Creativity, Boredom Proneness and Well-Being in the Pandemic
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Questionnaires
2.2. Available Activities Pre-Pandemic
2.3. Creative Behaviours Inventory (CBI)
2.4. Measures of Well-Being
3. Results
4. Discussion
5. Limitations
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- O’Connor, R.C.; Wetherall, K.; Cleare, S.; McClelland, H.; Melson, A.J.; Niedzwiedz, C.L.; Robb, K.A. Mental health and well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic: Longitudinal analyses of adults in the UK COVID-19 Mental Health & Wellbeing study. Br. J. Psychiatry 2020, 218, 326–333. [Google Scholar]
- Lopez, A.; Caffò, A.O.; Tinella, L.; Di Masi, M.N.; Bosco, A. Variations in mindfulness associated with the COVID-19 outbreak: Differential effects on cognitive failures, intrusive thoughts and rumination. Appl. Psychol. Health Well-Being 2021, 13, 761–780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cullen, W.; Gulati, G.; Kelly, B.D. Mental health in the COVID-19 pandemic. QJM Int. J. Med. 2020, 113, 311–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pfefferbaum, B.; North, C.S. Mental health and the COVID-19 pandemic. N. Engl. J. Med. 2020, 383, 510–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boylan, J.; Seli, P.; Scholer, A.A.; Danckert, J. Boredom in the COVID-19 pandemic: Trait boredom proneness, the desire to act, and rule-breaking. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2020, 171, 110387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolff, W.; Martarelli, C.S.; Schüler, J.; Bieleke, M. High boredom proneness and low trait self-control impair adherence to social distancing guidelines during the COVID-19 pandemic. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 5420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- DeYoung, C.G.; Krueger, R.F. A Cybernetic Theory of Psychopathology. Psychol. Inq. 2018, 29, 117–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jansen, E.J.; Danckert, J.; Seli, P.; Scholer, A. Under pressure: Locomotion and assessment in the COVID-19 pandemic. Self Identity 2022, in press. [CrossRef]
- Strauman, T.J. Self-regulation and psychopathology: Toward an integrative translational research paradigm. Ann. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2017, 13, 497–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Easterbrook-Smith, G. By bread alone: Baking as leisure, performance, sustenance, during the COVID-19 crisis. Leis. Sci. 2020, 43, 36–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nadhifah, U.N.; Puspitasari, D. Learning English Through Duolingo: Narrating Students’ Experience During COVID-19 Pandemic Time. Ethical Ling. J. Lang. Teach. Lit. 2021, 8, 302–312. [Google Scholar]
- Whitehead, B.R.; Torossian, E. Older adults’ experience of the COVID-19 pandemic: A mixed-methods analysis of stresses and joys. Gerontologist 2021, 61, 36–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Spano, G.; D’Este, M.; Giannico, V.; Elia, M.; Cassibba, R.; Lafortezza, R.; Sanesi, G. Association between indoor-outdoor green features and psychological health during the COVID-19 lockdown in Italy: A cross-sectional nationwide study. Urban For. Urban Green. 2021, 62, 127156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- VandenBos, G.R. APA Dictionary of Psychology; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Martin, L.; Wilson, N. Defining creativity with discovery. Creat. Res. J. 2017, 29, 417–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cropley, A.J. Creativity and mental health in everyday life. Creat. Res. J. 1990, 3, 167–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garfield, S.J.; Cohen, H.A.; Roth, R.M. Creativity and mental health. J. Edu. Res. 1969, 63, 147–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leckey, J. The therapeutic effectiveness of creative activities on mental well-being: A systematic review of the literature. J. Psychiatr. Ment. Health Nurs. 2011, 18, 501–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cropley, A.J.; Sikand, J.S. Creativity and schizophrenia. J. Consult Clin. Psych. 1973, 40, 462–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Montes-Santiago, J. The lead-poisoned genius: Saturnism in famous artists across five centuries. Prog. Brain Res. 2013, 203, 223–240. [Google Scholar]
- Post, F. Creativity and psychopathology a study of 291 world-famous men. Br. J. Psychiatry 1994, 165, 22–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Acar, S.; Chen, X.; Cayirdag, N. Schizophrenia and creativity: A meta-analytic review. Schizophr. Res. 2021, 195, 23–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Richards, R. Creativity and the schizophrenia spectrum: More and more interesting. Creat. Res. J. 2001, 13, 111–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dalley, T. Art as Therapy: An Introduction to the Use of Art as a Therapeutic Technique; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Nichols, J.G. Creativity in the person who will never produce anything original and useful: The concept of creativity as a normally distributed trait. Am. Psychol. 1972, 27, 717–727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Secker, J.; Heydinrych, K.; Kent, L.; Keay, J. Why art? Exploring the contribution to well-being of the creative aspects and processes of visual art-making in an arts and mental health course. Arts Health 2018, 10, 72–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Conner, T.S.; DeYoung, C.G.; Silvia, P.J. Everyday creative activity as a path to flourishing. J. Posit. Psychol. 2018, 13, 181–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mercier, M.; Vinchon, F.; Pichot, N.; Bonetto, E.; Bonnardel, N.; Girandola, F.; Lubart, T. COVID-19: A Boon or a Bane for Creativity? Front. Psychol. 2021, 11, 3916. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Karwowski, M.; Zielińska, A.; Jankowska, D.M.; Strutyńska, E.; Omelańczuk, I.; Lebuda, I. Creative lockdown? A daily diary study of creative activity during pandemics. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhang, Z.; Mai, Y. WebPower: Basic and Advanced Statistical Power Analysis; (R Package Version 0.6) [Computer Software]. 2021. Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=WebPower (accessed on 9 June 2021).
- Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Dollinger, S.J. Need for uniqueness, need for cognition, and creativity. J. Creat. Behav. 2003, 37, 99–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silvia, P.; Rodriguez, R.; Kaufman, J.C.; Reiter-Palmon, R.; Puryear, J.S. Taking Inventory of the Creative Behavior Inventory: An Item Response Theory Analysis of the CBI. PsyArXiv 2021. Available online: https://psyarxiv.com/b7cfd/ (accessed on 9 June 2021).
- Watson, D.; Clark, L.A. The PANAS-X: Manual for the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule-Expanded Form. 1999. Available online: https://www2.psychology.uiowa.edu/faculty/clark/panas-x.pdf (accessed on 9 June 2021).
- Henry, J.D.; Crawford, J.R. The short-form version of the Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale (DASS-21): Construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. Br. J. Psychiatry 2005, 44, 227–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lovibond, P.F.; Lovibond, S.H. The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behav. Res. Ther. 1995, 33, 335–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosenberg, M. Rosenberg self-esteem scale (RSE). Accept. Commit. Ther. Meas. Package 1965, 61, 18. [Google Scholar]
- Paradise, A.W.; Kernis, M.H. Self-esteem and psychological well-being: Implications of fragile self-esteem. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 2002, 21, 345–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Glaesmer, H.; Rief, W.; Martin, A.; Mewes, R.; Brähler, E.; Zenger, M.; Hinz, A. Psychometric properties and population-based norms of the Life Orientation Test Revised (LOT-R). Br. J. Health Psychol. 2012, 17, 432–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Conversano, C.; Rotondo, A.; Lensi, E.; Della Vista, O.; Arpone, F.; Reda, M.A. Optimism and its impact on mental and physical well-being. Clin. Prac. Epidem. Ment. Health 2010, 6, 25–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brosowsky, N.; van Tilburg, W.A.; Scholer, A.; Boylan, J.; Seli, P.; Danckert, J. Boredom proneness, political orientation and adherence to social-distancing in the pandemic. Motiv. Emot. 2021, 45, 631–640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Larson, R.W. Emotions and the creative process; anxiety, boredom, and enjoyment as predictors of creative writing. Imagin. Cogn. Personal. 1990, 9, 275–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mann, S.; Cadman, R. Does being bored make us more creative? Creat. Res. J. 2014, 26, 165–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Danckert, J.; Mugon, J.; Struk, A.; Eastwood, J. Boredom: What is it good for? In The Function of Emotions; Lench, H.C., Ed.; Springer International Publishing: London, UK, 2018; pp. 93–119. [Google Scholar]
- Struk, A.A.; Carriere, J.S.; Cheyne, J.A.; Danckert, J. A short boredom proneness scale: Development and psychometric properties. Assessment 2017, 24, 346–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tam, K.Y.; Van Tilburg, W.A.; Chan, C.S. What is boredom proneness? A comparison of three characterizations. J. Personal. 2021, 89, 831–846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mugon, J.; Struk, A.; Danckert, J. A failure to launch: Regulatory modes and boredom proneness. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eastwood, J.D.; Frischen, A.; Fenske, M.J.; Smilek, D. The unengaged mind: Defining boredom in terms of attention. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 7, 482–495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Isacescu, J.; Struk, A.A.; Danckert, J. Cognitive and affective predictors of boredom proneness. Cogn. Emot. 2017, 31, 1741–1748. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Acar, S.; Tadik, H.; Myers, D.; Van der Sman, C.; Uysal, R. Creativity and Well-being: A Meta-analysis. J. Creat. Behav. 2020, 55, 738–751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Amabile, T.M. How to kill creativity. Harv. Bus. Rev. 1998, 76, 76–87. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Li, H.; Li, F.; Chen, T. A motivational–cognitive model of creativity and the role of autonomy. J. Bus. Res. 2018, 92, 179–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slabbert, J.A. Creativity in Education Revisited: Reflection in Aid of Progression. J. Creat. Behav. 1994, 28, 60–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kukita, A.; Nakamura, J.; Csikszentmihalyi, M. How experiencing autonomy contributes to a good life. J. Posit. Psychol. 2020, 17, 34–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Danckert, J.; Eastwood, J.D. Out of My Skull: The Psychology of Boredom; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Ryan, R.M.; Deci, E.L. Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: Classic definitions and new directions. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2000, 25, 54–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Csikszentmihalyi, M. Creativity: The psychology of Discovery and Invention; Harper Perennial: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Kapoor, H.; Kaufman, J.C. Meaning-making through creativity during COVID-19. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 3659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tang, M.; Hofreiter, S.; Reiter-Palmon, R.; Bai, X.; Murugavel, V. Creativity as a means to well-being in times of COVID-19 pandemic: Results of a cross-cultural study. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Germani, A.; Buratta, L.; Delvecchio, E.; Mazzeschi, C. Emerging adults and COVID-19: The role of individualism-collectivism on perceived risks and psychological maladjustment. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Smallwood, J.; Schooler, J.W. The science of mind wandering: Empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 2015, 66, 487–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dahlberg, L. Loneliness during the COVID-19 pandemic. Aging Ment. Health 2021, 25, 1161–1164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lefdahl-Davis, E.M.; Stefan, D.; Huffman, L.; Alayan, A. Positive psychology during a pandemic: REFRAME for well-being. Eur. J. Appl. Posit. Psychol. 2020, 4, 2397–7116. [Google Scholar]
- Waters, L.; Algoe, S.B.; Dutton, J.; Emmons, R.; Fredrickson, B.L.; Heaphy, E.; Steger, M. Positive psychology in a pandemic: Buffering, bolstering, and building mental health. J. Posit. Psychol. 2021, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yamaguchi, K.; Takebayashi, Y.; Miyamae, M.; Komazawa, A.; Yokoyama, C.; Ito, M. Role of focusing on the positive side during COVID-19 outbreak: Mental health perspective from positive psychology. Psychol. Trauma Theory Res. Pract. Policy 2020, 12, S49–S50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Measure | Mean | SD | Skew | Kurtosis | Cronbach’s Alpha |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Creative Activities | 0.19 | 0.26 | 0.21 | 3.18 | - |
Availability of All Activities Pre-COVID | 62.31 | 26.54 | −0.57 | 2.37 | 0.96 |
Rule Breaking | 0.01 | 1.00 | 2.12 | 7.24 | - |
Short Boredom Proneness Scale | 3.52 | 1.52 | 0.11 | 2.06 | 0.92 |
Positive and Negative Affect Schedule: Positive | 29.46 | 8.96 | 0.16 | 2.36 | 0.91 |
Positive and Negative Affect Schedule: Negative | 19.32 | 9.42 | 1.10 | 3.26 | 0.94 |
Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale-21 | 35.39 | 15.02 | 0.97 | 2.83 | 0.97 |
Life Orientation Test-revised | 20.63 | 6.62 | −0.40 | 2.31 | 0.92 |
Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale | 6.52 | 1.88 | −0.57 | 2.53 | 0.94 |
2. | 3. | 4. | 5. | 6. | 7. | 8. | 9. | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Creative Activities | 0.04 | −0.13 * | −0.17 * | 0.12 * | −0.16 * | −0.22 * | 0.13 * | 0.17 * |
2. Availability of All Activities Pre-COVID | −0.02 | −0.08 * | 0.14 * | −0.04 | −0.08 * | 0.12 * | 0.14 * | |
3. Rule Breaking | 0.36 * | 0.28 * | 0.55 * | 0.6 * | −0.14 * | −0.29 * | ||
4. Short Boredom Proneness Scale | −0.24 * | 0.58 * | 0.63 * | −0.43 * | −0.52 * | |||
5. Positive and Negative Affect Schedule: Positive | −0.01 | −0.03 | 0.39 * | 0.35 * | ||||
6. Positive and Negative Affect Schedule: Negative | 0.85 * | −0.38 * | −0.53 * | |||||
7. Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale−21 | −0.45 * | −0.64 * | ||||||
8. Life Orientation Test-revised | 0.70 * | |||||||
9. Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale |
| |||||
Predictors | Beta | 95% CI | t-value | p-value | |
(Intercept) | 0.48 | 0.19–0.76 | 3.28 | 0.001 ** | |
Age | −0.01 | −0.02–0.00 | −3.10 | 0.002 ** | |
Gender | −0.16 | −0.22–−0.09 | −4.62 | <0.001 *** | |
Availability of All Activities Pre-COVID | 0.00 | 0.00–0.00 | −0.47 | 0.638 | |
Creative Activities | −0.55 | −0.79–−0.30 | −4.38 | <0.001 *** | |
R2/R2 adjusted | 0.059/0.054 | ||||
| |||||
Predictors | Beta | 95% CI | t-value | p-value | |
(Intercept) | 5.25 | 4.83–5.68 | 24.28 | <0.001 *** | |
Age | −0.03 | −0.04–−0.02 | −7.38 | <0.001 *** | |
Gender | −0.12 | −0.22–−0.03 | −2.50 | 0.013 * | |
Availability of All Activities Pre-COVID | 0.00 | −0.01–0.00 | −2.59 | 0.01 ** | |
Creative Activities | −1.05 | −1.41–−0.69 | −5.68 | <0.001 *** | |
R2/R2 adjusted | 0.107/0.103 | ||||
| |||||
Predictors | Beta | 95% CI | t-value | p-value | |
(Intercept) | 22.36 | 19.78–24.94 | 17.01 | <0.001 *** | |
Age | 0.09 | 0.03–0.14 | 3.24 | 0.001 ** | |
Gender | −0.67 | −1.26–−0.07 | −2.20 | 0.028 * | |
Availability of All Activities Pre-COVID | 0.05 | 0.02–0.07 | 4.16 | <0.001 *** | |
Creative Activities | 4.08 | 1.87–6.28 | 3.62 | <0.001 *** | |
R2/R2 adjusted | 0.047/0.043 | ||||
| |||||
Predictors | Beta | 95% CI | t-value | p-value | |
(Intercept) | 26.38 | 23.68–29.08 | 19.17 | <0.001 *** | |
Age | −0.13 | −0.19–−0.08 | −4.81 | <0.001 *** | |
Gender | −0.44 | −1.06–0.19 | −1.37 | 0.17 | |
Availability of All Activities Pre-COVID | −0.01 | −0.04–0.01 | −1.10 | 0.272 | |
Creative Activities | −5.94 | −8.26–−3.63 | −5.05 | <0.001 *** | |
R2/R2 adjusted | 0.057/0.053 | ||||
| |||||
Predictors | Beta | 95% CI | t-value | p-value | |
(Intercept) | 50.81 | 46.61–55.02 | 23.72 | <0.001 *** | |
Age | −0.27 | −0.35–−0.18 | −6.13 | <0.001 *** | |
Gender | −0.87 | −1.84–−0.1 | −1.75 | 0.08 | |
Availability of All Activities Pre-COVID | −0.04 | −0.08–−0.01 | −2.46 | 0.014 * | |
Creative Activities | −12.97 | −16.57–−9.37 | −7.08 | <0.001 *** | |
R2/R2 adjusted | 0.101/0.097 | ||||
| |||||
Predictors | Beta | 95% CI | t-value | p-value | |
(Intercept) | 14.00 | 12.12–15.89 | 14.56 | <0.001 *** | |
Age | 0.11 | 0.07–0.14 | 5.41 | <0.001 *** | |
Gender | 0.20 | −0.23–0.64 | 0.92 | 0.357 | |
Availability of All Activities Pre-COVID | 0.03 | 0.01–0.05 | 3.79 | <0.001 *** | |
Creative Activities | 3.44 | 1.83–5.06 | 4.18 | <0.001 *** | |
R2/R2 adjusted | 0.067/0.063 | ||||
| |||||
Predictors | Beta | 95% CI | t-value | p-value | |
(Intercept) | 4.43 | 3.90–4.96 | 16.38 | <0.001 *** | |
Age | 0.03 | 0.02–0.04 | 5.95 | <0.001 *** | |
Gender | 0.08 | 0.04–0.21 | 1.36 | 0.175 | |
Availability of All Activities Pre-COVID | 0.01 | 0.01–0.01 | 4.24 | <0.001 *** | |
Creative Activities | 1.23 | 0.78–1.69 | 5.32 | <0.001 *** | |
R2/R2 adjusted | 0.089/0.084 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Brosowsky, N.P.; Barr, N.; Mugon, J.; Scholer, A.A.; Seli, P.; Danckert, J. Creativity, Boredom Proneness and Well-Being in the Pandemic. Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 68. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12030068
Brosowsky NP, Barr N, Mugon J, Scholer AA, Seli P, Danckert J. Creativity, Boredom Proneness and Well-Being in the Pandemic. Behavioral Sciences. 2022; 12(3):68. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12030068
Chicago/Turabian StyleBrosowsky, Nicholaus P., Nathaniel Barr, Jhotisha Mugon, Abigail A. Scholer, Paul Seli, and James Danckert. 2022. "Creativity, Boredom Proneness and Well-Being in the Pandemic" Behavioral Sciences 12, no. 3: 68. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12030068
APA StyleBrosowsky, N. P., Barr, N., Mugon, J., Scholer, A. A., Seli, P., & Danckert, J. (2022). Creativity, Boredom Proneness and Well-Being in the Pandemic. Behavioral Sciences, 12(3), 68. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12030068