The Ultimate List of the Most Frightening and Disgusting Animals: Negative Emotions Elicited by Animals in Central European Respondents
Abstract
:Simple Summary
Abstract
1. Introduction
- Find out what animals across the whole animal kingdom evoke the most intense fear or disgust in the nonclinical population, i.e., determine the scariest and most disgusting animals.
- Discuss which characteristics of the tested animals influence the evaluation (e.g., body size, venomousness, physical dangerousness).
- Analyze how respondents agree on the evaluation of pictures, i.e., whether they fear or are disgusted by the same animals.
- Explore which characteristics of respondents influence their evaluation of animals (e.g., gender, education, keeping pets, scores of questionnaires).
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Selection and Preparation of Stimuli
2.2. Testing Procedure
2.2.1. Participants
2.2.2. Questionnaires
2.2.3. Stimulus Ranking According to Fear or Disgust
2.3. Statistical Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Emotional Salience of the Examined Stimuli
3.2. Agreement among Respondents
3.3. Variability among Respondents
4. Discussion
4.1. Fear
- Evolutionary experience with an important predator of human/human ancestors and, thus, possible co-evolution.
- An animal representing an actual danger for contemporary humans who have direct or indirect experience with it. However, this category could also include cultural influences (e.g., movies, legends, and mythology), which are not always based on the real threat presented by the animal.
- Presence of features that might be perceived as weapons, features of possible aggressive motivation, or fighting ability (e.g., teeth, claws, large body size, or aposematic coloration).
4.1.1. Sharks (Lamniformes)
4.1.2. Crocodiles (Crocodylia)
4.1.3. Snakes (Squamata)
4.1.4. Big Cats and Bears (Carnivora)
4.1.5. Arachnids (Araneae, Scorpiones, Solifugae)
4.1.6. Hyenas and Wolves (Carnivora)
4.1.7. Large Herbivores (Artiodactyla, Perissodactyla, Proboscidea)
4.1.8. Piranha (Characiformes)
4.1.9. Insect (Hymenoptera)
4.2. Disgust
- Animals representing a real threat by spreading diseases or infections.
- Animals possessing visual features resembling primary disgust-evoking stimuli (such as animals perceived as slimy) but representing no or very little danger to humans.
- Venomous animals that arguably evoke both emotions, fear and disgust.
4.2.1. Endoparasites (Cyclophyllidea, Ascaridida)
4.2.2. Ectoparasitic Insects and Arachnids (Ixodida, Phthiraptera, Siphonaptera, Hemiptera)
4.2.3. Leeches (Arhynchobdellida)
4.2.4. Larvae (Coleoptera, Diptera)
4.2.5. Centipedes (Scolopendromorpha)
4.2.6. Mexican Mole Lizard and Earthworm (Squamata, Opisthopora)
4.2.7. Spiders (Araneae)
4.2.8. Frogs (Anura)
4.2.9. Snakes (Squamata)
4.3. Agreement among Respondents
4.4. Threshold
4.5. Effect of Respondents’ Characteristics
4.6. Limitations of the Study
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Wilson, E.O. Biofilia, Trad. It; Mondadori: Milano, Italy, 1985; Volume 189. [Google Scholar]
- Mormann, F.; Dubois, J.; Kornblith, S.; Milosavljevic, M.; Cerf, M.; Ison, M.; Fried, I. A category-specific response to animals in the right human amygdala. Nat. Neurosci. 2011, 14, 1247–1249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yang, J.; Bellgowan, P.S.; Martin, A. Threat, domain-specificity and the human amygdala. Neuropsychologia 2012, 50, 2566–2572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- New, J.; Cosmides, L.; Tooby, J. Category-specific attention for animals reflects ancestral priorities, not expertise. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2007, 104, 16598–16603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Headland, T.N.; Greene, H.W. Hunter–gatherers and other primates as prey, predators, and competitors of snakes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2011, 108, E1470–E1474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Njau, J.K.; Blumenschine, R.J. Crocodylian and mammalian carnivore feeding traces on hominid fossils from FLK 22 and FLK NN 3, Plio-Pleistocene, Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania. J. Hum. Evol. 2012, 63, 408–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Öhman, A.; Flykt, A.; Esteves, F. Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2001, 130, 466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yorzinski, J.L.; Penkunas, M.J.; Platt, M.L.; Coss, R.G. Dangerous animals capture and maintain attention in humans. Evol. Psychol. 2014, 12, 534–548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ekman, P.; Cordaro, D. What is meant by calling emotions basic. Emot. Rev. 2011, 3, 364–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Woody, S.R.; Teachman, B.A. Intersection of disgust and fear: Normative and pathological views. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 2000, 7, 291–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Curtis, V.; De Barra, M.; Aunger, R. Disgust as an adaptive system for disease avoidance behaviour. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2011, 366, 389–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Davey, G.C.L.; Marzillier, S. Disgust and animal phobias. In Disgust and Its Disorders: Theory, Assessment, and Treatment Implication; Olatunji, B.O., McKay, D., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2009; pp. 169–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davey, G.C. Disgust: The disease-avoidance emotion and its dysfunctions. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2011, 366, 3453–3465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fredrikson, M.; Annas, P.; Fischer, H.; Wik, G. Gender and age differences in the prevalence of specific fears and phobias. Behav. Res. Ther. 1996, 34, 33–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- LeBeau, R.T.; Glenn, D.; Liao, B.; Wittchen, H.U.; Beesdo-Baum, K.; Ollendick, T.; Craske, M.G. Specific phobia: A review of DSM-IV specific phobia and preliminary recommendations for DSM-V. Depress. Anxiety 2010, 27, 148–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oosterink, F.M.; De Jongh, A.; Hoogstraten, J. Prevalence of dental fear and phobia relative to other fear and phobia subtypes. Eur. J. Oral Sci. 2009, 117, 135–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Klorman, R.; Weerts, T.C.; Hastings, J.E.; Melamed, B.G.; Lang, P.J. Psychometric description of some specific-fear questionnaires. Behav. Ther. 1974, 5, 401–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Polák, J.; Sedláčková, K.; Nácar, D.; Landová, E.; Frynta, D. Fear the serpent: A psychometric study of snake phobia. Psychiatry Res. 2016, 242, 163–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ware, J.; Jain, K.; Burgess, I.; Davey, G.C. Disease-avoidance model: Factor analysis of common animal fears. Behav. Res. Ther. 1994, 32, 57–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davey, G.C. Self-reported fears to common indigenous animals in an adult UK population: The role of disgust sensitivity. Br. J. Psychol. 1994, 85, 541–554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Matchett, G.; Davey, G.C. A test of a disease-avoidance model of animal phobias. Behav. Res. Ther. 1991, 29, 91–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tucker, M.; Bond, N.W. The roles of gender, sex role, and disgust in fear of animals. Personal. Individ. Differ. 1997, 22, 135–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rádlová, S.; Janovcová, M.; Sedláčková, K.; Polák, J.; Nácar, D.; Peléšková, Š.; Frynta, D.; Landová, E. Snakes represent emotionally salient stimuli that may evoke both fear and disgust. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 1085. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Landová, E.; Marešová, J.; Šimková, O.; Cikánová, V.; Frynta, D. Human responses to live snakes and their photographs: Evaluation of beauty and fear of the king snakes. J. Environ. Psychol. 2012, 32, 69–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frynta, D.; Peléšková, Š.; Rádlová, S.; Janovcová, M.; Landová, E. Human evaluation of amphibian species: A comparison of disgust and beauty. Sci. Nat. 2019, 106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Janovcová, M.; Rádlová, S.; Polák, J.; Sedláčková, K.; Peléšková, Š.; Žampachová, B.; Frynta, D.; Landová, E. Human attitude toward reptiles: A relationship between fear, disgust, and aesthetic preferences. Animals 2019, 9, 238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Prokop, P.; Uşak, M.; Erdoğan, M.; Fančovičová, J.; Bahar, M. Slovakian and Turkish students’ fear, disgust and perceived danger of invertebrates. Hacettepe Üniversitesi Eğitim Fakültesi Dergisi 2011, 40, 344–352. Available online: https://dergipark.org.tr/en/pub/hunefd/issue/7796/102081 (accessed on 24 September 2020).
- Marešová, J.; Frynta, D. Noah’s Ark is full of common species attractive to humans: The case of boid snakes in zoos. Ecol. Econ. 2008, 64, 554–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Landová, E.; Bakhshaliyeva, N.; Janovcová, M.; Peléšková, Š.; Suleymanova, M.; Polák, J.; Guliev, A.; Frynta, D. Association between fear and beauty evaluation of snakes: Cross cultural findings. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lišková, S.; Frynta, D. What determines bird beauty in human eyes? Anthrozoös 2013, 26, 27–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Landová, E.; Poláková, P.; Rádlová, S.; Janovcová, M.; Bobek, M.; Frynta, D. Beauty ranking of mammalian species kept in the Prague Zoo: Does beauty of animals increase the respondents’ willingness to protect them? Sci. Nat. 2018, 105, 69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frynta, D.; Šimková, O.; Lišková, S.; Landová, E. Mammalian collection on Noah’s ark: The effects of beauty, brain and body size. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e63110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Possidónio, C.; Graça, J.; Piazza, J.; Prada, M. Animal images database: Validation of 120 images for human-animal studies. Animals 2019, 9, 475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Polák, J.; Rádlová, S.; Janovcová, M.; Flegr, J.; Landová, E.; Frynta, D. Scary and nasty beasts: Self-reported fear and disgust of common phobic animals. Br. J. Psychol. 2020, 111, 297–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Peléšková, Š. Positive and Negative Attitudes towards Animals. Master’s Thesis, Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Polák, J.; Sedláčková, K.; Frynta, D.; Landová, E. Standardization and psychometric characteristics of two spider fear assessments. Unpublished data.
- Haidt, J.; McCauley, C.; Rozin, P. Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Personal. Individ. Differ. 1994, 16, 701–713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Olatunji, B.O.; Williams, N.L.; Tolin, D.F.; Abramowitz, J.S.; Sawchuk, C.N.; Lohr, J.M.; Elwood, L.S. The Disgust Scale: Item analysis, factor structure, and suggestions for refinement. Psychol. Assess. 2007, 19, 281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Polák, J.; Landová, E.; Frynta, D. Undisguised disgust: A psychometric evaluation of a disgust propensity measure. Curr. Psychol. 2019, 38, 608–617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marešová, J.; Krása, A.; Frynta, D. We all appreciate the same animals: Cross cultural comparison of human aesthetic preferences for snake species in Papua New Guinea and Europe. Ethology 2009, 115, 297–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lišková, S.; Landová, E.; Frynta, D. Human preferences for colorful birds: Vivid colors or pattern? Evol. Psychol. 2015, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pohlert, T. The Pairwise Multiple Comparison of Mean Ranks Package (PMCMR). R Package 2014. Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=PMCMR (accessed on 24 September 2020).
- Oksanen, J.; Blanchet, F.G.; Friendly, M.; Kindt, R.; Legendre, P.; McGlinn, D. Vegan: Community Ecology Package. R Package Version 2.4–5 2017. Available online: https://CRAN.Rproject.org/package=vegan (accessed on 24 September 2020).
- R Development Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- StatSoft Inc. Statistica (Data Analysis Software System), Version 13. Available online: http://www.statsoft.com/ (accessed on 20 July 2020).
- Rozin, P.; Haidt, J.; Fincher, K. From oral to moral. Science 2009, 323, 1179–1180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hart, D. Predation on primates: A biogeographical analysis. In Primate Anti-Predator Strategies. Developments in Primatology: Progress and Prospects; Gursky, S.L., Nekaris, K.A.I., Eds.; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 2007; pp. 27–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Muter, B.A.; Gore, M.L.; Gledhill, K.S.; Lamont, C.; Huveneers, C. Australian and US news media portrayal of sharks and their conservation. Conserv. Biol. 2013, 27, 187–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- International Shark Attack File. Available online: https://www.floridamuseum.ufl.edu/shark-attacks/ (accessed on 8 December 2020).
- Batt, S. Human attitudes towards animals in relation to species similarity to humans: A multivariate approach. Biosci. Horiz. 2009, 2, 180–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cook, M.; Mineka, S. Observational conditioning of fear to fear-relevant versus fear irrelevant stimuli in rhesus monkeys. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1989, 98, 448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caldicott, D.G.; Croser, D.; Manolis, C.; Webb, G.; Britton, A. Crocodile attack in Australia: An analysis of its incidence and review of the pathology and management of crocodilian attacks in general. Wilderness Environ. Med. 2005, 16, 143–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- CrocBITE 2013, The Worldwide Crocodilian Attack Database. Available online: http://www.crocodile-attack.info/ (accessed on 8 December 2020).
- Öhman, A.; Mineka, S. The malicious serpent: Snakes as a prototypical stimulus for an evolved module of fear. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2003, 12, 5–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Isbell, L.A. Snakes as Agents of Evolutionary Change in Primate Brains. J. Hum. Evol. 2006, 51, 1–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- World Health Organization. Snakebite Envenoming 2019. Available online: http://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/snakebite-envenoming (accessed on 26 November 2020).
- Landová, E.; Peléšková, Š.; Sedláčková, K.; Janovcová, M.; Polák, J.; Rádlová, S.; Vobrubová, B.; Frynta, D. Venomous snakes elicit stronger fear than nonvenomous ones: Psychophysiological response to snake images. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0236999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hart, D.; Sussman, R.W. Man the Hunted: Primates, Predators, and Human Evolution; Westview Press: New York, NY, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Packer, C.; Hilborn, R.; Mosser, A.; Kissui, B.; Borner, M.; Hopcraft, G.; Wilmshurst, J.; Mduma, S.; Sinclair, A.R. Ecological change, group territoriality, and population dynamics in Serengeti lions. Science 2005, 307, 390–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Löe, J.; Röskaft, E. Large carnivores and human safety: A review. AMBIO J. Hum. Environ. 2004, 33, 283–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lorenz, K. The innate forms of potential experience. Z. Tierpsychol. 1943, 5, 235–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Corcoran, W.C. Big Cats Around the Globe: How Communities Around the World Perceive Their Backyard Big Cats. Bachelor’s Thesis, The University of Vermont, Burlington, VT, USA, 2019. Available online: https://scholarworks.uvm.edu/envstheses/61 (accessed on 30 November 2020).
- Shepherd, S.M.; Mills, A.; Shoff, W.H. Human attacks by large felid carnivores in captivity and in the wild. Wilderness Environ. Med. 2014, 25, 220–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Chippaux, J.P.; Goyffon, M. Epidemiology of scorpionism: A global appraisal. Acta Trop. 2008, 107, 71–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vetter, R.S.; Draney, M.L.; Brown, C.A.; Trumble, J.T.; Gouge, D.H.; Hinkle, N.C.; Pace-Schott, E.F. Spider Fear Versus Scorpion Fear in Undergraduate Students at Five American Universities. Am. Entomol. 2018, 64, 79–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- White, J. Bites and stings from venomous animals: A global overview. Ther. Drug Monit. 2000, 22, 65–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nentwig, W.; Kuhn-Nentwig, L. Spider venoms potentially lethal to humans. In Spider Ecophysiology; Nentwig, W., Ed.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2013; pp. 253–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Langley, R.L. Animal-related fatalities in the United States—An update. Wilderness Environ. Med. 2005, 16, 67–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davey, G.C. The “disgusting” spider: The role of disease and illness in the perpetuation of fear of spiders. Soc. Anim. 1994, 2, 17–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Linnell, J.; Andersen, R.; Andersone, Z.; Balciauskas, L.; Blanco, J.C.; Boitani, L.; Wabakken, P. The Fear of Wolves: A Review of Wolf Attacks on Humans; NINA: Trondheim, Norway, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Treves, A.; Bonacic, C. Humanity’s dual response to dogs and wolves. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2016, 31, 489–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Torres, D.F.; Oliveira, E.S.; Alves, R.R.N. Understanding human–wildlife conflicts and their implications. In Ethnozoology; Alves, R.R.N., Albuquerque, U.P., Eds.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2018; pp. 421–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Treves, A.; Naughton-Treves, L. Risk and opportunity for humans coexisting with large carnivores. J. Hum. Evol. 1999, 36, 275–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Haddad, V., Jr.; Sazima, I. Piranha attacks on humans in southeast Brazil: Epidemiology, natural history, and clinical treatment, with description of a bite outbreak. Wilderness Environ. Med. 2003, 14, 249–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sazima, I.; de Andrade Guimaraes, S. Scavenging on human corpses as a source for stories about man-eating piranhas. Environ. Biol. Fishes 1987, 20, 75–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Český statistický úřad (Czech Statistical Office). Available online: https://www.czso.cz/csu/czso/ceska-republika-podle-pohlavi-a-veku-2008-2017-b1c67xq334 (accessed on 30 November 2020).
- Schmidt, J.O. Hymenoptera venoms: Striving toward theultimate defense against vertebrates. In Insect defences. Adaptive Mechanisms and Strategiesof Prey and Predators; Evansm, D.L., Schmidt, J.O., Eds.; State University of New York Press: Albany, NY, USA, 1990; pp. 387–419. [Google Scholar]
- Ekman, P. An argument for basic emotions. Cogn. Emot. 1992, 6, 169–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kellert, S.R. Values and perceptions of invertebrates. Conserv. Biol. 1993, 7, 845–855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- World Health Organization. Disease Burden and Mortality Estimates. Available online: https://www.who.int/healthinfo/global_burden_disease/estimates/en/ (accessed on 26 November 2020).
- Curtis, V.; Biran, A. Dirt, disgust, and disease: Is hygiene in our genes? Perspect. Biol. Med. 2001, 44, 17–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Davey, G.C.L. Characteristics of individuals with fear of spiders. Anxiety Res. 1991, 4, 299–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- World Health Organization. Soil-Transmitted Helminth Infections 2020. Available online: http://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/soil-transmitted-helminth-infections (accessed on 26 November 2020).
- Prokop, P.; Fančovičová, J. The association between disgust, danger and fear of macroparasites and human behaviour. Acta Ethol. 2010, 13, 57–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- World Health Organization. Vector-Borne Diseases 2020. Available online: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/vector-borne-diseases (accessed on 26 November 2020).
- European Centre for Disease Prevention and Control. Annual Epidemiological Report for 2018, Tick-Borne Encephalitis. Available online: https://www.ecdc.europa.eu/sites/default/files/documents/TBE-annual-epidemiological-report-2018.pdf (accessed on 26 November 2020).
- Delaunay, P.; Blanc, V.; Del Giudice, P.; Levy-Bencheton, A.; Chosidow, O.; Marty, P.; Brouqui, P. Bedbugs and infectious diseases. Clin. Infect. Dis. 2011, 52, 200–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pritchard, M.J.; Hwang, S.W. Severe anemia from bedbugs. Cmaj 2009, 181, 287–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Reinhardt, K. Bedbug; Reaktion Books: London, UK, 2018; pp. 1–184. [Google Scholar]
- Nehili, M.; Ilk, C.; Mehlhorn, H.; Ruhnau, K.; Dick, W.; Njayou, M. Experiments on the possible role of leeches as vectors of animal and human pathogens: A light and electron microscopy study. Parasitol. Res. 1994, 80, 277–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krüger, C.; Malleyeck, I.; Olsen, O.H. Aquatic leech infestation: A rare cause of severe anaemia in an adolescent Tanzanian girl. Eur. J. Pediatrics 2004, 163, 297–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bilgen, C.; Karci, B.; Uluöz, Ü. A nasopharyngeal mass: Leech in the nasopharynx. Int. J. Pediatric Otorhinolaryngol. 2002, 64, 73–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rozin, P.; Haidt, J.; McCauley, C. Disgust. In Handbook of Emotions; Lewis, M., Haviland, J.M., Eds.; The Guilford Publications: New York, NY, USA, 2000; pp. 637–653. [Google Scholar]
- Balit, C.R.; Harvey, M.S.; Waldock, J.M.; Isbister, G.K. Prospective study of centipede bites in Australia. J. Toxicol. Clin. Toxicol. 2004, 42, 41–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Supakthanasiri, P.; Ruxrungtham, K.; Klaewsongkram, J.; Chantaphakul, H. Anaphylaxis to centipede bite. J. Allergy Clin. Immunol. 2004, 113, S244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gerdes, A.B.; Uhl, G.; Alpers, G.W. Spiders are special: Fear and disgust evoked by pictures of arthropods. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2009, 30, 66–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tolin, D.F.; Lohr, J.M.; Sawchuk, C.N.; Lee, T.C. Disgust and disgust sensitivity in blood-injection-injury and spider phobia. Behav. Res. Ther. 1997, 35, 949–953. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thorpe, S.J.; Salkovskis, P.M. Studies on the role of disgust in the acquisition and maintenance of specific phobias. Behav. Res. Ther. 1998, 36, 877–893. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, M.; Davidson, J. It Makes My Skin Crawl...’: The Embodiment of Disgust in Phobias of ‘Nature. Body Soc. 2006, 12, 43–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bennett-Levy, J.; Marteau, T. Fear of animals: What is prepared? Br. J. Psychol. 1984, 75, 37–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ceríaco, L.M. Human attitudes towards herpetofauna: The influence of folklore and negative values on the conservation of amphibians and reptiles in Portugal. J. Ethnobiol. Ethnomed. 2012, 8, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Daly, J.W.; Brown, G.B.; Mensah-Dwumah, M.; Myers, C.W. Classification of skin alkaloids from neotropical poison-dart frogs (Dendrobatidae). Toxicon 1978, 16, 163–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rádlová, S.; Polák, J.; Janovcová, M.; Sedláčková, K.; Peléšková, Š.; Landová, E.; Frynta, D. Emotional Reaction to Fear-and Disgust-Evoking Snakes: Sensitivity and Propensity in Snake-Fearful Respondents. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frynta, D.; Lišková, S.; Bültmann, S.; Burda, H. Being attractive brings advantages: The case of parrot species in captivity. PLoS ONE 2010, 5, e12568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frynta, D.; Marešová, J.; Řeháková-Petrů, M.; Šklíba, J.; Šumbera, R.; Krása, A. Cross-cultural agreement in perception of animal beauty: Boid snakes viewed by people from five continents. Hum. Ecol. 2011, 39, 829–834. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Herzog, H.A. Gender differences in human–animal interactions: A review. Anthrozoös 2007, 20, 7–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Abbreviation | Variable | Definition |
---|---|---|
SNAQ | Snake Questionnaire | Questionnaire measuring fear of snakes |
SPQ | Spider Questionnaire | Questionnaire measuring fear of spiders |
DS-R | Disgust Scale-Revised | Questionnaire evaluating disgust propensity |
AFQ | Animal Fear Questionnaire | Questionnaire testing a nonspecific/general level of fear from different animal groups |
W | Kendall’s coefficient of concordance | Nonparametric statistic used for assessing agreement among respondents |
RDA | Redundancy analysis | Statistical method analyzing the relationship between multiple response and explanatory variables |
LM | Linear model | Mathematical method describing a continuous response variable as a function of one or more predictor variables |
R | Spearman’s correlation | Coefficient describing the strength of a link between two sets of data |
R2 | Coefficient of determination | Descriptive measure of goodness-of-fit for linear models, indicating the percentage of the variance in the dependent variable that is explained by the independent variable(s) |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Staňková, H.; Janovcová, M.; Peléšková, Š.; Sedláčková, K.; Landová, E.; Frynta, D. The Ultimate List of the Most Frightening and Disgusting Animals: Negative Emotions Elicited by Animals in Central European Respondents. Animals 2021, 11, 747. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11030747
Staňková H, Janovcová M, Peléšková Š, Sedláčková K, Landová E, Frynta D. The Ultimate List of the Most Frightening and Disgusting Animals: Negative Emotions Elicited by Animals in Central European Respondents. Animals. 2021; 11(3):747. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11030747
Chicago/Turabian StyleStaňková, Helena, Markéta Janovcová, Šárka Peléšková, Kristýna Sedláčková, Eva Landová, and Daniel Frynta. 2021. "The Ultimate List of the Most Frightening and Disgusting Animals: Negative Emotions Elicited by Animals in Central European Respondents" Animals 11, no. 3: 747. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11030747