1. Introduction
Understanding the ontogeny of social behaviour in any mammal species ideally requires observation of animals at different stages between dependent infancy to reproduction. Among placental mammals, there are two main dependent infancy stage types: altricial and precocial. Among the Carnivora, altricial infants are typical of terrestrial species, while infants of the aquatic pinnipeds are all precocial. However, the pinnipeds are again divided phylogenetically into sea lions and fur seals (Otariidae), and walruses (Odobenidae) and seals (Phocidae) [
1].
The phocids diverged from the otariids around 25 MYA [
2]. Sea lions and fur seals retained dependence on land for breeding, limbs sufficient for agile locomotion on land, and all breed on land; they are all polygynous and sexually dimorphic, with breeding males competing for mating privileges. Phocid evolution headed towards a more fully marine orientation with the development of a thick subcutaneous fat layer (“blubber”) and a more streamlined body shape with external limbs too reduced to assist locomotion on land [
1,
2]. The extent of land (or ice) dependence for breeding and the degree of polygyny varies widely between species, from relatively little (such as spotted seals and Caspian seals) to extreme (elephant seals). The otariids all have a maternal dependency period varying from about six months to a year, whereas all phocid species have a lactation period ranging from only a few days (hooded seals) to about six weeks (e.g., monk seals), followed by several years of independence as juveniles before becoming a potentially breeding adult [
1].
During the later months of the lactation period, sea lion and fur seal mothers leave their pups onshore while they forage at sea; while their mothers are absent, pups gather in pods on the beach and interact socially, typically in a mode usually characterised as “play-fighting” [
3,
4,
5]. Less is known about the social interactions of phocid seal pups, likely because of their more aquatic habitat. After weaning, elephant seal pups engage in “play fighting” on their natal beach [
6,
7], although play fighting has not so far been documented in other phocid pups. However, the elephant seal belongs to the southern monachine group, which diverged from the northern phocids ~20 MYA [
2].
Grey and harbour seals are in a subfamily of northern phocids that probably diverged from other northern phocids ~6 MYA, while the grey seal (and its closest relative, the Caspian seal) diverged from the harbour seal ~2.5 MYA [
8]. Grey seals (
Halichoerus grypus) are sexually dimorphic, polygynous to a variable degree, and are dependent on ice or land for pupping. Adult grey seal social structure is complex, involving territoriality and dominance order among breeding adult males [
9] and inter-year mate fidelity [
10]. Harbour seals (
Phoca vitulina) are only slightly sexually dimorphic and are less dependent on land for breeding [
11]. Juvenile social interactions in both species can rarely be characterised as “play fighting”; rather, they are more appropriately described as “gentle play” [
12] (pp. 308–310), in which the seals either on land or in the water engage in repeated or protracted gentle body contact in the water involving twisting or somersaulting over each other, termed
rolling [
11,
13,
14,
15]. In the water, both species may engage in locomotor play, which includes splashing, bubble blowing, “torpedoing”, and “porpoising” or “high diving” [
11,
14,
16].
The present article focuses on the social development of young grey seals as observed in their natural habitat. Some introduction to the history of the grey seal or its natural environment may therefore be helpful to understanding the behaviour of young seals. The grey seal was historically a typical ice-breeding phocid seal, with the edge of fast ice or drifting floes of pack ice being the optimum habitat [
17,
18], although since the end of the last Ice Age, most populations now breed on land. The average pup density on pack ice in the Baltic has been estimated at 0.2 pups/100 m
2 compared to higher densities on land of up to 5.2 pups/100 m
2 [
18]. The female gives birth to only one infant, which interacts closely only with its mother. Lactation length averages ~18 d, at the end of which the mother leaves the pup abruptly. The weaned pups then stay onshore for ~1–4 weeks (average of ~3 weeks) before departing for the sea [
19,
20,
21]. During this period, pups at the relatively dense land-based colonies may encounter one another, whereas it seems likely that pups more sparsely distributed on ice floes rarely have that opportunity.
After departure for the sea, the pups may cover considerable distances over the next few months, exploring, gaining experience in navigation, and learning to forage [
22]. After leaving their natal site, therefore, pups may have little social contact with conspecifics for some weeks or months. This likely means that the lactation and post-weaning periods are critical for pup primary socialisation [
23].
Social contact play between mother and pup has been documented in harbour seals [
15,
24,
25] but the record for grey seals is less clear. A study of the social development of a zoo-born female pup (“Kara”) described brief contact play bouts immediately before or after suckling, during which mother and pup leaned their heads over each other and made gentle nose-to-nose contacts [
23], which have been previously described for juveniles during field observations [
14]. However, in a field study, observed pup play was limited to wriggling movements and did not involve interaction with the mother [
19]. Pup-to-mother nosing contacts only occurred <1% of the time in early lactation and up to ~2% of the time in mid or late lactation [
19,
26]. Pup-to-mother body nosing was considered to relate to pup soliciting suckling [
19] and play-like interaction to be the mother inciting suckling [
27].
After weaning, the grey seal pup has been described as
unsociable and hostile to all other seals [
28] or as aggressive towards other pups [
29]. The zoo study of pup “Kara” post-weaning found her to be initially
shy of any social contact, even with her mother, responding defensively by splashing or snorting. However, two months post-weaning, Kara began to follow the other seals, often nosing their hind-flippers and with locomotor play, although close contact was not observed [
23]. Post-weaning pups on the Isle of May (Scotland) gravitated to inland pools where they started to play exuberantly, with splashing and somersaulting. However, the play then observed was self- or object-directed; pups occasionally approached one another, but further contact was not seen [
19].
Thus, the social experience of young grey seal pups appears to be very limited, although subadults are known to be highly social at the water’s edge of haul-out sites [
14,
30]. Social interaction usually takes the form of one seal leaning its head over the other’s back as an “invitation”; play interaction then proceeds when this
head-over contact is reciprocated and continues with repeated reciprocal
head-over and variations thereof [
14]. The play appears to be socially contagious, with several dyads sometimes playing simultaneously [
30].
The aim of the present study is to delve further, by field observation, into the grey seal pups’ early experience of positive social interaction before and after weaning, their social interaction as subadults, and speculate how this may underlie the social cohesion of breeding colonies and courtship by breeding males. Because mother–pup social interaction has previously been found to occur infrequently, and mainly in relation to suckling bouts, this study aimed to focus on mother–pup social interaction around suckling bouts. Finally, the commonality of grey and harbour seal pup and subadult play is considered in terms of their social structure, respective habitats, and phylogenetic relationship.
5. Conclusions
Close olfactory contact and play-like affective touch in grey seals is a theme beginning in infancy with the mother and continuing intermittently through the years of immaturity to courtship, mating, and—in females—to raising the next generation of pups.
Grey seal mother-pup pairs in this study generally engaged in body-nosing contacts, while some pairs also engaged in contact play. The first break in the developmental continuum of close social contact came immediately after weaning when pups did not immediately transfer their close contact with their mothers to their peers; instead, they went through a period of exploring a new environment and making initial acquaintances with other pups. The defensive behaviours initially occurring in weaned pup encounters suggest this process is stressful. The play-like swimming, splashing, and play-displaying may serve to reduce stress, both in the performer and the spectator, and thereby facilitate preliminary positive social contact.
The consequences of the greater degree of sexual dimorphism and polygyny in grey seals are understood here to be superimposed seasonally on an essentially cohesive social structure, which is only superficially different from that in harbour seals. The commonality of contact play patterns between grey and harbour seals suggests that similar play patterns would also be predicted in other northern seals in the closely related genera Pusa and Phoca, which have radiated during the past 2.5 million years from a common ancestor.