Next Article in Journal
Whole-Genome Sequencing Reveals the Progress of Genetic Breeding in Eriocheir sinensis
Next Article in Special Issue
Behavioral and Physiological Responses of Therapy Dogs to Animal-Assisted Treatment in an Inpatient Stroke Rehabilitation Program
Previous Article in Journal
A New ‘cyclotis-morphotype’ Species of Tube-Nosed Bat (Chiroptera: Vespertilionidae: Murina) from China
Previous Article in Special Issue
Mechanisms of Social Attachment Between Children and Pet Dogs
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

A Study on the Attachment to Pets Among Owners of Cats and Dogs Using the Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS) in the Basque Country

by
Eider Egaña-Marcos
1,
Olatz Goñi-Balentziaga
2 and
Garikoiz Azkona
1,*
1
Department of Basic Psychological Processes and Their Development, Euskal Herriko Unibertsitatea (UPV/EHU), Tolosa Hiribidea, 20018 Donostia, Spain
2
Department of Clinical and Health Psychology and Research Methodology, Euskal Herriko Unibertsitatea (UPV/EHU), Tolosa Hiribidea, 20018 Donostia, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2025, 15(1), 76; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15010076
Submission received: 7 November 2024 / Revised: 20 December 2024 / Accepted: 30 December 2024 / Published: 1 January 2025
(This article belongs to the Special Issue The Complexity of the Human–Companion Animal Bond)

Simple Summary

The bond between humans and their pets has long captivated researchers, particularly in understanding how attachment varies based on the type of pet. Cats and dogs display distinct behavioral and social characteristics that shape the dynamics of human–pet relationships. Furthermore, certain human traits have also been identified as influencing this attachment. Our study investigates factors affecting pet attachment among cat and dog owners in the Basque Country, located in northern Spain. By investigating these aspects, our research aims to confirm the human factors that influence the human–animal bond in a previously unstudied population. Our findings confirm that attachment tends to be notably stronger with dogs than with cats and that owner’s traits such as being female, younger, not living with children, and the amount of time spent with pets on weekends are associated with stronger attachments to pets.

Abstract

The relationship between humans and their pets has long fascinated researchers, particularly in exploring how attachment varies according to the type of pet. Cats and dogs exhibit unique behavioral and social traits that influence the dynamics of human–pet relationships. Moreover, specific human characteristics have been found to affect this attachment. Our study examines the human factors that influence pet attachment among cat and dog owners in the Basque Country, located in northern Spain. By investigating these elements, our research aims to enhance the understanding of how human factors shape the human–animal bond. The study included a total of 202 participants, of whom 66.8% were dog owners, and 74.8% identified as women, with ages ranging from 18 to 74 years. Consistent with many previous studies, our results indicate that attachment is generally stronger with dogs compared to cats and that owner’s characteristics such as being female, younger, not living with children, and the amount of time spent with pets on weekends are linked to stronger attachments to pets.

1. Introduction

Through Law 7/2023 on the Protection of Animal Rights and Welfare, the Spanish legal system defines pets as “domestic or wild animals in captivity, kept by humans, primarily in the home” [1]. A recent worldwide survey indicated that 58% of respondents owned pets. Of these pet owners, 59% had dogs and 53% had cats [2]. In Spain, dogs and cats are the preferred companion animals [3]. In fact, there are 10,165,498 dogs and 967,834 cats registered [4], meaning there are more pets than people under 18 (8,589,495 out of a total population of 48,592,909) [5]. The same trend is reported in the Basque Country, a northern region where 392,234 dogs and 32,138 cats are registered [4] and the under-18 population is 399,011 (total population of 2,196,745) [6].
Global pet ownership is estimated to grow, particularly in millennial households (1980–1994), which tend to have smaller families and children later in life [7]. Economic factors also play a role; while pets require some investment, their maintenance costs are generally lower [8]. In any case, pet owners value their relationship with their pet very highly, as shown by an international survey of dog and cat owners in which 95% of respondents considered their pet to be a member of the family and almost 90% described their relationship as close [9].
The human–animal interaction that results in a human–animal bond is a critical factor in the existence of multi-species families. The human–animal bond has been described as a mutually beneficial relationship between humans and animals. This bond is influenced by behaviors that are essential for the mental, physical, and social health and well-being of both parties [10]. Whereas few studies have focused on the benefits for the animals [11,12,13], numerous studies have shown how beneficial interactions with pets can be for human health, particularly in regard to stress management [14,15,16,17,18,19]. One of the main explanations for how people benefit from interacting with their pets is the social support they provide [20,21]. A recent systematic review showed that there are no discernible differences between dogs and cats in their relationships with their owners and pet owners’ perceptions of loneliness and social isolation. This suggests that both species provide companionship and emotional support [22].
Several theories attempt to explain the foundations of the human–animal bond. One key explanation is the biophilia theory, which suggests that humans have a natural interest in nature and, by extension, in animals. [23,24]. Although the domestication of dogs predates that of cats [25,26], both species have long coexisted in our society. Historically, the relationship between dogs and humans has been more cooperative, involving elements such as assisting with hunting. However, while cats have provided a service to humans in the sense that they hunt mice, their relationship with humans has always been less cooperative [27].
A second model posits that the bond between humans and animals is based on attachment [28,29], a universal human trait in affiliative behavior [30,31]. This suggests that humans have an innate need to connect with others, forming bonds of affiliation and affection. This need for connection may also explain why attachments can form with individuals of other species [24,32,33].
The bond between humans and their pets has long been a topic of fascination for researchers, with a particular focus on how attachment differs depending on the type of pet [34,35,36]. Certain behaviors of dogs and cats toward humans are similar, whereas others are different. The ability of both species to follow human pointing is similar, but dogs are more likely to look at their owners when faced with a problem-solving scenario in which they need their owners’ assistance to obtain food [37]. Dogs are generally perceived as more interactive, trainable, and dependent on human attention. Dogs tend to be more social and responsive to human interaction, exhibiting bonding behaviors such as tail wagging, eye contact, and playfulness. These behaviors often lead to stronger bonds, as dogs are perceived as more interactive and affectionate companions [38]. In contrast, cats are often perceived as more autonomous and self-reliant, characteristics that may lead to a different type of attachment, one that is less intense but still meaningful [39], which may contribute to a relationship that is perceived as more “coexistent” than “companionate” [40]. Cat owners tend to place a high value on their cats’ autonomy, a trait that may be less desirable in dogs [41,42]. From this perspective, it is possible that the traditional gap in the amount of care owners provide for their cats may be due to a natural human response to the different behavioral characteristics of cats and dogs [43].
In human bonding processes, eye contact is fundamental in establishing closeness and expressing feelings of love and loyalty, and, at the neuroendocrine level, oxytocin plays an important role in bonding [44,45]. Recently, it has been described that oxytocin is released in both dogs and owners when they look at each other, suggesting that humans feel an affection for their dogs similar to that felt for family members [46]. It has also been shown that interbrain synchronization occurs within an interacting human–dog dyad, which may be responsible for interspecies communication [47]. In cats, one study found that intranasally administered oxytocin increased male, but not female, gaze toward humans [48]. The male-specific increase in gaze toward humans observed in this study differs from previous research on dogs, where such effects were observed only in females [46]. These findings suggest that exogenous oxytocin may have a general effect on cats’ social relationships with humans as well as the possibility of different mechanisms between cat–human and dog–human relationships.
The Lexington Pet Attachment Scale (LAPS) is one of the most popular scaled instruments used to measure the quality of attachment between pet owners and their pets. It has been applied to many species, although it is primarily aimed at cat and dog owners, and has been used in various countries, including Australia [49], Austria [50], Brazil [51], Canada [52], Denmark [50], France [53], German [54], Italy [13,55,56,57], Mexico [58,59], New Zealand [49], Portugal [60], the United Kingdom (UK) [49,50], and the United States of America (USA) [35,61,62,63,64,65,66]. These studies have shown that dog owners generally score higher than cat owners and that the demographic characteristics of the owner, such as age, gender, and education, for example, influence the level of attachment. Interestingly, one study found that dog–human attachment was only influenced by owner characteristics [13].
We are aware of only one study that analyzed attachment using the short version of the LAPS in Spain. This study found that mean scores were very high among volunteers from shelters in Andalusia, a southern region [67]. Given the lack of data in Spain, this work aimed to explore the level of attachment between cat and dog owners in the Basque Country, a region in the north of the country, and the effect of pet owners’ demographic factors on pet attachment. By examining these aspects, our research aims to confirm the human factors that influence the human–animal bond in a previously unstudied population.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants and Procedure

The participants were recruited online between 10 July 2023 and 30 September 2024 through email lists provided by various Basque veterinary colleges, where veterinarians were asked to disseminate the invitation among their clients. Additionally, a snowball sampling technique was employed: respondents were asked to invite other cat or dog owners to participate in the survey. The study was restricted to current cats or dogs owners living in the Basque Country (Spain) who were over 18 years old. Participants who owned both cats and dogs simultaneously were excluded.
In a cover letter attached to the questionnaire, participants were informed that the survey data would be used for scientific purposes only and that their responses would remain completely anonymous. All participants provided voluntary informed consent before completing the short online questionnaire, which took approximately 10 min (via the Google Drive platform). The study was conducted in accordance with the guidelines established by the Declaration of Helsinki. All procedures and informed consent protocols were approved by the Ethics Committee for Human-Related Research (CEISH) of the University of the Basque Country (UPV/EHU); M10/2023/222.

2.2. Instruments

The survey included questions about participants’ personal information, such as whether they were cat or dog owners, gender, sexual orientation, age (years), education, sentimental relationship (yes/no), household composition, area of residence, salary range, number of pets they lived with, who usually took care of the pets, how long they had lived with their pets (in years), and how much time (in hours) they spent with their pets during the weekdays and on weekends.
The attachment to cats and dogs was assessed using the Spanish version of the LAPS [59], substituting the term dog (“perro”) for pet (“animal de compañia”). This scale comprises 23 items rated on a four-point Likert-type scale (0 = strongly disagree; 3 = strongly agree) and measures three subscales: General Attachment (GA), Person Substitution (PS) and Animal Rights (AR). The GA subscale reflects the level of affection between the owner and the pet, whereas the PS subscale measures how central the pet is in the owner’s life. Finally, the AR subscale indicates the pet’s role or status within the household. The participants were asked to answer the questions with their current pet(s) in mind. Supplementary Table S1 shows the means and standard deviation of the 23 items. The reliability analysis demonstrated very good internal consistency for all tests (α = 0.89, ω = 0.91).

2.3. Statistical Data Analysis

All statistical analyses were performed using the Jamovi software package (version 2.3.21.0) and GraphPad Prism software package (version 10.3.1), with the significance level set to p < 0.05. Frequency (%) and distribution statistics (mean ± standard deviation (SD), median, and minimum-maximum) were used to describe the sample. Mean, standard deviation, and homogeneity indices were calculated for each LAPS item. The reliability of the LAPS total score and subscales was analyzed using standardized Cronbach’s alpha (α) and McDonald’s ordinal omega (ω) coefficients.
The normality test (Shapiro–Wilk) indicated a non-parametric distribution for all variable scores. Subsequently, Mann–Whitney U tests (for variables with two categories) or Kruskal–Wallis one-way analyses of variance (for variables with more than two categories) were conducted to analyze differences in LAPS scores. To calculate effect sizes, we used the rank biserial correlation (rrb), reference values of <0.3 (small effect), 0.3–0.5 (moderate effect), and >0.5 (large effect), and the Squared Epsilon coefficient (ε2), reference values of 0.01–<0.06 (small effect), 0.06–<0.14 (moderate effect), and ≥0.14 (large effect). We used a two-way ANOVA to analyze the possible interaction of species with gender and with the factor of living with children at home, and specific comparisons were analyzed using the post hoc Tukey test. A Cohen’s d (d) test for the effect size was performed to estimate the strength of the effects between two groups (>0.8 large effect, 0.5–0.8 moderate effect, and < 0.5 small effect).
Associations between parameters were analyzed using a bivariate Spearman correlation (rho) with interpretations of <0.09 (very small effect), 0.10–0.29 (small effect), 0.30–0.49 (moderate effect), and >0.50 (large effect). There were no missing data. Only significant differences between groups are presented in the results section; small effect sizes are not considered.
From this initial analysis, we observed that certain factors (species, gender, age and living with/without children) could influence the total LAPS score and its subscales. Time spent with pets at weekends could influence the LAPS, and time spent during the weekdays and at weekends (also total time) could influence the GA and AR scores. These factors met the criteria for statistical significance in the analysis of variance and were correlated. To determine the independent influence of these variables, we then performed linear regression analyses.

3. Results

3.1. Participants’ Information

A total of 202 individuals completed the survey (Table 1), of whom more than half were dog owners (66.8%). The vast majority identified as women (74.8%) and heterosexual (80.2%). Their ages ranged from 18 to 74 years (mean: 39.1 ± 13.8; median: 38). Slightly more than half of the participants had a university degree (63.8%) and no romantic partner (59.4%). Most participants lived accompanied at home (83.7%), without children (71%), and in urban areas (82.7%). The majority of participants earned between EUR 12,000 and EUR 52,000 per year (68.8%). Slightly less than half of the participants lived with one pet (69.3%) and shared its care (53.5%). The median number of close friends was 5 (0–40), and the median number of close relatives was three (0–17), bringing the total median number of people in the participants’ most intimate circle to eight (1–55). There were no significant differences in the number of close friends and relatives according to species.
Cat owners reported living with their pets for significantly longer periods of time than dog owners (U = 3561.5, p = 0.014, rrb = 0.21). Cat owners had been living with them for an average of 6 ± 4.8 years (median: 4, ranging from 4 months to 18 years), whereas dog owners reported an average of 7.2 ± 4.2 years (median: 7, ranging from 2 months to 19 years). Both groups of pet owners reported spending similar amounts of time with their pets during weekdays (cat owners: 49.6 ± 39.2 h, median: 40, ranging from 1 to 120 h; dog owners: 49.8 ± 40.5 h, median: 35, ranging from 1 to 120 h). However, cat owners reported spending slightly less time than dog owners on weekends (21.1 ± 14.4 h, median: 18, ranging from 1 to 48 h, vs. 26.7 ± 15.9 h, median: 24, ranging from 1 to 48 h, respectively) (U = 3615, p = 0.02, rrb = 0.20). Overall, cat owners spent 70.7 ± 50.1 h per week interacting with their cats (median: 62, ranging from 2 to 168 h), whereas dog owners spent 76.6 ± 52.9 h per week (median: 64, ranging from 2 to 168 h).

3.2. Attachment to Pets

Cat owners scored lower on the total LAPS (U = 3060, p < 0.001, rrb = 0.32) and on the Person Substitution subscale (U = 2871, p < 0.001, rrb = 0.36) than dog owners (Table 2). Significant differences with small effect sizes were observed in the General Attachment (U = 3399, p = 0.004, rrb = 0.24) and Animal Rights (U = 3665, p = 0.027, rrb = 0.19) subscales.
Significant differences were found according to the gender of the participants and according to whether or not they were living at home with children. No statistical differences were observed for the other variables described in Table 1. Participants who identified as female scored higher on the LAPS (U = 2619.5, p < 0.001, rrb = 0.32) and on the Person Substitution subscale (U = 2556, p < 0.001, rrb = 0.34) than those who identified as male. Gender-significant differences were also observed on the General Attachment (U = 2913, p = 0.009, rrb = 0.24) and Animal Rights (U = 3046, p = 0.024, rrb = 0.21) subscales, though with small effect sizes (Table 3).
We analyzed whether gender influenced attachment to cats or dogs using a two-way ANOVA (gender and species) analysis. Statistical analysis revealed no gender–species interaction (Figure 1) but showed significant differences for the species factor in the LPAS total score (F (1,198) = 13.69, p = 0.0003) and the GA (F (1,198) = 8.935, p = 0.0032), PS (F (1,198) = 16.20, p < 0.0001), and AR (F (1,198) = 5.822, p = 0.017) subscales, as well as for the gender factor in the LPAS (F (1,198) = 8.553, p = 0.0039) and the PS (F (1,198) = 11.74, p = 0.0007) and AR (F (1,198) = 5.581, p = 0.0191) subscales. The results showed that female dog owners scored higher on all scales (Supplementary Table S2). Female dog owners scored significantly higher than male cat owners (p < 0.0001, d = −1.13) and female cat owners (p = 0.007, d = −0.57) in regard to total LPS. Female dog owners also scored significantly higher in PS compared to male cat owners (p < 0.0001, d = −1.27), female cat owners (p < 0.001, d = −0.70), and male dog owners (p = 0.017, d = −0.59). Additionally, female dog owners scored significantly higher than male cat owners in GA (p = 0.008, d = −0.82) and AR (p = 0.009, d = −0.81).
Participants who lived with children were found to score lower on the LAPS (U = 1955, p < 0.001, rrb = 0.34) and the General Attachment (U = 2210, p = 0.011, rrb = 0.25), Person Substitution (U = 1951, p < 0.001, rrb = 0.34), and Animal Rights (U = 1933, p < 0.001, rrb = 0.34) subscales compared to those who did not live with children (Table 4).
A two-way ANOVA (children and species) analysis revealed no children–species interactions (Figure 2), but there were significant differences in the children factor in the LPAS total score (F (1,166) = 8.393, p = 0.0043) as well as the GA (F (1,166) = 5.730, p = 0.0178), PS (F (1,166) = 6.679, p = 0.0106) and AR (F (1,166) = 10.74, p = 0.0013) subscales, although there were only differences in the species factor in the PS subscale (F (1,166) = 6.246, p = 0.0134). Dog owners living without children at home scored higher on all scales (Supplementary Table S3). Dog owners without children at home scored significantly higher in the total LAPS than cat owners with children (p = 0.037, d = −0.54) and without children (p = 0.017, d = 0.98) and dog owners with children (p = 0.007, d = 0.70). A similar trend was observed in PS, where dog owners without children scored significantly higher than cat owners with children (p < 0.01, d = 1.02) and without children (p < 0.01, d = −0.65) and dog owners with children (p < 0.01, d = 0.61). Additionally, dog owners without children scored significantly higher in GA compared to dog owners with children (p = 0.038, d = 0.57). They also scored higher in AR compared to cat owners (p = 0.011, d = 0.79) and dog owners (p = 0.003, d = 0.74) living with children.

3.3. Bivariate Correlation Analyisis

A correlational analysis revealed a weak negative correlation between age and the LAPS total score (−0.26, p < 0.001), as well as the General Attachment (−0.22, p = 0.01), Person Substitution (−0.29, p < 0.001) and Animal Rights (−0.25, p < 0.001) subscales. A weak positive correlation was observed between time spent on weekdays and the General Attachment (0.16, p = 0.02) and Animal Rights (0.15, p = 0.04) subscales. A weak positive correlation was also observed between time spent on weekends and the LAPS (0.20, p = 0.005), as well as the General Attachment (0.24, p < 0.001) and Animal Rights (0.15, p = 0.03) subscales.

3.4. Linear Regression

Previous analyses have shown that certain qualitative (species, gender, living with or without children at home) and quantitative (age and time spent with pets) variables influence LAPS scores and the GA, PS and AR subscales. To determine the influence of each of these variables, we performed a regression analysis.
The linear regression model revealed that approximately 24.7% of the variability in total LAPS is explained by the independent variables, while the amount is 14.9% for GA, 23.3% for PS, and 16.1% for AR. Among the variables, species was found to have a significant effect in the LAPS and the PS subscale, gender in the LAPS and PS subscale, age in the LAPS and the PS and AR subscales, and living with/without children in the LAPS and the GA and AR subscales. Time during the weekend was only significant in the LAPS regression model (Table 5).

4. Discussion

Many human characteristics have been identified as influencing the strength and nature of the human–animal bond. In our study, we investigated the relationship between the strength of the human–animal bond and the characteristics of cat and dog owners. We included only owners of either cats or dogs, but not both, to ensure that the bond measurement was species-specific and in order to minimize the possible bias arising from owning several pets of different species.
In our study, we first analyzed the psychometric characteristics of the adaptations we made for the Mexican version of the LAPS to generalize it to different pet species within the Spanish population (including Spanish speakers from other countries). As mentioned in the introduction, there is only one study in Spain that explores pet attachment, and it was conducted in the south of the country with volunteers from shelters rather than with pet owners. Although our study focused on a region in the north of the country, mainly for reasons of sample access, we believe that our results can be extrapolated to the rest of the country.
Our findings, along with previous research [49,50,54,61,65,66], support the notion that dog owners tend to have higher levels of attachment, consistently scoring higher on the LAPS than cat owners worldwide. When comparing LAPS scores across different studies, we found that not all of them use the same numerical scale. Although the original work by Johnson et al. (1992) [61] used a scale from 0 to 3, other studies have evaluated the scale from 1 to 4 [41,68,69,70] or even 1 to 5 [60,67]. Therefore, to accurately compare the total LAPS scores between our study and others, we included only those studies that used the original numerical scale, which is also used in our study.
Overall, the average LAPS values for cat and dog owners in our study were similar to those in studies from Austria [50], New Zealand [49], and the UK [50]; slightly lower than those from Brazil [51], France [53] and Germany [54]; and slightly higher than those from Denmark [50] and the USA [61,71,72]. A similar trend was observed when comparing the results of cat and dog owners separately. In a study conducted exclusively among dog owners in Italy [55], the total LAPS scores were slightly higher than ours, whereas, in Mexico [59], the scores were slightly lower. However, based on the categorization proposed by Marinelli et al. (2017)—with medium (23–46) and high (47–69) categories [13]—our results would fall within the upper part of this categorization. Consistent with these findings, a recent study of dog and cat owners from various parts of the world, mostly from the Global North [73], found no significant differences based on geographic location, and the results were very similar to ours. These data suggest a cultural globalization in the relationship patterns between cat and dog owners in Northern countries.
Our findings are consistent with previous research on owner characteristics that influence the strength of the pet–owner bond. Women scored higher on the LAPS, a result that aligns with all prior studies [49,50,51,53,54,56,57,61,73,74,75]. Specifically, we observed that female owners of dogs reported the highest attachment levels. This gender difference may stem from different social and cultural expectations, with women often being encouraged to display nurturing behaviors [76], which can lead to higher attachment scores. Additionally, women are generally more inclined than men to engage in positive behaviors and hold favorable attitudes toward animals, such as viewing pets as family members, supporting animal welfare, and opposing their exploitation [77]. These factors contribute to a stronger emotional bond with animals.
In the original 1992 study, older adults (aged 60 and above) had higher scores on the LAPS [61], and two subsequent studies found no significant relationship between an owner’s age and their attachment to pets [13,49]. However, our findings show that younger participants scored higher in regard to pet attachment than older participants—a pattern also observed in recent studies [54,56,73]. This shift may reflect changes in the role of pets among younger generations that are influenced by evolving cultural norms, increased awareness of animal welfare, and shifting social dynamics. This trend is particularly notable in Western cultures, where pets are increasingly anthropomorphized and valued as close companions. Pets are often given human names, showcased on social media, and viewed as family members rather than property [78,79].
Another factor that influences attachment to pets is the presence of children in the household [53,54,61]. Our data indicated that participants living with children scored lower on the LAPS, a trend previously observed among dog owners [13], though not among cat owners [80]. This distinction between dog and cat owners was also evident in our study. In this context, individuals without children may view pets as substitutes for offspring, thereby fostering stronger emotional connections. Prior studies support this idea, suggesting that voluntarily childless women may perceive their attachment to pets as comparable to, or even exceeding, the bonds typically formed between parents and their children [81]. Overall, these findings imply that pets play a distinctive role in filling familial voids, particularly in the absence of children or within certain family structures.
The time an owner spends with their pet is another factor influencing owner–pet attachment. This result appears to be species-influenced, as our data show that dog owners spent more time with their pet(s) during the weekend than cat owners. Generally, the more time spent with a pet, the stronger the attachment, as frequent interactions help to strengthen the bond [82]. Dog owners, for instance, often spend more time actively engaging with their pets due to the need for regular walks and outdoor activities, which may contribute to the higher attachment levels typically seen in dog owners compared to cat owners [60]. In contrast, cat owners might engage in fewer direct interactions, as cats are generally more independent and do not require as much attention or structured activity as dogs. Thus, certain species-specific interactions can strengthen pet–owner attachments. Research has shown that pet owners who regularly engage in caregiving activities—such as feeding, grooming, or playing—tend to report stronger attachment levels [13]. However, the quality of interactions also plays an important role; meaningful interactions that foster companionship and mutual understanding can enhance the human–animal bond regardless of the total time spent together.
Our study, however, did not identify certain owner characteristics—such as educational level [53,54,61], residential area [66,83,84], income range [53,54,61], relationship status [61,66], or social network size [61]—as predictors, contrary to findings from previous research. Differences in demographic factors, such as the cultural background of the participants, may have contributed to these outcomes. Additionally, discrepancies may be due to variations in sample size and recruitment methods, which can influence both the generalizability and reliability of the findings. Further studies using more diverse and representative samples are necessary to better understand the role of these variables and to determine whether our results reflect broader patterns or are specific to our sample.
Our study has several potential limitations. First, there are concerns regarding the use of the animal welfare questions in the LAPS, particularly the fact that some questions are framed negatively whereas others are framed positively. Moreover, the LAPS focuses on the emotional aspect of the pet–owner relationship, without accounting for other factors that may also be important, such as the personality of the owners. Research has shown that attachment levels can be linked to specific personality traits [71,73,85]. Second, the reliance on self-reported questionnaires introduces the potential for response bias, as it depends on participants’ honesty. Third, our study had a predominance of female participants, a trend also seen in most of the previous studies we referenced. This may reflect the fact that women are generally more likely to respond to online surveys [86]. Future research should aim to increase male representation for a more balanced perspective.

5. Conclusions

Our findings support the conclusion that attachment is generally more pronounced with dogs compared to cats and that certain owner characteristics—such as being female, younger, not living with children, and the amount of time spent with pets on weekends—are associated with stronger bonds with pets.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ani15010076/s1, Table S1: Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale: Spanish Version; Table S2: Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS) results by pet owners and gender; Table S3: Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS) results by pet owners and children presence at home.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, G.A.; methodology, O.G.-B. and G.A.; formal analysis, E.E.-M., O.G.-B. and G.A.; writing—original draft preparation, E.E.-M., O.G.-B. and G.A.; writing—review and editing, G.A.; supervision, G.A. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board (or Ethics Committee) of the University of the Basque Country (UPV/EHU) M10/2023/222 (07/07/2023).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The study data will be made available upon reasonable request to the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all respondents for participating in the study, as well as the Veterinary Boards of Araba, Bizkaia, and Gipuzkoa for distributing the questionnaire.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. BOE-A-2023-7936, Ley 7/2023, de 28 de Marzo, de Protección de los Derechos y el Bienestar de los Animales. 2023. Available online: https://www.boe.es/eli/es/l/2023/03/28/7 (accessed on 14 March 2024).
  2. TGM-Research. Pet Care Consumer Behavior in 2023: What You Need to Know. Available online: https://tgmresearch.com/tgm-global-pet-care-survey.html#:~:text=Results%20from%20TGM%20Research’s%20Pet,cats%20as%20their%20chosen%20companions (accessed on 14 March 2024).
  3. López-Cepero, J.; Martos-Montes, R.; Ordóñez, D. Classification of Animals as Pet, Pest, or Profit: Consistency and Associated Variables Among Spanish University Students. Anthrozoös 2021, 34, 877–888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. REIAC. Red Española de Identificación de Animales de Compañia. Available online: https://www.reiac.es/reiac-info-02.php?lang=ESP (accessed on 14 March 2024).
  5. INE. Instituto Nacional de Estadística. Estadística Continua de Población. Últimos Datos. Available online: https://www.ine.es/dyngs/INEbase/es/operacion.htm?c=Estadistica_C&cid=1254736177095&menu=ultiDatos&idp=1254735572981 (accessed on 14 March 2024).
  6. Eustat. Instituto Vasco de Estadística. Población de la C.A. de Euskadi por Ámbitos Territoriales, Según Grandes Grupos de Edad y Sexo. 01/01/2023. Available online: https://www.eustat.eus/elementos/ele0011400/poblacion-de-la-ca-de-euskadi-por-ambitos-territoriales-segun-grandes-grupos-de-edad-y-sexo/tbl0011427_c.html (accessed on 14 March 2024).
  7. HealthforAnimals, Global State of Pet Care Stats, Facts and Trends. 2022. Available online: https://healthforanimals.org/wp-content/uploads/2022/07/Global-State-of-Pet-Care.pdf (accessed on 14 March 2024).
  8. Enfoka-Trends, Las Mascotas: Una Tendencia de Consumo Creciente. 2022. Available online: https://www.euskadi.eus/contenidos/documentacion/obs_mer_tendinnov_doc/es_def/adjuntos/2023_09_tendencias_septiembre_2023.pdf (accessed on 14 March 2024).
  9. HABRI, “New Research Confirms the Strong Bond Between People and Pets is a Global Phenomenon, 95% Worldwide Say Pets Are Family,” ed. 2022. Available online: https://habri.org/pressroom/20220116 (accessed on 14 March 2024).
  10. AVMA. American Veterinary Medical Association. Human-Animal Bond. Available online: https://www.avma.org/resources-tools/one-health/human-animal-bond (accessed on 14 April 2024).
  11. Wensley, S.P. Animal welfare and the human-animal bond: Considerations for veterinary faculty, students, and practitioners. J. Vet. Med. Educ. 2008, 35, 532–539. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Samet, L.E.; Vaterlaws-Whiteside, H.; Harvey, N.D.; Upjohn, M.M.; Casey, R.A. Exploring and Developing the Questions Used to Measure the Human-Dog Bond: New and Existing Themes. Animals 2022, 12, 805. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Marinelli, L.; Adamelli, S.; Normando, S.; Bono, G. Quality of life of the pet dog: Influence of owner and dog’s characteristics. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 108, 143–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Shiloh, S.; Sorek, G.; Terkel, J. Reduction of State-Anxiety by Petting Animals in a Controlled Laboratory Experiment. Anxiety Stress Coping 2003, 16, 387–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Huber, A.; Klug, S.J.; Abraham, A.; Westenberg, E.; Schmidt, V.; Winkler, A.S. Animal-Assisted Interventions Improve Mental, But Not Cognitive or Physiological Health Outcomes of Higher Education Students: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 2022, 22, 1597–1628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Charry-Sánchez, J.D.; Pradilla, I.; Talero-Gutiérrez, C. Animal-assisted therapy in adults: A systematic review. In Complementary Therapies in Clinical Practice; Churchill Livingstone: London, UK, 2018; Volume 32, pp. 169–180. [Google Scholar]
  17. Peel, N.; Nguyen, K.; Tannous, C. The Impact of Campus-Based Therapy Dogs on the Mood and Affect of University Students. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, 4759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Grajfoner, D.; Harte, E.; Potter, L.M.; McGuigan, N. The Effect of Dog-Assisted Intervention on Student Well-Being, Mood, and Anxiety. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 483. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Chute, A.; Vihos, J.; Johnston, S.; Buro, K.; Velupillai, N. The effect of animal-assisted intervention on undergraduate students’ perception of momentary stress. Front. Psychol. 2023, 14, 1253104. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Serpell, J.A. 2–Animal-Assisted Interventions in Historical Perspective; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 2010; pp. 17–32. [Google Scholar]
  21. McConnell, A.R.; Brown, C.M.; Shoda, T.M.; Stayton, L.E.; Martin, C.E. Friends with benefits: On the positive consequences of pet ownership. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 101, 1239–1252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Kretzler, B.; König, H.H.; Hajek, A. Pet ownership, loneliness, and social isolation: A systematic review. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2022, 57, 1935–1957. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Wilson, O. Biophilia; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1984; p. 157. [Google Scholar]
  24. Serpell, J. In the Company of Animals: A Study of Human-Animal Relationships; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1996; p. 283. [Google Scholar]
  25. Vigne, J.D.; Guilaine, J.; Debue, K.; Haye, L.; Gérard, P. Early taming of the cat in Cyprus. Science 2004, 304, 259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Savolainen, P.; Zhang, Y.P.; Luo, J.; Lundeberg, J.; Leitner, T. Genetic evidence for an East Asian origin of domestic dogs. Science 2002, 298, 1610–1613. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Walsh, F. Human-animal bonds I: The relational significance of companion animals. Fam. Process 2009, 48, 462–480. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Bretherton, I. Attachment Theory: Retrospect and Prospect. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 1985, 50, 3–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Cassidy, J.; Shaver, P.R. Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  30. Depue, R.A.; Morrone-Strupinsky, J.V. A neurobehavioral model of affiliative bonding: Implications for conceptualizing a human trait of affiliation. Behav. Brain Sci. 2005, 28, 313–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Schultheiss, O.C.; Brunstein, C. Implicit Motives; Oxford University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2010; p. 592. [Google Scholar]
  32. Sable, P. The Pet Connection: An Attachment Perspective. Clin. Soc. Work. J. 2013, 41, 93–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Beetz, A.; Julius, H.; Turner, D.; Kotrschal, K. Effects of Social Support by a Dog on Stress Modulation in Male Children with Insecure Attachment. Front. Psychol. 2012, 3, 352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Hosey, G.; Melfi, V. Human-animal interactions, relationships and bonds: A review and analysis of the literature. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 2014, 27, 117–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Halbreich, E.D.; Hefner, T.D.; Healy, A.S.; Van Allen, J. Measurement of attachment in human-animal interaction research. Hum. -Anim. Interact. 2024, 12, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Leighton, S.C.; Nieforth, L.O.; Griffin, T.C.; O’Haire, M.E. Researcher-practitioner interaction: Collaboration to advance the field of human-animal interaction. Hum.-Anim. Interact. 2022, 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Miklósi, A.; Pongrácz, P.; Lakatos, G.; Topál, J.; Csányi, V. A comparative study of the use of visual communicative signals in interactions between dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans and cats (Felis catus) and humans. J. Comp. Psychol. 2005, 119, 179–186. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Serpell, J. The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behaviour and Interactions with People; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  39. Stammbach, K.B.; Turner, D.C. Understanding the Human—Cat Relationship: Human Social Support or Attachment. Anthrozoos 1999, 12, 162–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Archer, J. Why do people love their pets? Evol. Hum. Behav. 1997, 18, 237–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Borrelli, C.; Riggio, G.; Howell, T.J.; Piotti, P.; Diverio, S.; Albertini, M.; Mongillo, P.; Marinelli, L.; Baragli, P.; Di Iacovo, F.P.; et al. The Cat-Owner Relationship: Validation of the Italian C/DORS for Cat Owners and Correlation with the LAPS. Animals 2022, 13, 69. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Howell, T.J.; Bowen, J.; Fatjó, J.; Calvo, P.; Holloway, A.; Bennett, P.C. Development of the cat-owner relationship scale (CORS). Behav. Process. 2017, 141 Pt 3, 305–315. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Colleen, P.K. Dogs have masters, cats have staff: Consumers’ psychological ownership and their economic valuation of pets. J. Bus. Res. 2019, 99, 306–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Buchheim, A.; Heinrichs, M.; George, C.; Pokorny, D.; Koops, E.; Henningsen, P.; O’Connor, M.-F.; Gündel, H. Oxytocin enhances the experience of attachment security. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2009, 34, 1417–1422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Guastella, A.J.; Mitchell, P.B.; Dadds, M.R. Oxytocin increases gaze to the eye region of human faces. Biol. Psychiatry 2008, 63, 3–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Nagasawa, M.; Mitsui, S.; En, S.; Ohtani, N.; Ohta, M.; Sakuma, Y.; Onaka, T.; Mogi, K.; Kikusui, T. Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human-dog bonds. Science 2015, 348, 333–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Ren, W.; Yu, S.; Guo, K.; Lu, C.; Zhang, Y.Q. Disrupted Human-Dog Interbrain Neural Coupling in Autism-Associated Shank3 Mutant Dogs. Adv. Sci. 2024, 11, e2402493. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Hattori, M.; Kinoshita, K.; Saito, A.; Yamamoto, S. Exogenous oxytocin increases gaze to humans in male cats. Sci. Rep. 2024, 14, 8953. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Melfi, V.; Skyner, L.; Birke, L.; Ward, S.J.; Shaw, W.S.; Hosey, G. Furred and feathered friends: How attached are zookeepers to the animals in their care? Zoo. Biol. 2022, 41, 122–129. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Sandøe, P.; Palmer, C.; Corr, S.A.; Springer, S.; Lund, T.B. Do people really care less about their cats than about their dogs? A comparative study in three European countries. Front. Vet. Sci. 2023, 10, 1237547. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Saraiva de Albuquerque, N.; Costa, D.B.; dos Reis Rodrigues, G.; Silva Sessegolo, N.; Moret-Tatay, C.; Quarti Irigaray, T. Adaptation and psychometric properties of Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale: Brazilian version (LAPS-B). J. Vet. Behav. 2023, 61, 50–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. von Rentzell, K.A.; van Haaften, K.; Morris, A.; Protopopova, A. Investigation into owner-reported differences between dogs born in versus imported into Canada. PLoS ONE 2022, 17, e0268885. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Blanchard, T.; Zaboski-Pena, L.; Harroche, I.; Théodon, O.; Meynadier, A. Investigating influences on pet attachment in France: Insights from the adaptation of the French Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale. Hum.-Anim. Interact. 2024, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Hielscher, B.; Gansloßer, U.; Froboese, I. Attachment to Dogs and Cats in Germany: Translation of the Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS) and Description of the Pet Owning Population in Germany. Hum.-Anim. Interact. Bull. 2019, 7, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Uccheddu, S.; De Cataldo, L.; Albertini, M.; Coren, S.; Da Graça Pereira, G.; Haverbeke, A.; Mills, D.S.; Pierantoni, L.; Riemer, S.; Ronconi, L.; et al. Pet Humanisation and Related Grief: Development and Validation of a Structured Questionnaire Instrument to Evaluate Grief in People Who Have Lost a Companion Dog. Animals 2019, 9, 933. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Testoni, I.; De Vincenzo, C.; Campigli, M.; Caregnato Manzatti, A.; Ronconi, L.; Uccheddu, S. Validation of the HHHHHMM Scale in the Italian Context: Assessing Pets’ Quality of Life and Qualitatively Exploring Owners’ Grief. Animals 2023, 13, 1049. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Testoni, I.; De Cataldo, L.; Ronconi, L.; Zamperini, A. Pet Loss and Representations of Death, Attachment, Depression, and Euthanasia. Anthrozoös 2017, 30, 135–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Schoenfeld-Tacher, R.; Kogan, L.R.; Wright, M.L. Comparison of strength of the human-animal bond between Hispanic and non-Hispanic owners of pet dogs and cats. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 2010, 236, 529–534. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Ramírez, M.T.G.; Quezada Berumen, L.d.C.; Hernández, R.L. Psychometric Properties of the Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale: Mexican Version (LAPS-M). Anthrozoös 2014, 27, 351–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Martins, C.F.; Silva, L.; Soares, J.; Pinto, G.S.; Abrantes, C.; Cardoso, L.; Pires, M.A.; Sousa, H.; Mota, M.P. Walk or be walked by the dog? The attachment role. BMC Public Health 2024, 24, 684. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Johnson, T.P.; Garrity, T.F.; Stallones, L. Psychometric Evaluation of the Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (Laps). Anthrozoös 1992, 5, 160–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Zebrowska, M.; Strohmaier, S.; Huttenhower, C.; Eliassen, A.H.; Zeleznik, O.A.; Westgarth, C.; Huang, T.; Laden, F.; Hart, J.E.; Rosner, B.; et al. Pet Attachment and Anxiety and Depression in Middle-Aged and Older Women. JAMA Netw. Open 2024, 7, e2424810. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Bures, R.M.; Mueller, M.K.; Gee, N.R. Measuring Human-Animal Attachment in a Large U.S. Survey: Two Brief Measures for Children and Their Primary Caregivers. Front. Public Health 2019, 7, 107. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Carroll, A.D.; Cisneros, A.; Porter, H.; Moody, C.; Stellato, A.C. Dog Owner Perceptions of Veterinary Handling Techniques. Animals 2022, 12, 1387. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Mueller, M.K. The Relationship between Types of Human–Animal Interaction and Attitudes about Animals: An Exploratory Study. Anthrozoös 2014, 27, 295–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Nugent, W.R.; Daugherty, L. A Measurement Equivalence Study of the Family Bondedness Scale: Measurement Equivalence Between Cat and Dog Owners. Front. Vet. Sci. 2021, 8, 812922. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. López-Cepero, J.; Garcia-Martínez, J.; Martos-Montes, R.; Rivera, F. Bereavement for Companion Animals: Intensity, Moderating Variables, and Effects on Wellbeing. J. Loss Trauma 2024, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Weiss, E.; Gramann, S. A comparison of attachment levels of adopters of cats: Fee-based adoptions versus free adoptions. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2009, 12, 360–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Riggio, G.; Piotti, P.; Diverio, S.; Borrelli, C.; Di Iacovo, F.; Gazzano, A.; Howell, T.J.; Pirrone, F.; Mariti, C. The Dog-Owner Relationship: Refinement and Validation of the Italian C/DORS for Dog Owners and Correlation with the LAPS. Animals 2021, 11, 2166. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Piotti, P.; Karagiannis, C.; Satchell, L.; Michelazzi, M.; Albertini, M.; Alleva, E.; Pirrone, F. Use of the Milan Pet Quality of Life Instrument (MPQL) to Measure Pets’ Quality of Life during COVID-19. Animals 2021, 11, 1336. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Bagley, D.K.; Gonsman, V.L. Pet attachment and personality type. Anthrozoös 2005, 18, 28–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Shore, E.R.; Douglas, D.K.; Riley, M.L. What’s in it for the companion animal? Pet attachment and college students’ behaviors toward pets. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2005, 8, 1–11. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Wells, D.L.; Treacy, K.R. Pet attachment and owner personality. Front. Psychiatry 2024, 15, 1406590. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Corr, S.A.; Lund, T.B.; Sandøe, P.; Springer, S. Cat and dog owners’ expectations and attitudes towards advanced veterinary care (AVC) in the UK, Austria and Denmark. PLoS ONE 2024, 19, e0299315. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Atkinson, J.; Boden, T.; Mocho, J.P.; Johnson, T. Refining the unilateral ureteral obstruction mouse model: No sham, no shame. Lab. Anim. 2021, 55, 21–29. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Christov-Moore, L.; Simpson, E.A.; Coudé, G.; Grigaityte, K.; Iacoboni, M.; Ferrari, P.F. Empathy: Gender effects in brain and behavior. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2014, 46 Pt 4, 604–627. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Herzog, H.A. Gender Differences in Human–Animal Interactions: A Review. Anthrozoös 2007, 20, 7–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Dotson, M.J.; Hyatt, E.M. Understanding dog–human companionship. J. Bus. Res. 2008, 61, 457–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Power, E. Furry families: Making a human–dog family through home. Soc. Cult. Geogr. 2008, 9, 535–555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Adamelli, S.; Marinelli, L.; Normando, S.; Bono, G. Owner and cat features influence the quality of life of the cat. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2005, 94, 89–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Peterson, H.; Engwall, K. “Why Would You Want a Baby When You Could Have a Dog?” Voluntarily Childless Women’s “Peternal” Feelings, Longing and Ambivalence. Soc. Sci. 2019, 8, 126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Kurdek, L.A. Pet dogs as attachment figures. J. Social. Personal. Relatsh. 2008, 25, 247–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Poresky, R.H.; Daniels, A.M. Demographics of Pet Presence and Attachment. Anthrozoös 1998, 11, 236–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Baranyiová, E.; Holub, A.; Tyrlík, M.; Janáčková, B.; Ernstová, M. The Influence of Urbanization on the Behaviour of Dogs in the Czech Republic. Acta Vet. Brno 2005, 74, 401–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Reevy, G.M.; Delgado, M.M. Are emotionally attached companion animal caregivers conscientious and neurotic? Factors that affect the human-companion animal relationship. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2015, 18, 239–258. (In English) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Smith, G. Does Gender Influence Online Survey Participation? A Record-Linkage Analysis of University Faculty Online Survey Response Behavior. 2008. Available online: https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:154437630 (accessed on 25 October 2024).
Figure 1. Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS), General Attachment (GA), Person Substitution (PS), and Animal Rights (AR) results by gender and species. Data are presented as group median (min to max). * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001 and **** p < 0.0001.
Figure 1. Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS), General Attachment (GA), Person Substitution (PS), and Animal Rights (AR) results by gender and species. Data are presented as group median (min to max). * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001 and **** p < 0.0001.
Animals 15 00076 g001
Figure 2. Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS), General Attachment (GA), Person Substitution (PS) and Animal Rights (AR) results by living with or without children and species. Data are presented as the group median (min to max). * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Figure 2. Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS), General Attachment (GA), Person Substitution (PS) and Animal Rights (AR) results by living with or without children and species. Data are presented as the group median (min to max). * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Animals 15 00076 g002
Table 1. Participants’ personal information.
Table 1. Participants’ personal information.
Cat OwnerDog OwnerGeneral
n (%)
67 (33.2%)135 (66.8%)202 (100%)
Gender
Female49 (73.1%)102 (75.5%)151 (74.8%)
Male18 (26.8%)33 (24.5%)51 (25.2%)
Sexual orientation
Bisexual13 (19.4%)11 (8.1%)24 (11.9%)
Heterosexual51 (76.2%)111 (82.2%)162 (80.2%)
Homosexual3 (4.4%)13 (9.7%)16 (7.9%)
Education
Primary school4 (5.9%)2 (1.5%)6 (2.9%)
Secondary school8 (11.9%)15 (11.1%)23 (11.3%)
Vocational training 11 (16.4%)21 (15.5%)32 (15.8%)
Undergraduate degree41 (61.1%)88 (65.2%)129 (63.8%)
Ph.D.3 (4.5%)9 (6.7%)12 (5.9%)
Sentimental relationship
Yes25 (37.3%)57 (42.2%)82 (40.6%)
No42 (62.7%)78 (57.8%)120 (59.4%)
Household composition
Live alone12 (17.9%)21 (15.5%)33 (16.3%)
Live accompanied55 (82.1%)114 (84.5%)169 (83.7%)
   Without children36 (65.5%)84 (73.7%)120 (71%)
   With children19 (34.5%)30 (26.3%)49 (29%)
Living area
Rural9 (13.4%)26 (19.3%)35 (17.3%)
Urban58 (86.6%)109 (80.7%)167 (82.7%)
Salary range (euros/year)
Prefer not to say9 (13.5%)25 (18.5%)34 (16.8%)
<12,0008 (11.9%)14 (10.3%)22 (10.9%)
12,000–<28,00030 (44.8%)44 (32.6%)74 (36.6%)
28,000–<52,00019 (28.3%)46 (34.2%)65 (32.2%)
≥52,0001 (1.5%)6 (4.4%)7 (3.5%)
Number of pets
One39 (58.2%)101 (74.8%)140 (69.3%)
Two25 (37.3%)26 (19.2%)51 (25.2%)
Three1 (1.5%)3 (2.2%)4 (1.9%)
Four04 (2.9%)4 (1.9%)
Five1 (1.5%)1 (0.7%)2 (0.9%)
Six1 (1.5%)01 (0.5%)
Who takes care
Myself33 (49.2%)47 (34.8%)80 (39.5%)
Shared responsibility31 (46.3%)77 (57.1%)108 (53.5%)
Usually someone else3 (4.5%)11 (8.1%)14 (7%)
Table 2. Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS) results by pet owners. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table 2. Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS) results by pet owners. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
MeanSDMedianRange
LAPS Total score
General51.812.025411–69
Cat owners47.1 ***13.285011–68
Dog owners54.110.665616–69
General Attachment
General29.56.24314–36
Cat owners27.6 **7.07299–36
Dog owners30.55.57324–36
Person Substitution
General12.94.97130–21
Cat owners10.8 ***5.02100–20
Dog owners14.04.59143–21
Animal Rights
General12.02.78133–15
Cat owners11.3 *3.12123–15
Dog owners12.32.55133–15
Table 3. Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS) results by gender. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table 3. Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS) results by gender. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
MeanSDMedianRange
LAPS Total score
Men47.611.214911–65
Women53.2 ***11.985516–69
General Attachment
Men28.25.87299–36
Women30.0 **6.32324–36
Person Substitution
Men10.84.38110–19
Women13.6 ***4.97142–21
Animal Rights
Men11.23.00123–15
Women12.3 *2.67133–15
Table 4. Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS) results by living or not with children at home. * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001.
Table 4. Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS) results by living or not with children at home. * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001.
MeanSDMedianRange
LAPS Total score
Living with children46.413.54716–69
Living without children53.9 ***10.955.511–69
General Attachment
Living with children27.27.6294–36
Living without children30.5 *5.5327–36
Person Substitution
Living with children114.7103–21
Living without children13.6 ***4.7140–21
Animal Rights
Living with children10.73.2113–15
Living without children12.5 ***2.4133–15
Table 5. Results of the linear regression analysis for LAPS involving predictor variables of pet species and participants gender, age, living with or without children, and time spent with their pet(s). * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Reference level: cat, male, no children.
Table 5. Results of the linear regression analysis for LAPS involving predictor variables of pet species and participants gender, age, living with or without children, and time spent with their pet(s). * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Reference level: cat, male, no children.
Standardized β95% CItp
LAPS Total score
R2 = 0.247; adjR2 = 0.224; F(5, 163) = 10.74945, p < 0.001
Species0.3530.06–0.642.410.017 *
Gender0.3380.016–0.662.080.039 *
Age−0.253−0.41–−0.09−3.120.002 **
Children−0.419−0.76–−0.07−2.410.017 *
Time during weekend0.2470.11–0.393.46<0.001 ***
General Attachment
R2 = 0.179; adjR2 = 0.149; F(6, 162) = 5.92587, p < 0.001
Species0.264−0.04–0.571.680.091
Gender0.179−0.16–0.521.030.302
Age−0.162−0.33–0.01−1.980.059
Children−0.409−0.76–−0.04−2.240.028 *
Time during weekdays0.119−0.07–0.311.220.224
Time during weekend0.193−0.001–0.391.880.061
Person Substitution
R2 = 0.251; adjR2 = 0.233; F(4, 164) = 13.76174, p < 0.001
Species0.5530.27–0.833.83<0.001 ***
Gender0.5290.21–0.843.310.001 **
Age−0.251−0.41–−0.09−3.200.002 *
Children−0.309−0.65–0.03−1.7890.075
Animal Rights
R2 = 0.191; adjR2 = 0.161; F(6, 162) = 6.3890, p < 0.001
Species0.163−0.14–0.471.050.293
Gender0.3570.02–0.692.070.039 *
Age−0.208−0.37–−0.04−2.470.025 *
Children−0.497−0.85–−0.14−2.750.007 **
Time during weekdays0.096−0.09–0.280.980.326
Time during weekend0.112−0.08–0.311.090.275
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Egaña-Marcos, E.; Goñi-Balentziaga, O.; Azkona, G. A Study on the Attachment to Pets Among Owners of Cats and Dogs Using the Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS) in the Basque Country. Animals 2025, 15, 76. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15010076

AMA Style

Egaña-Marcos E, Goñi-Balentziaga O, Azkona G. A Study on the Attachment to Pets Among Owners of Cats and Dogs Using the Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS) in the Basque Country. Animals. 2025; 15(1):76. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15010076

Chicago/Turabian Style

Egaña-Marcos, Eider, Olatz Goñi-Balentziaga, and Garikoiz Azkona. 2025. "A Study on the Attachment to Pets Among Owners of Cats and Dogs Using the Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS) in the Basque Country" Animals 15, no. 1: 76. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15010076

APA Style

Egaña-Marcos, E., Goñi-Balentziaga, O., & Azkona, G. (2025). A Study on the Attachment to Pets Among Owners of Cats and Dogs Using the Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS) in the Basque Country. Animals, 15(1), 76. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15010076

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop