Anxiety-Related Attention Bias in Four- to Eight-Year-Olds: An Eye-Tracking Study
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Measures: Parents
2.2.1. Childhood Anxiety; Spence Child Anxiety Scale (SCAS) and Preschool Anxiety Scale (PAS) and Child Version; (SCAS)
2.2.2. Child Behaviour Questionnaire—Effortful Control Scale (CBQ-EFC)
2.2.3. The Autism Spectrum Quotient: Children’s Version (AQ: Child; Auyeung, Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright and Allison, 2008)
2.3. Measures: Children
2.3.1. The Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI-IV)
2.3.2. Attention Bias Task
2.4. Procedure
2.5. Recording and Preprocessing of Eye-Tracking Data
2.6. Statistical Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Differences Between High- and Low-Anxious Groups
3.2. Repeated-Measures Analysis
3.2.1. Proportion of Trials Where the Initial Look to Faces Was to the Emotional Face
3.2.2. Proportion of Time Spent Looking at the Faces During Initial Looks
3.3. Growth Curve Analyses: Initial Looks to Faces
3.3.1. Data Preparation
3.3.2. Model 1: Influence of Anxiety Level on Attention Bias
3.3.3. Model 2: Moderation of the Relationship Between Anxiety and Attention Bias by Age
3.3.4. Model 3: Do Age Effects Remain When Controlling for Effortful Control?
3.3.5. Model 4: Influence of Verbal and Nonverbal Cognitive Abilities on Initial Looks to Faces
4. Discussion
4.1. Implications for Treatment
4.2. Strengths and Limitations
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
df | F | p | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Anxiety Differences | ||||
Anxiety | 1 | 0.002 | 0 | 0.968 |
Emotion type | 1 | 0.002 | 0 | 0.966 |
Autistic Quotient | 1 | 1.56 | 0.02 | 0.215 |
Anxiety × Emotion type | 1 | 0.001 | 0 | 0.999 |
Moderation by Age | ||||
Anxiety | 1 | 1.01 | 0.05 | 0.325 |
Autistic Quotient | 1 | 2.5 | 0.11 | 0.128 |
Age | 40 | 1.72 | 0.77 | 0.091 |
Anxiety × Emotion type | 1 | 1.59 | 0.07 | 0.221 |
Anxiety × Age | 10 | 0.35 | 0.14 | 0.956 |
Emotion type × Age | 40 | 1.79 | 0.77 | 0.078 |
Anxiety × Age × Emotion type | 10 | 0.20 | 0.09 | 0.993 |
Influence by Effortful Control | ||||
Anxiety | 1 | 0.49 | 0.002 | 0.829 |
Emotion type | 1 | 2.04 | 0.17 | 0.183 |
Autistic Quotient | 1 | 1.85 | 0.16 | 0.160 |
Effortful Control | 1 | 3.15 | 0.14 | 0.091 |
Age | 40 | 1.85 | 0.79 | 0.071 |
Anxiety × Emotion type | 1 | 1.46 | 0.07 | 0.241 |
Anxiety × Age | 10 | 0.54 | 0.21 | 0.845 |
Emotion type × Age | 40 | 1.70 | 0.77 | 0.101 |
Anxiety × Age × Emotion type | 10 | 0.20 | 0.09 | 0.994 |
df | F | p | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Anxiety Differences | ||||
Anxiety | 1 | 0.09 | 0.001 | 0.768 |
Emotion type | 1 | 1.85 | 0 | 0.966 |
Face Type | 1 | 2.03 | 0.03 | 0.158 |
Autistic Quotient | 1 | 0.74 | 0.01 | 0.394 |
Anxiety × Emotion type | 1 | 0.19 | 0.003 | 0.668 |
Anxiety × Face type | 1 | 0.03 | 0 | 0.873 |
Emotion type × Face type | 1 | 1.16 | 0.02 | 0.285 |
Moderation by Age | ||||
Anxiety | 1 | 1.32 | 0.06 | 0.262 |
Emotion type | 1 | 13.45 | 0.39 | 0.001 *** |
Face type | 1 | 0.95 | 0.04 | 00.341 |
Autistic Quotient | 1 | 0.001 | 0.986 | 0.001 |
Age | 40 | 2.77 | 0.84 | 0.007 ** |
Anxiety × Emotion type | 1 | 0.95 | 0.04 | 0.341 |
Anxiety × Age | 10 | 0.66 | 0.24 | 0.746 |
Anxiety × Face type | 1 | 0.21 | 0.01 | 0.655 |
Emotion type × Age | 40 | 1.65 | 0.78 | 0.341 |
Emotion type × Face type | ||||
Age × Face type | 40 | 2.64 | 0.83 | 0.01 ** |
Anxiety × Age × Emotion type | 10 | 2.06 | 0.50 | 0.078 |
Anxiety × Age × Face type | 10 | 0.40 | 0.16 | 0.933 |
Age x Emotion type × Face type | 40 | 2.10 | 0.80 | 0.039 * |
Anxiety × Age × Emotion type × Face type | 10 | 0.57 | 0.21 | 0.822 |
Influence by Effortful Control | ||||
Anxiety | 1 | 1.35 | 0.06 | 0.257 |
Emotion type | 1 | 2.36 | 0.10 | 0.139 |
Face type | 1 | 0.04 | 0.002 | 0.840 |
Autistic Quotient | 1 | 1.40 | 0.06 | 0.250 |
Effortful control | 1 | 1.79 | 0.08 | 0.194 |
Age | 41 | 2.26 | 0.81 | 0.022 * |
Anxiety × Emotion type | 1 | 0.68 | 0.03 | 0.419 |
Anxiety × Age | 11 | 1.08 | 0.35 | 0.421 |
Anxiety × Face type | 1 | 0.16 | 0.01 | 0.695 |
Emotion type × Age | 41 | 2.68 | 0.83 | 0.008 * |
Emotion type × Face type | 1 | 1.33 | 0.06 | 0.260 |
Age × Face type | 41 | 1.94 | 0.78 | 0.049 * |
Anxiety × Age × Emotion type | 11 | 1.36 | 0.41 | 0.259 |
Anxiety × Age × Face type | 11 | 0.58 | 0.22 | 0.828 |
Anxiety × Emotion type × Face type | 1 | 0.64 | 0.03 | 0.431 |
Age × Emotion type × Face type | 41 | 0.95 | 0.64 | 0.566 |
Anxiety × Age × Emotion type × Face type | 11 | 0.16 | 0.07 | 0.998 |
References
- Polanczyk, G.V.; Salum, G.A.; Sugaya, L.S.; Caye, A.; Rohde, L.A. Annual Research Review: A Meta-Analysis of the Worldwide Prevalence of Mental Disorders in Children and Adolescents. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2015, 56, 345–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Copeland, W.; Shanahan, L.; Costello, E.J.; Angold, A. Cumulative Prevalence of Psychiatric Disorders by Young Adulthood: A Prospective Cohort Analysis from the Great Smoky Mountains Study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2011, 50, 252–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Van Bockstaele, B.; Verschuere, B.; Tibboel, H.; De Houwer, J. A Review of Current Evidence for the Causal Impact of Attentional Bias on Fear. Psychol. Bull. 2013, 140, 682–721. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kendall, P. Toward a Cognitive-Behavioral Model of Child Psychopathology and a Critique of Related Interventions. Int. Soc. Res. Child. Adolesc. Psychopathol. 1985, 13, 357–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bar-Haim, Y.; Lamy, D.; Pergamin, L.; Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J.; van Ijzendoom, M.H. Threat-Related Attentional Bias in Anxious and Nonanxious Individuals: A Meta-Analytic Study. Psychol. Bull. 2007, 133, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dudeney, J.; Sharpe, L.; Hunt, C. Attentional Bias Towards Threatening Stimuli in Children with Anxiety: A Meta-Analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2015, 40, 66–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mueller, S.C.; Hardin, M.G.; Mogg, K.; Benson, V.; Bradley, B.P.; Reinholdt-Dunne, M.L.; Liversedge, S.P.; Pine, D.S.; Ernst, M. The Influence of Emotional Stimuli on Attention Orienting and Inhibitory Control in Pediatric Anxiety. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2012, 53, 856–863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Susa, G.; Pitică, I.; Benga, O.; Miclea, M. The Self Regulatory Effect of Attentional Control in Modulating the Relationship between Attentional Biases toward Threat and Anxiety Symptoms in Children. Cogn. Emot. 2012, 26, 1069–1083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Waters, A.M.; Kokkoris, L.L.; Mogg, K.; Bradley, B.P.; Pine, D.S. The Time Course of Attentional Bias for Emotional Faces in Anxious Children. Cogn. Emot. 2010, 24, 1173–1181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, H.M.; McAdams, T.A.; Lester, K.J.; Goodman, R.; Clark, D.M.; Eley, T.C. Attentional Threat Avoidance and Familial Risk Are Independently Associated with Childhood Anxiety Disorders. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2013, 54, 678–685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stirling, L.J.; Eley, T.C.; Clark, D.M. Preliminary Evidence for an Association between Social Anxiety Symptoms and Avoidance of Negative Faces in School-Age Children. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2006, 35, 431–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mogg, K.; Bradley, B.P. A Cognitive-Motivational Analysis of Anxiety. Behav. Res. Ther. 1998, 36, 809–848. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mogg, K.; Bradley, B.P. Time Course of Attentional Bias for Fear-Relevant Pictures in Spider-Fearful Individuals. Behav. Res. Ther. 2006, 44, 1241–1250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Garner, M.; Mogg, K.; Bradley, B.P. Orienting and Maintenance of Gaze to Facial Expressions in Social Anxiety. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2006, 115, 760–770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rinck, M.; Becker, E.S. Spider Fearful Individuals Attend to Threat, Then Quickly Avoid It: Evidence from Eye Movements. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2006, 115, 231–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- In-Albon, T.; Kossowsky, J.; Schneider, S. Vigilance and Avoidance of Threat in the Eye Movements of Children with Separation Anxiety Disorder. Int. Soc. Res. Child. Adolesc. Psychopathol. 2010, 38, 225–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Field, A.; Lester, K. Is There Room for ‘Development’ in Developmental Models of Information Processing Biases to Threat in Children and Adolescents? Clin. Child. Fam. Psychol. Rev. 2010, 13, 315–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benoit, K.E.; McNally, R.J.; Rapee, R.M.; Gamble, A.L.; Wiseman, A.L. Processing of Emotional Faces in Children and Adolescents with Anxiety Disorders. Behav. Chang. 2007, 24, 183–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gamble, A.L.; Rapee, R.M. The Time-Course of Attentional Bias in Anxious Children and Adolescents. J. Anxiety Disord. 2009, 23, 841–847. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eldar, S.; Apter, A.; Lotan, D.; Edgar, K.P.; Naim, R.; Fox, N.A.; Pine, D.S.; Bar-Haim, Y. Attention Bias Modification Treatment for Pediatric Anxiety Disorders: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Am. J. Psychiatry 2012, 169, 213–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Shechner, T.; Jarcho, J.M.; Wong, S.; Leibenluft, E.; Pine, D.S.; Nelson, E.E. Threats, Rewards, and Attention Deployment in Anxious Youth and Adults: An Eye Tracking Study. Biol. Psychol. 2017, 122, 121–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Rothbart, M.K.; Rueda, M.R. The Development of Effortful Control. In Decade of Behavior. Developing Individuality in the Human Brain: A tribute to Michael I. Posner; Mayr, U., Awh, E., Keele, S.W., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2005; pp. 167–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jones, L.B.; Rothbart, M.K.; Posner, M.I. Development of Executive Attention in Preschool Children. Dev. Sci. 2003, 6, 498–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mezzacappa, E. Alerting, Orienting, and Executive Attention: Developmental Properties and Sociodemographic Correlates in an Epidemiological Sample of Young, Urban Children. Child. Dev. 2004, 75, 1373–1386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Knudsen, E.I. Fundamental Components of Attention. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2007, 30, 57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kindt, M.; Van Den Hout, M. Selective Attention and Anxiety: A Perspective on Developmental Issues and the Causal Status. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 2001, 23, 193–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kindt, M.; van den Hout, M.; de Jong, P.; Hoekzema, B. Cognitive Bias for Pictorial and Linguistic Threat Cues in Children. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 2000, 22, 201–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morren, M.; Kindt, M.; van Den Hout, M.; van Kasteren, H. Anxiety and the Processing of Threat in Children: Further Examination of the Cognitive Inhibition Hypothesis. Behav. Chang. 2003, 20, 131–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, H.M.; Eley, T.C.; Broeren, S.; Macleod, C.; Rinck, M.; Hadwin, J.A.; Lester, K.J. Psychometric Properties of Reaction Time Based Experimental Paradigms Measuring Anxiety-Related Information-Processing Biases in Children. J. Anxiety Disord. 2014, 28, 97–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Van Damme, S.; Crombez, G. Measuring Attentional Bias to Threat in Children and Adolescents: A Matter of Speed? J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 2009, 40, 344–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Ruiter, C.; Brosschot, J.F. The Emotional Stroop Interference Effect in Anxiety: Attentional Bias or Cognitive Avoidance? Behav. Res. Ther. 1994, 32, 315–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clarke, P.J.F.; Macleod, C.; Guastella, A.J. Assessing the Role of Spatial Engagement and Disengagement of Attention in Anxiety-Linked Attentional Bias: A Critique of Current Paradigms and Suggestions for Future Research Directions. Anxiety Stress Coping 2013, 26, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Price, S.D.; Salekin, R.T.; Klinger, M.R.; Barker, E.D. Psychopathy and Depression as Predictors of Psychosocial Difficulties in a Sample of Court Evaluated Adolescents. Personal. Disord. 2013, 4, 261–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dodd, H.F.; Hudson, J.L.; Williams, T.; Morris, T.; Lazarus, R.S.; Byrow, Y. Anxiety and Attentional Bias in Preschool-Aged Children: An Eyetracking Study. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2015, 43, 1055–1065. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lagattuta, K.H.; Kramer, H.J. Try to Look on the Bright Side: Children and Adults Can (Sometimes) Override Their Tendency to Prioritize Negative Faces. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2017, 146, 89–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mirman, D.; Dixon, J.A.; Magnuson, J.S. Statistical and Computational Models of the Visual World Paradigm: Growth Curves and Individual Differences. J. Mem. Lang. 2008, 59, 475–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Byrow, Y.; Chen, N.T.M.; Peters, L. Time Course of Attention in Socially Anxious Individuals: Investigating the Effects of Adult Attachment Style. Behav. Ther. 2016, 47, 560–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mahr, T.; McMillan, B.T.M.; Saffran, J.R.; Weismer, S.E.; Edwards, J. Anticipatory Coarticulation Facilitates Word Recognition in Toddlers. Cognition 2015, 142, 345–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Spence, S.H.; Rapee, R.; McDonald, C.; Ingram, M. The Structure of Anxiety Symptoms among Preschoolers. Behav. Res. Ther. 2001, 39, 1293–1316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nauta, M.H.; Scholing, A.; Rapee, R.M.; Abbott, M.; Spence, S.H.; Waters, A. A Parent-Report Measure of Children’s Anxiety: Psychometric Properties and Comparison with Child-Report in a Clinic and Normal Sample. Behav. Res. Ther. 2004, 42, 813–839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eisenberg, N.; Michalik, N.; Spinrad, T.L.; Hofer, C.; Kupfer, A.; Valiente, C.; Liew, J.; Cumberland, A.; Reiser, M. The Relations of Effortful Control and Impulsivity to Children’s Sympathy: A Longitudinal Study. Cogn. Dev. 2007, 22, 544–567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rothbart, M.K.; Ahadi, S.A.; Hershey, K.L.; Fisher, P. Investigations of Temperament at Three to Seven Years: The Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Child. Dev. 2001, 72, 1394–1408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Auyeung, B.; Baron-Cohen, S.; Wheelwright, S.; Allison, C. The Autism Spectrum Quotient: Children’s Version (AQ-Child). J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2008, 38, 1230–1240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Weiss, L.G.; Keith, T.Z.; Zhu, J.; Chen, H. WISC-IV and Clinical Validation of the Four- and Five-Factor Interpretative Approaches. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 2013, 31, 114–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bernier, A.; Beauchamp, M.H.; Bouvette-Turcot, A.A.; Carlson, S.M.; Carrier, J. Sleep and Cognition in Preschool Years: Specific Links to Executive Functioning. Child. Dev. 2013, 84, 1542–1553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dethorne, L.S.; Schaefer, B.A. A Guide to Child Nonverbal IQ Measures. Am. J. Speech-Lang. Pathol. 2004, 13, 275–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Langner, O.; Dotsch, R.; Bijlstra, G.; Wigboldus, D.H.J.; Hawk, S.T.; van Knippenberg, A. Presentation and Validation of the Radboud Faces Database. Cogn. Emot. 2010, 24, 1377–1388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lang, P.J.; Bradley, M.M.; Cuthbert, B.N. International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Technical Manual and Affective Ratings; University of Florida, Center for Research in Psychophysiology: Gainesville, FL, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Stuijfzand, S.; Chakrabarti, B.; Reynolds, S.; Dodd, H.F. Look Out Captain, I Hear an Ambiguous Alien! A Study of Interpretation Bias and Anxiety in Young Children. Behav. Res. Ther. 2019, 121, 103450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Psychology Software Tools. Inc. Eprime 2.0. Pittsburgh, PA. 2012. Available online: http://www.pstnet.com (accessed on 10 November 2013).
- Psychology Software Tools Inc Eprime Extension for Tobii. 2012. Available online: http://www.pstnet.com (accessed on 8 July 2014).
- The MathWorks Inc. Matlab; The MathWorks Inc.: Natick, MA, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Warnes, G.; Bolker, B.; Gorjanc, G.; Grothendieck, G.; Korosec, A.; Lumley, T.; Rogers, J. Gdata: Various R Programming Tools for Data Manipulation. 2015. Available online: https://rdrr.io/cran/gdata/ (accessed on 1 October 2017).
- Dink, J.W.; Ferguson, B. Eyetracking R: An R Library for Eye-Tracking Data Analysis. 2015. Available online: https://www.rdocumentation.org/packages/eyetrackingR/versions/0.1.8 (accessed on 1 October 2017).
- Douglas, B.; Martin, M.; Ben, B.; Steve, W. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 2015, 67, 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wickham, H.; Sievert, C. Ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis, 2nd ed.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Bates, D.; Maechler, M. Matrix: Sparse and Dense Matrix Classes and Methods. 2017. Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/301841014_Matrix_sparse_and_dense_matrix_classes_and_methods (accessed on 1 October 2017).
- Wickham, H. Tidyverse: Easily Install and Load “Tidyverse” Packages. 2017. Available online: https://tidyverse.tidyverse.org (accessed on 1 October 2017).
- Schofield, C.A.; Inhoff, A.W.; Coles, M.E. Time-Course of Attention Biases in Social Phobia. J. Anxiety Disord. 2013, 27, 661–669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kruijt, A.; Parsons, S.; Fox, E. A meta-analysis of bias at baseline in RCTs of attention bias modification: No evidence for dot-probe bias towards threat in clinical anxiety and PTSD. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2019, 128, 563–573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Batty, M.; Taylor, M.J. The Development of Emotional Face Processing during Childhood. Dev. Sci. 2006, 9, 207–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kuchuk, A.; Vibbert, M.; Bornstein, M.H. Perception of Smiling and Its Experimental Correlates in Three-Month-Old Infants. Child. Dev. 1986, 57, 1054–1061. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Labarbera, J.D.; Izard, C.E.; Vietze, P.; Parisi, S.A. Four-and Six-Month-Old Infants’ Visual Responses to Joy, Anger, and Neutral Expressions. Child. Dev. 1976, 47, 535–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Waters, A.M.; Henry, J.; Mogg, K.; Bradley, B.P.; Pine, D.S. Attentional Bias Towards Angry Faces in Childhood Anxiety Disorders. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 2010, 41, 158–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Waters, A.M.; Mogg, K.; Bradley, B.P.; Pine, D.S. Attentional Bias for Emotional Faces in Children With Generalized Anxiety Disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child. Adolesc. Psychiatry 2008, 47, 435–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Waters, A.M.; Pittaway, M.; Mogg, K.; Bradley, B.P.; Pine, D.S. Attention Training Towards Positive Stimuli in Clinically Anxious Children. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2013, 4, 77–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eldar, S.; Bar-Haim, Y. Neural Plasticity in Response to Attention Training in Anxiety. Psychol. Med. 2010, 40, 667–677. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Koster, E.H.W.; Baert, S.; Bockstaele, M.; De Raedt, R. Attentional Retraining Procedures: Manipulating Early or Late Components of Attentional Bias? Emotion 2010, 10, 230–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Office of National Statistics, Ethnicity and National Identity in England and Wales: 2011. Available online: https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/culturalidentity/ethnicity/articles/ethnicityandnationalidentityinenglandandwales/2012-12-11#differences-in-ethnicity-across-local-authorities (accessed on 3 November 2017).
- Hallett, P.E. Primary and Secondary Saccades to Goals Defined by Instructions. Vis. Res. 1978, 18, 1279–1296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klein, C.; Foerster, F. Development of Prosaccade and Antisaccade Task Performance in Participants Aged 6 to 26 Years. Psychophysiology 2001, 38, 179–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Mean Proportions (SD) | |||
---|---|---|---|
Total | High-Anxious Group (n = 65) | Low-Anxious Group (n = 39) | |
Mean Proportions of Trials Where the Initial Look Was to the Emotional Face | |||
Angry Trials | 0.59 (0.22) | 0.60 (0.23) | 0.57 (0.20) |
Happy Trials | 0.63 (0.21) | 0.64 (0.22) | 0.63 (0.20) |
Mean Proportions of Time Spent Looking at Faces During Initial Looks | |||
Angry Trials | |||
Emotional Face | 0.35 (0.17) | 0.35 (0.16) | 0.36 (0.18) |
Neutral Face | 0.16 (0.16) | 0.15 (0.15) | 0.17 (0.16) |
Happy Trials | |||
Emotional Face | 0.40 (0.19) | 0.41 (0.20) | 0.39 (0.20) |
Neutral Face | 0.13 (0.12) | 0.13 (0.13) | 0.13 (0.11) |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Stuijfzand, S.; Stuijfzand, B.; Reynolds, S.; Dodd, H. Anxiety-Related Attention Bias in Four- to Eight-Year-Olds: An Eye-Tracking Study. Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 194. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs10120194
Stuijfzand S, Stuijfzand B, Reynolds S, Dodd H. Anxiety-Related Attention Bias in Four- to Eight-Year-Olds: An Eye-Tracking Study. Behavioral Sciences. 2020; 10(12):194. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs10120194
Chicago/Turabian StyleStuijfzand, Suzannah, Bobby Stuijfzand, Shirley Reynolds, and Helen Dodd. 2020. "Anxiety-Related Attention Bias in Four- to Eight-Year-Olds: An Eye-Tracking Study" Behavioral Sciences 10, no. 12: 194. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs10120194
APA StyleStuijfzand, S., Stuijfzand, B., Reynolds, S., & Dodd, H. (2020). Anxiety-Related Attention Bias in Four- to Eight-Year-Olds: An Eye-Tracking Study. Behavioral Sciences, 10(12), 194. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs10120194