 0.30, p < 0.001), and adult mental wellbeing (β = 0.22, p < 0.001) (Table 3). The indirect impacts of childhood residential green space on adult nature contact (β = 0.15, p < 0.001), adult nature connectedness (β = 0.16, p < 0.001), and adult mental wellbeing (β = 0.12, p < 0.001) were also statistically significant (Table 3).
We also examined the specific indirect pathways between childhood residential green space and adult mental wellbeing (Table 4). Although all pathways were statistically significant, the pathway mediated by childhood nature contact and adult nature connectedness was relatively weak (β = 0.008, p = 0.033). The pathway mediated by adult nature contact showed the strongest effect (β = 0.029, p < 0.001).

4.4. Sensitivity Analysis

We compared the final model with two competing models based on the extra hypotheses: (1) adults with higher mental wellbeing are more willing to contact nature (model C1), and (2) higher nature connectedness may encourage more frequent nature contact (model C2) (Supplementary Figure S1). The competing models showed acceptable fits, but the final model retained the lowest AIC and BIC value, ultimately indicating a better fit (Supplementary Table S1).
In models stratified by gender, the baseline model (unconstrained model) and constrained model (structural weights were constrained) showed acceptable fits to the data for both males and females without statistically significant difference between the two models (Δχ2 = 22.71, p = 0.09). This finding indicated that the final model was suitable for both genders.
A statistical difference was found when stratifying the models by age (Δχ2 = 31.73, p = 0.01). We compared pathways in the two models and found that the difference resulted from changes in some confounding pathways. The pathways between the core variables remained statistically significant in both groups, indicating that the main framework could fit both age groups well.

5. Discussion

We examined the potential causal pathways between childhood residential green space and adult mental wellbeing. We found that childhood residential green space promoted childhood nature contact, which contributed to adult nature contact, nature connectedness, and mental wellbeing.

5.1. Role of Childhood Residential Green space on Adult Nature Contact and Nature Connectedness

We found two indirect pathways between childhood residential green space and adult nature contact and nature connectedness. Childhood nature contact was a common mediator in both. These findings support our first hypothesis, indicating a mechanism to understand the long-term impact of childhood residential green space. In previous pathway analysis, children’s experience in nature was found to promote nature connectedness and nature-based recreational activities as an adult [29]. Our findings confirm this pathway and extend it to childhood residential green space, underling the role of childhood residential green space in offering children places for nature contact.
We found several pathways that were not assumed in our hypothesized framework. First, we found direct impacts of childhood residential green space on adult nature contact and nature connectedness. These results are not entirely surprising, however, since residential green space may directly and continuously impact residents by buffering environmental disturbances that can be easily perceived, such as noise and air pollution [11,61]. In addition, residential green space is the type of green space closest to the home. Thereby, some remote exposures, such as viewing green space through windows, may improve mental health [62,63]. These impacts may affect children and change their attitudes towards nature and nature-related activities without physically entering residential green spaces.
Another unexpected finding is the pathway between adult nature contact and adult nature connectedness. Theoretically, a reciprocal relationship may exist between the two variables because nature connectedness is believed to be fostered through nature contact [32]. Meanwhile, nature connectedness may be a motivator for people to engage with nature [29,64]. Based on our cross-sectional design, the time-sequences of the investigated variables, and the results of sensitivity analysis, a pathway from adult nature contact to adult nature connectedness appears more logical. Taken together, our results suggest that childhood and recent nature contact may both contribute to nature connectedness. These findings partially support a previous study, where childhood nature experience promoted adult recreational nature contact and, in turn, enhanced nature connectedness [28]. Our findings also imply a relationship between nature connectedness and lifetime nature-related experiences [65].

5.2. Role of Childhood Residential Green Space on Adult Mental Wellbeing

We found that childhood residential green space had indirect positive impacts on adult mental wellbeing. Adult nature connectedness and adult nature contact were critical mediators for the indirect effects. These results support our second hypothesis. The identified indirect pathways may partially explain previous findings that childhood residential green space may predict mental health conditions later in life [13,14]. The effects of childhood experiences encourage health promoting behaviors and psychosocial characteristics across the life course [15,66,67]. For example, Engemann, et al. [13] speculated that stress-processing abilities developed through exposure to childhood green space could explain their long-term health benefits. Based on our findings, nature connectedness and nature contact may be durative health-related factors that connect childhood and adulthood.
In addition to the observed indirect pathways, a direct pathway was found between residential green space and adult mental wellbeing. Although the effect of this direct pathway was subtle (β = 0.09, p < 0.05), it implies a long-term impact of residential green space across the life course. This may be explained by the potential for stress reduction through green space exposure [68,69]. As persistent stress in childhood may negatively change the nervous system and make individuals vulnerable to illness [67], residential green space may buffer against this harmful process and protect mental health in later life.
It is noteworthy that our study was conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic. In the past two years, the novel virus has threatened physical and mental health worldwide. In this context, nature contact has been advocated as a way to cope with mental health issues [70]. Although social blockades may limit residents’ access to green space [71], it has also been reported that some residents increased green space utilization for recreation [72]. Some previous non-users of urban green space began using these spaces during the pandemic [73]. The conditions in China are currently understudied. Nevertheless, it is reasonable to assume that some people in China may visit green spaces for mental health benefits because evidence suggests the Chinese demonstrated high demand for green spaces during the pandemic [74], and most green spaces were accessible during the period leading up to the current study. Therefore, the connection between adult nature contact and mental wellbeing may have been strengthened during the pandemic, which may have increased the impact of childhood residential green space on adult mental wellbeing.

5.3. Implications and Suggestions

This study underlines the role of residential green space as a source of childhood nature contact and adult nature contact, nature connectedness, and mental wellbeing. There is a rising demand for enabling children to contact nature and build emotional connections with nature [75]. This demand has begun to receive more attention in China, as China has been urbanizing faster than other countries in the past decade [76]. In a previous study, Zhang, et al. [77] recommended that policymakers encourage the public to cultivate emotional engagement with nature and prioritize the improvement of nature connectedness in children. Rosa, et al. [29] concluded that one strategy to promote nature-based recreations is access to safe green spaces near residential areas and workplaces. Based on the information that Chinese children may prefer to perform physical activity in residential green space, the strategy stated by Rosa, et al. [29] seems practical for children in China. Thus, policymakers are recommended to prioritize greening projects in residential areas. To ensure the safety and convenience of children’s activities, specialist recreational facilities in green spaces need more attention.
Moreover, our findings may provide guidance for schools. Children spend more waking hours in school than at home, especially in developing countries such as China. Therefore, the greenness in and around school campuses is essential to provide children with nature contact. The current study focused on children between 5 and 10 years old, which is approximately equivalent to the age range of elementary school. According to a previous study, the years before age 11 may be more critical for fostering nature connectedness, indicating the significance of nature connectedness promotion in elementary school [78]. Dedicated environmental education programming is a popular method to foster nature connectedness in children [78,79,80]. However, it may not be suitable for Chinese children because their schedule is full. Incorporating nature contact into pre-existing school curricula may be more realistic. For example, building a green playground for physical education classes can be helpful. In addition, informing parents of the benefits of nature contact and encouraging outdoor activities in nature during the weekends may help foster nature connectedness.

5.4. Limitations

Our study had several limitations. First, we used self-reported data. Potential reporting bias can not be ruled out. Some people may have been afraid to disclose their poor mental health conditions, so wellbeing measures may have been inflated. In addition, we introduced the research topic when recruiting participants. It is possible that people who cared about urban green spaces responded to the survey. They may have had higher nature connectedness and nature contact than the general public. For these reasons, the effects of the observed pathways may have been overestimated. Our data may not have represented the general public due to its online administration. Future studies should use stratified sampling and offline investigation approaches to help reduce the risk of bias.
We also could not obtain real-time data for childhood variables. Thus, longitudinal studies are warranted. For example, starting an investigation among elementary school students that measures their current residential green space and nature contact and subsequently records their nature connectedness and nature contact every few years would help re-examine the observed pathways more precisely. Additionally, future studies may consider objective historical data, such as using Normalized Difference Vegetation Index (NDVI) values to measure residential green space. Further, some schools have started testing and recording students’ mental health conditions at different ages. These data may be available under a legal procedure to measure changes in mental health over time. Some of our variables were measured with single items, such as childhood residential green space and nature contact. This is because there is no widely accepted questionnaire or method for such variables. Although we tried to follow the measurements in previous research, the reliability of these measurements remains unclear. Future studies may need to address this issue by exploring appropriate dimensions or developing new questionnaires.
Our model was examined using data captured in a cross-sectional investigation. The causal relationships between variables were based on theoretical assumptions drawn from previous literature. Controlled trials are needed to examine the hypothesized causal relationships. In addition, though distinctive pathways were observed in the model, the effects of some indirect pathways were weak. Our model explained a small percentage of the total variation in adult nature contact and adult mental wellbeing. Additional mechanisms need to be investigated. Additional variables that might help to extend the framework include environmental disturbances, social cohesion, and physical activity, which may link residential green spaces with mental wellbeing [11,81,82].
People of different ages from 18 to 68 were involved in the current study. Given the rapid urbanization process in China, the characteristics of childhood residential green spaces may vary by generation. Older generations’ childhood residential green spaces may be wilder, while those of younger participants may be more cultivated or manicured. Thus, the impact of childhood residential green spaces may reflect qualitatively different experiences [83]. In addition, memory may have been a problem. Younger participants could have recalled their childhoods more easily than older participants. Although we conducted a multigroup analysis to test for differences by age, such biases may not have been eliminated completely. Future research should select suitable subjects to control the time intervals between past variables and current reports. Recruiting people of similar ages may be one solution.

6. Conclusions

The current study examined the role of childhood residential green space on adult mental health via multiple mediators. Adult nature contact and nature connectedness played important roles in linking childhood residential green space and adult mental wellbeing. Since childhood residential space can be an important source for children’s nature contact, policymakers are advised to improve residential green space as well as green recreational facilities for children to realize the potential of urban green spaces in health and health-promoting behavior.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/bs12030084/s1, Figure S1: Competing models; Table S1: Model fits of competing models.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, H.L.; investigation; writing—original draft preparation, H.L.; writing—review and editing, H.L. and M.H.E.M.B.; supervision, G.Z.; project administration, G.Z. and Y.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Southwest University.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data is available upon request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to volunteers from Southwest university who helped spread recruiting information.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Ritchie, H.; Roser, M. Urbanization. Our World In Data 2018. Available online: https://ourworldindata.org/urbanization?source=content_type%3Areact%7Cfirst_level_url%3Aarticle%7Csection%3Amain_content%7Cbutton%3Abody_link (accessed on 5 January 2022).
  2. Capaldi, C.A.; Dopko, R.L.; Zelenski, J.M. The relationship between nature connectedness and happiness: A meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 976. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Hartig, T.; Mitchell, R.; De Vries, S.; Frumkin, H. Nature and health. Annu. Rev. Public Health 2014, 35, 207–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Markevych, I.; Schoierer, J.; Hartig, T.; Chudnovsky, A.; Hystad, P.; Dzhambov, A.M.; De Vries, S.; Triguero-Mas, M.; Brauer, M.; Nieuwenhuijsen, M.J. Exploring pathways linking green space to health: Theoretical and methodological guidance. Environ. Res. 2017, 158, 301–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Stanhope, J.; Breed, M.F.; Weinstein, P. Exposure to green spaces could reduce the high global burden of pain. Environ. Res. 2020, 187, 109641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Ekkel, E.D.; de Vries, S. Nearby green space and human health: Evaluating accessibility metrics. Landsc. Urban. Plan. 2017, 157, 214–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Coombes, E.; Jones, A.P.; Hillsdon, M. The relationship of physical activity and overweight to objectively measured green space accessibility and use. Soc. Sci. Med. 2010, 70, 816–822. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Beyer, K.M.; Kaltenbach, A.; Szabo, A.; Bogar, S.; Nieto, F.J.; Malecki, K.M. Exposure to neighborhood green space and mental health: Evidence from the survey of the health of Wisconsin. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2014, 11, 3453–3472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Kothencz, G.; Kolcsár, R.; Cabrera-Barona, P.; Szilassi, P. Urban green space perception and its contribution to well-being. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Yang, M.; Dijst, M.; Faber, J.; Helbich, M. Using structural equation modeling to examine pathways between perceived residential green space and mental health among internal migrants in China. Environ. Res. 2020, 183, 109121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Liu, Y.; Wang, R.; Grekousis, G.; Liu, Y.; Yuan, Y.; Li, Z. Neighbourhood greenness and mental wellbeing in Guangzhou, China: What are the pathways? Landsc. Urban. Plan. 2019, 190, 103602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Dzhambov, A.M.; Lercher, P.; Browning, M.; Stoyanov, D.; Petrova, N.; Novakov, S.; Dimitrova, D.D. Does greenery experienced indoors and outdoors provide an escape and support mental health during the COVID-19 quarantine? Environ. Res. 2021, 196, 110420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Engemann, K.; Pedersen, C.B.; Arge, L.; Tsirogiannis, C.; Mortensen, P.B.; Svenning, J.C. Residential green space in childhood is associated with lower risk of psychiatric disorders from adolescence into adulthood. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2019, 116, 5188–5193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Engemann, K.; Svenning, J.C.; Arge, L.; Brandt, J.; Erikstrup, C.; Geels, C.; Hertel, O.; Mortensen, P.B.; Plana-Ripoll, O.; Tsirogiannis, C.; et al. Associations between growing up in natural environments and subsequent psychiatric disorders in Denmark. Environ. Res. 2020, 188, 109788. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Lee, L.O.; Aldwin, C.M.; Kubzansky, L.D.; Mroczek, D.K.; Spiro, A., III. The long arm of childhood experiences on longevity: Testing midlife vulnerability and resilience pathways. Psychol. Aging 2019, 34, 884. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Chapman, D.P.; Whitfield, C.L.; Felitti, V.J.; Dube, S.R.; Edwards, V.J.; Anda, R.F. Adverse childhood experiences and the risk of depressive disorders in adulthood. J. Affect. Disord. 2004, 82, 217–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Landes, S.D.; Ardelt, M.; Vaillant, G.E.; Waldinger, R.J. Childhood Adversity, Midlife Generativity, and Later Life Well-Being. J. Gerontol. Ser. B 2014, 69, 942–952. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Lercher, P.; Evans, G.W.; Meis, M.; Kofler, W.W. Ambient neighbourhood noise and children’s mental health. Occup. Environ. Med. 2002, 59, 380–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Brewer, M.; Kimbro, R.T. Neighborhood context and immigrant children’s physical activity. Soc. Sci. Med. 2014, 116, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Timperio, A.; Crawford, D.; Telford, A.; Salmon, J. Perceptions about the local neighborhood and walking and cycling among children. Preventive Medicine 2004, 38, 39–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Madzia, J.; Ryan, P.; Yolton, K.; Percy, Z.; Newman, N.; LeMasters, G.; Brokamp, C. Residential Green space Association with Childhood Behavioral Outcomes. J. Pediatrics 2019, 207, 233–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Bijnens, E.M.; Derom, C.; Thiery, E.; Weyers, S.; Nawrot, T.S. Residential green space and child intelligence and behavior across urban, suburban, and rural areas in Belgium: A longitudinal birth cohort study of twins. PLoS Med. 2020, 17, e1003213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Liao, J.; Zhang, B.; Xia, W.; Cao, Z.; Zhang, Y.; Liang, S.; Hu, K.; Xu, S.; Li, Y. Residential exposure to green space and early childhood neurodevelopment. Environ. Int. 2019, 128, 70–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Browning, M.H.E.M.; Rigolon, A.; McAnirlin, O.; Yoon, H. Where green space matters most: A systematic review of urbanicity, green space, and physical health. Landsc. Urban. Plan. 2022, 217, 104233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Baoxin, Z.; Wei, Z. A study on the temporal and spatial characteristics of children’s outdoor activities in big cities—A case study of Shanghai. City Plan. 2018, 42, 87–96. (In Chinese) [Google Scholar]
  26. Thompson, C.W.; Aspinall, P.; Montarzino, A. The childhood factor: Adult visits to green places and the significance of childhood experience. Environ. Behav. 2008, 40, 111–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Holt, E.W.; Lombard, Q.K.; Best, N.; Smiley-Smith, S.; Quinn, J.E. Active and Passive Use of Green Space, Health, and Well-Being amongst University Students. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Rosa, C.D.; Profice, C.C.; Collado, S. Nature Experiences and Adults’ Self-Reported Pro-environmental Behaviors: The Role of Connectedness to Nature and Childhood Nature Experiences. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1055. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Rosa, C.D.; Collado, S.; Profice, C.C.; Larson, L.R. Nature-based recreation associated with connectedness to nature and leisure satisfaction among students in Brazil. Leis. Stud. 2019, 38, 682–691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Houlden, V.; Weich, S.; Porto de Albuquerque, J.; Jarvis, S.; Rees, K. The relationship between green space and the mental wellbeing of adults: A systematic review. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0203000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Pačalat, Ž. Health Benefits of Nature. In Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science; Weekes-Shackelford, V., Shackelford, T.K., Weekes-Shackelford, V.A., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2016; pp. 1–3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Mayer, F.S.; Frantz, C.M. The connectedness to nature scale: A measure of individuals’ feeling in community with nature. J. Environ. Psychol. 2004, 24, 503–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Mayer, F.S.; Frantz, C.M.; Bruehlman-Senecal, E.; Dolliver, K. Why Is Nature Beneficial?: The Role of Connectedness to Nature. Environ. Behav. 2009, 41, 607–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Richardson, M.; McEwan, K. 30 Days Wild and the Relationships Between Engagement With Nature’s Beauty, Nature Connectedness and Well-Being. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Nisbet, E.K.; Zelenski, J.M.; Murphy, S.A. The Nature Relatedness Scale Linking Individuals’ Connection With Nature to Environmental Concern and Behavior. Environ. Behav. 2009, 41, 715–740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Howell, A.J.; Dopko, R.L.; Passmore, H.A.; Buro, K. Nature connectedness: Associations with well-being and mindfulness. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2011, 51, 166–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Richardson, M.; Hamlin, I. Noticing nature, nature connectedness and time in nature: Associations with Human and Nature’s Wellbeing during the Corona Pandemic. J. Public Ment. Health 2021, 20, 83–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Moriki, E.; Petreniti, V.; Marini, K.; Skanavis, C. Connectedness to Nature as a Factor Influencing Well-Being: Implications on Nature-Centered Tourism. In Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Islands Tourism, Macau, China, 3–5 November 2022. [Google Scholar]
  39. Steptoe, A.; Deaton, A.; Stone, A.A. Subjective wellbeing, health, and ageing. Lancet 2015, 385, 640–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Kiely, K.M.; Brady, B.; Byles, J. Gender, mental health and ageing. Maturitas 2019, 129, 76–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Gresenz, C.R.; Sturm, R.; Tang, L. Income and mental health: Unraveling community and individual level relationships. J. Ment. Health Policy Econ. 2001, 4, 197–204. [Google Scholar]
  42. Haluza, D.; Simic, S.; Höltge, J.; Cervinka, R.; Moshammer, H. Connectedness to Nature and Public (Skin) Health Perspectives: Results of a Representative, Population-Based Survey among Austrian Residents. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2014, 11, 1176–1191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Di Fabio, A.; Rosen, M.A. Accounting for Individual Differences in Connectedness to Nature: Personality and Gender Differences. Sustainability 2019, 11, 1693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Bolte, G.; Nanninga, S.; Dandolo, L. Sex/Gender Differences in the Association between Residential Green Space and Self-Rated Health—A Sex/Gender-Focused Systematic Review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 4818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Cherney, I.D.; London, K. Gender-linked Differences in the Toys, Television Shows, Computer Games, and Outdoor Activities of 5- to 13-year-old Children. Sex. Roles 2006, 54, 717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Rigolon, A.; Browning, M.H.; McAnirlin, O.; Yoon, H.V. Green space and health equity: A systematic review on the potential of green space to reduce health disparities. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 2563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Harpham, T. Urbanization and mental health in developing countries: A research role for social scientists, public health professionals and social psychiatrists. Soc. Sci. Med. 1994, 39, 233–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Guo, Z.; Wu, Z.; Schimmele, C.M.; Li, S. The Effect of Urbanization on China’s Fertility. Popul. Res. Policy Rev. 2012, 31, 417–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Wood, C.; Barron, D.; Smyth, N. The Current and Retrospective Intentional Nature Exposure Scales: Development and Factorial Validity. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 4443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  50. Larson, L.R.; Stedman, R.C.; Cooper, C.B.; Decker, D.J. Understanding the multi-dimensional structure of pro-environmental behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 2015, 43, 112–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Pasca, L.; Aragonés, J.; Coello, M.T. An Analysis of the Connectedness to Nature Scale Based on Item Response Theory. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Rosa, C.D.; Collado, S.; Profice, C.C.; Pires, P.P. The 7-items version of the connectedness to nature scale: A study of its validity and reliability with Brazilians. Curr. Psychol. 2020, 26, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Faruk, M.O.; Alam, F.; Chowdhury, K.U.A.; Soron, T.R. Validation of the Bangla WHO-5 Well-being Index. Glob. Ment. Health 2021, 8, e26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Wang, R.; Feng, Z.; Pearce, J.; Liu, Y.; Dong, G. Are green space quantity and quality associated with mental health through different mechanisms in Guangzhou, China: A comparison study using street view data. Environ. Pollut. 2021, 290, 117976. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Bagozzi, R.P.; Yi, Y. Specification, evaluation, and interpretation of structural equation models. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 2012, 40, 8–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Rogerson, P.A. Statistical Methods for Geography: A Student’s Guide; Sage: Newcastle Upon Tyne, UK, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  57. Haukoos, J.S.; Lewis, R.J. Advanced statistics: Bootstrapping confidence intervals for statistics with “difficult” distributions. Acad. Emerg. Med. 2005, 12, 360–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Kelley, K. The Effects of Nonnormal Distributions on Confidence Intervals Around the Standardized Mean Difference: Bootstrap and Parametric Confidence Intervals. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 2005, 65, 51–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Brown, T.A. Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research; Guilford Publications: New York, NY, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  60. Moss, S. Fit Indices for Structural Equation Modeling. 2009. Available online: http://www.psych-it.com.au/Psychlopedia/article.asp (accessed on 12 January 2022).
  61. Reuben, A.; Arseneault, L.; Belsky, D.W.; Caspi, A.; Fisher, H.L.; Houts, R.M.; Moffitt, T.E.; Odgers, C. Residential neighborhood greenery and children’s cognitive development. Soc. Sci. Med. 2019, 230, 271–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Soga, M.; Evans, M.J.; Tsuchiya, K.; Fukano, Y. A room with a green view: The importance of nearby nature for mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic. Ecol. Appl. 2021, 31, e02248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Li, H.; Zhang, X.; You, C.; Chen, X.; Cao, Y.; Zhang, G. Can Viewing Nature Through Windows Improve Isolated Living? A Pathway Analysis on Chinese Male Prisoners During the COVID-19 Epidemic. Front. Psychiatry 2021, 12, 127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Hinds, J.; Sparks, P. Engaging with the natural environment: The role of affective connection and identity. J. Environ. Psychol. 2008, 28, 109–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Wells, N.M.; Lekies, K.S. Nature and the life course: Pathways from childhood nature experiences to adult environmentalism. Child. Youth Environ. 2006, 16, 1–24. [Google Scholar]
  66. Bures, R.M. Childhood Residential Stability and Health at Midlife. Am. J. Public Health 2003, 93, 1144–1148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Chartier, M.J.; Walker, J.R.; Naimark, B. Childhood Abuse, Adult Health, and Health Care Utilization: Results from a Representative Community Sample. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2007, 165, 1031–1038. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  68. Roe, J.; Thompson, C.; Aspinall, P.; Brewer, M.; Du Ff, E.; Miller, D.; Mitchell, R.; Clow, A. Green Space and Stress: Evidence from Cortisol Measures in Deprived Urban Communities. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2013, 10, 4086–4103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  69. Thompson, C.W.; Roe, J.; Aspinall, P.; Mitchell, R.; Clow, A.; Miller, D. More green space is linked to less stress in deprived communities: Evidence from salivary cortisol patterns. Landsc. Urban. Plan. 2012, 105, 221–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Pouso, S.; Borja, Á.; Fleming, L.E.; Gómez-Baggethun, E.; White, M.P.; Uyarra, M.C. Contact with blue-green spaces during the COVID-19 pandemic lockdown beneficial for mental health. Sci. Total Environ. 2021, 756, 143984. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Burnett, H.; Olsen, J.R.; Nicholls, N.; Mitchell, R. Change in time spent visiting and experiences of green space following restrictions on movement during the COVID-19 pandemic: A nationally representative cross-sectional study of UK adults. BMJ Open 2021, 11, e044067. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Venter, Z.S.; Barton, D.N.; Gundersen, V.; Figari, H.; Nowell, M.S. Back to nature: Norwegians sustain increased recreational use of urban green space months after the COVID-19 outbreak. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2021, 214, 104175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Berdejo-Espinola, V.; Suárez-Castro, A.F.; Amano, T.; Fielding, K.S.; Oh, R.R.Y.; Fuller, R.A. Urban green space use during a time of stress: A case study during the COVID-19 pandemic in Brisbane, Australia. People Nat. 2021, 3, 597–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Zhu, J.; Xu, C. Sina microblog sentiment in Beijing city parks as measure of demand for urban green space during the COVID-19. Urban. For. Urban. Green. 2021, 58, 126913. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Taylor, A.; Kuo, M. Is contact with nature important for healthy child development? State of the evidence. Child. Environ. Learn. Using Des. Spaces 2006, 124–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Zhang, W.; Goodale, E.; Chen, J. How contact with nature affects children’s biophilia, biophobia and conservation attitude in China. Biol. Conserv. 2014, 177, 109–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Zhang, J.W.; Howell, R.T.; Iyer, R. Engagement with natural beauty moderates the positive relation between connectedness with nature and psychological well-being. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 38, 55–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Liefländer, A.K.; Fröhlich, G.; Bogner, F.X.; Schultz, P.W. Promoting connectedness with nature through environmental education. Environ. Educ. Res. 2013, 19, 370–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Ernst, J.; Theimer, S. Evaluating the effects of environmental education programming on connectedness to nature. Environ. Educ. Res. 2011, 17, 577–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Otto, S.; Pensini, P. Nature-based environmental education of children: Environmental knowledge and connectedness to nature, together, are related to ecological behaviour. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2017, 47, 88–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Dzhambov, A.; Tilov, B.; Markevych, I.; Dimitrova, D. Residential road traffic noise and general mental health in youth: The role of noise annoyance, neighborhood restorative quality, physical activity, and social cohesion as potential mediators. Environ. Int. 2017, 109, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Wang, R.; Helbich, M.; Yao, Y.; Zhang, J.; Liu, P.; Yuan, Y.; Liu, Y. Urban greenery and mental wellbeing in adults: Cross-sectional mediation analyses on multiple pathways across different greenery measures. Environ. Res. 2019, 176, 108535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  83. Li, H.; Zhang, X.; Bi, S.; Cao, Y.; Zhang, G. Psychological benefits of green exercise in wild or urban green spaces: A meta-analysis of controlled trials. Urban. For. Urban. Green. 2022, 68, 127458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 2. Initial model tested, including control variables.
Figure 2. Initial model tested, including control variables.
Behavsci 12 00084 g002
Figure 3. Final SEM with standardized coefficients (β). Solid lines indicate statistically significant pathways; dotted lines indicate statistically non-significant pathways. *, p < 0.05; **, p < 0.01; ***, p < 0.001. R2, explained variance.
Figure 3. Final SEM with standardized coefficients (β). Solid lines indicate statistically significant pathways; dotted lines indicate statistically non-significant pathways. *, p < 0.05; **, p < 0.01; ***, p < 0.001. R2, explained variance.
Behavsci 12 00084 g003
Table 1. Participant characteristics (n = 752).
Table 1. Participant characteristics (n = 752).
Variable CategoryMean (SD)Range (Min–Max)Percentage (N)
Urbanization rate-64% (0.09)50.05–100%-
Age-31.15 (10.45)18–68-
GenderMale--56.38% (424)
Female--43.62% (328)
IncomeNone--19.41% (146)
0–3000--6.38% (48)
3001–5000--18.35% (138)
5001–7000--15.30% (115)
7001–9000--13.56% (102)
9001–11,000--12.77% (96)
11,001–13,000--4.65% (35)
13,001–15,000--3.72% (28)
More than 15,000--5.85% (44)
Childhood residential green spaceExtremely low--6.10% (47)
Low--20.61% (155)
Moderate--21.41% (161)
High--25.93% (195)
Extremely high--25.80% (194)
Nature connectedness-27.23 (5.58)7–35-
Mental wellbeing-17.94 (5.61)0–25-
Childhood nature contactNever--3.19% (24)
A few days--17.42% (131)
Half of days--25.13% (189)
Most of days--25.39% (191)
Almost every day--28.86% (217)
Adult nature contactNever--1.46% (11)
A few days--26.33% (198)
Half of days--32.18% (242)
Most of days--29.92% (225)
Almost Every day--10.11% (76)
Table 2. Correlations (Spearman and point-biserial) between the measurements.
Table 2. Correlations (Spearman and point-biserial) between the measurements.
Variables123456789
1. Gender1
2. Age0.16 ***1.00
3. Income−0.030.52 ***1.00
4. Urbanization rate−0.010.010.10 ***1.00
5. Childhood green space−0.05−0.030.12 ***−0.041.00
6. Child nature contact−0.010.10 ***0.17 ***−0.060.58 ***1.00
7. Adult nature contact−0.030.22 ***0.24 ***0.08 **0.30 ***0.36 ***1.00
8. Adult nature connectedness0.06 *0.15 ***0.23 ***0.040.32 ***0.32 ***0.48 ***1.00
9. Adult mental wellbeing−0.060.070.16 ***0.060.24 ***0.25 ***0.35 ***0.39 ***1.00
Numbers in cells indicate Spearman’s rho. *, p < 0.1; **, p < 0.05; ***, p < 0.01. Significant correlations show in bold, p < 0.1.
Table 3. Standardized total and indirect effects of childhood residential green space on core variables.
Table 3. Standardized total and indirect effects of childhood residential green space on core variables.
VariablesTotal EffectpIndirect Effect
(95%CI)
p
Childhood nature contact0.55<0.001--
Adult nature contact0.30<0.0010.15<0.001
Adult nature connectedness0.30<0.0010.16<0.001
Adult mental wellbeing0.22<0.0010.12<0.001
Table 4. Indirect pathways between childhood residential green space and adult mental wellbeing.
Table 4. Indirect pathways between childhood residential green space and adult mental wellbeing.
PathwaysEffectp
Childhood green space → adult nature connectedness → adult mental wellbeing0.025<0.001
Childhood green space → adult nature contact → adult mental wellbeing0.033<0.001
Childhood green space → childhood nature contact → adult nature connectedness → mental wellbeing0.0080.033
Childhood green space → childhood nature contact → adult nature contact → mental wellbeing0.031<0.001
Childhood green space → adult nature contact → adult nature connectedness → mental wellbeing0.012<0.001
Childhood green space → childhood nature contact → adult nature contact → adult nature → adult nature connectedness → mental wellbeing0.011<0.001
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Li, H.; Browning, M.H.E.M.; Cao, Y.; Zhang, G. From Childhood Residential Green space to Adult Mental Wellbeing: A Pathway Analysis among Chinese Adults. Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 84. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12030084

AMA Style

Li H, Browning MHEM, Cao Y, Zhang G. From Childhood Residential Green space to Adult Mental Wellbeing: A Pathway Analysis among Chinese Adults. Behavioral Sciences. 2022; 12(3):84. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12030084

Chicago/Turabian Style

Li, Hansen, Matthew H. E. M. Browning, Yang Cao, and Guodong Zhang. 2022. "From Childhood Residential Green space to Adult Mental Wellbeing: A Pathway Analysis among Chinese Adults" Behavioral Sciences 12, no. 3: 84. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12030084

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop