Next Article in Journal
Gendered Career Pathways among Doctoral Graduates in the United Kingdom
Previous Article in Journal
Flipped Classroom to Improve University Student Centered Learning and Academic Performance
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Who Is Concerned about Terrorist Attacks? A Religious Profile

1
Universidade Católica Portuguesa, Faculdade de Filosofia e Ciências Sociais, Rua de Camões 60, 4710-362 Braga, Portugal
2
Universidade Europeia, Faculdade de Turismo e Hospitalidade, Rua Laura Ayres, 4, 1650-510 Lisboa, Portugal
3
UFP Energy, Environment and Health Research Unit (FP-ENAS), University Fernando Pessoa (UFP), Praça 9 de Abril 349, 4249-004 Porto, Portugal
4
Department of Mathematics (DM. UTAD), University of Trás-os-Montes and Alto Douro, Quinta de Prados, 5001-801 Vila Real, Portugal
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Soc. Sci. 2019, 8(11), 316; https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci8110316
Submission received: 14 October 2019 / Revised: 11 November 2019 / Accepted: 13 November 2019 / Published: 16 November 2019
(This article belongs to the Section Contemporary Politics and Society)

Abstract

:
As part of the study on the psychological impact of terrorist acts on ordinary people, the objective of this study is to understand if religious identity protects individuals from feeling concerned about the possibility of terrorist attacks. The study was based on a sample from the World Values Survey, wave 6 (2010–2014), of 30,446 citizens of countries whose dominant religion is Christianity. According to the concern felt regarding the possibility of becoming the target of a terrorist attack, a religious profile was identified. Most of the sample reported high levels of worry about terrorist attacks. The most religious respondents, more faithful and more devoted to religious practices, are more worried about the occurrence of terrorist attacks. Opposite to what is mostly found in the literature, religion does not act as a protective barrier to the primary objective of terrorism, which consists in the use of violence to create fear. People worried about the probability of becoming a target in terrorist attacks are also victims of terrorism.

1. Introduction

Terrorism consists in the use of violence to create fear (i.e., terror, psychic fear) for political, religious, or ideological reasons. Terror is intentionally aimed at non-combatant targets, i.e., civilians or iconic symbols, and its objective is to achieve the greatest attainable publicity for a group, cause, or individual (Matusitz 2013). According to the Global Terrorism Index, from the Institute of Economics and Peace (IEP 2016), based on the Global Terrorism Database (START 2016), terrorism is a form of violence that mostly targets a small number of countries and is perpetrated by a small number of groups. The overall figures of terrorism are quite significant, with over 150,000 terrorist attacks occurring between 1970 and 2015 around the world, including 75,000 bombings, 17,000 assassinations, and 9000 kidnappings (START 2016). The IEP (2016) report notes that high levels of terrorist activity are related to high levels of political terror and political instability and low respect of human rights and religious freedoms for the United Nations (UN) or the European Union (EU). Several studies explaining the reasons for these attacks have been carried out, namely, political (McCauley and Moskalenko 2008; Ömer 2009; Midlarsky 2011), social (King and Taylor 2011; Mink 2015), economic (De Mesquita 2008; Caruso and Locatelli 2014), cultural (Pisoiu 2014; Shaffer 2015; Kluch and Vaux 2017), religious (Jefferis 2009; Kingsley 2010; Rapoport 2013; Ross 2015; Feyyaz 2016; Laqueur 2017), and psychological reasons (Twemlow 2005; Berko 2007; Post et al. 2009; Horgan 2012; Perliger et al. 2016).
Although it is unlikely for the individual to become a victim of terrorism (low base rates), most people feel the terrorist threat when being reminded about terrorism (e.g., newspaper articles, pictures of attacks, political discussion) (Fischer et al. 2011). Exposure to terrorist attacks provided by the media (Walsh 2010; Sensales et al. 2014) makes terrorism a present phenomenon in the day-to-day life, with evident consequences, particularly psychological ones (Kaitz et al. 2009; Waxman 2011; Huq 2013). In addition, people understand that the terrorism struggle, since 11 September 2001, failed to achieve its objectives, remaining a global political agenda and posing a serious threat to world peace (Jamal 2014). Moreover, the internal security of each person does not restore itself because external measures are being taken and, according to Leonhard (2005), the enemy demonstrates innovation and adaptability.
Considering religion, the feelings, acts, and experiences of individual men in their solitude, so far as he apprehends himself to stand in relation to whatever may be considered the divine (Ireland 2018), and a symbolic transformation of experience (O’Dea and Aviad 1983), religion means different things to different people, according to social and cultural contexts and corresponding mind-sets (Küçükcan 2000). Religiosity refers to the aspects of religious activity, dedication, and belief (Cornwall et al. 1986). It is the state of being religious (O’Brien and Palmer 2000) and a belief in God together with a commitment to follow God principles (McDaniel and Burnett 1990). Religiosity affects the preferences of individuals (Esteban et al. 2015), exerting effects on individual decisions and behavior, and its intensity affects social interactions and attitudes (Brañas-Garza et al. 2013). The relationship between religion and terrorism has long been studied in the literature (Hoffman 1995; Jefferis 2009; Rapoport 2013; Feyyaz 2016; Laqueur 2017). “Religion-inspired terrorism appeared on the fringes of all major (and some minor) religions including Christianity, Judaism, and even Buddhism, but it was more frequent among Islamic groups” (Laqueur 2017, p. xi). Although religious decline is a general development across some countries, some scholars (Voas 2008; Kupor et al. 2015; Jonas and Fischer 2006) consider that religion has a protective effect on people’s lives. People endorse more strongly a conception of God with a protective side rather than with a punitive one (Shariff and Rhemtulla 2012), suggesting that the religiosity of each one may be predominant in the way terrorism is faced daily.
Some studies report the differences between religious and non-religious people (Kosmin et al. 2009; Pennycook et al. 2016), and between believers and non-believers (nones) (Jing 2014; Lin et al. 2016; Lindeman and Lipsanen 2016). A high probability of terrorism only negatively affects the mood of non-religious participants but not that of intrinsically religious persons. On a situation of high salience of terrorism, non-religious experience fewer positive emotions and less self-efficacy than intrinsically religious people. On a situation of low salience of terrorism, no differences were found between non-religious and intrinsically religious regarding mood and self-efficacy (Fischer et al. 2006). High perceived terrorist threat has serious effects on individual and collective psychological responses (Fischer et al. 2007; Fischer and Ai 2008; Fischer et al. 2010, 2011; Kastenmüller et al. 2011, 2014), though finding meaning in terrorism has been associated with low posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms and more emotional well-being (Updegraff et al. 2008; Fischer et al. 2011). However, the impact of terrorist violence and damage reaches more than the immediate target victims (Matusitz 2013). Experiences of anxiety or perceived threat in response to terrorism lead to an overestimation of risk and risk-aversive behavior (Lerner and Keltner 2001). There is an association of perceived threat with increases in intolerance, prejudice, ethnocentrism, and xenophobia (Huddy et al. 2005). Females experience more pessimistic risk than males, emotion differences explaining 60% to 80% of the gender difference (Lerner et al. 2003). After a collective trauma, individuals frequently perceive positive changes or benefits in others or in society, namely, prosocial behavior, religiosity, or political engagement (Poulin et al. 2009).
The relationship between terrorism and religion is usually studied in light of religious terrorism. However, as part of the study on the psychological impact of terrorist acts on ordinary people, this study aimed to understand if religious identity protects individuals from feeling concerned about the possibility of terrorist attacks. A religious profile of these citizens, regarding the concern felt with the possibility of becoming the target of a terrorist attack, was identified. The authors suppose that most people worry about being targeted by a terrorist attack (1); and more religious people feel less worry than less religious or non-religious (2), possibly due to the protective nature of religion.

2. Results

2.1. Sample Characterization

Women were slightly more numerous than men (56% against 44%). Respondents were aged from 16 to 97 years (M = 44.30, SD = 17.68). Most of the sample have a medium education (56%), are married or live together (61%), and are professionally active (51%) (see Table 1).
The sociodemographic and religious profile reveals statistically significant differences in the distribution of the sample by the different levels of concern about the possibility of being the target of a terrorist attack (see Table 2).

2.2. Worries about a Terrorist Attack

Most of the sample is worried about the fact that it could be the target of a terrorist attack (64%), with almost half of the sample being very worried (41%) (see Table 3).
There are significant differences across countries (F(30) = 299.100; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.228). First comes Rwanda, revealing a tendency to be concerned about terrorist attacks (3.89), followed by Georgia (3.63). In third and fourth places are Mexico (3.62) and Colombia (3.56). Armenia (3.47), Philippines (3.42), Peru (3.35), Ghana (3.34), Ecuador (3.18), and Russia (3.16) presented values above 3. Between the values of 2.92 and 2.51, Brazil, Zimbabwe, Belarus, Ukraine, Romania, Spain, United States, South Africa, and Poland can be found. Cyprus, Estonia, Trinidad and Tobago, Australia, Chile, Germany, Slovenia, Uruguay, and New Zealand have values between 2.42 and 2.11. Only Argentina (1.99), Sweden (1.94), and the Netherlands (1.90) present values below 2.
Women are more likely to present greater levels of concern than men (2.94 vs. 2.86; F(1) = 34.167, p < 0.001; η2 = 0.001). Regarding age, older people (>65) are less concerned about terrorist attacks (2.70) than the other groups: 16–24 years old, 2.95; 25–44 years old, 2.99; and 45–64 years old, 2.87; (F(3) = 88.549; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.009). Subjects who are married/living together (2.93) present higher values than those not married/not living together (2.86) (F(1) = 25.350, p < 0.001; η2 = 0.001). Respondents with no formal education report significantly higher levels of concern (3.36) than those with low (2.93) and medium (2.92) education, and the respondents with high education are the ones showing less concern with terrorist attacks (2.70; F(3) = 47.545; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.005). Professionally active respondents tend to report significantly more worry than non-active ones (2.92 vs. 2.88; F(1) = 9.667; p < 0.002; η2 = 0.000).

2.3. Religiosity—Religious Identity

In Table 4, the distribution of the items that define religious identity is presented and it can be found that the clear majority of the sample consider themselves religious (81%), considering religion very important (49%). Also, the overwhelming majority believe in God (94%), assuming that God is very important in their lives (60%).
Regarding worries about a terrorist attack, there are significant differences in religious identity. In what concerns the first item religious person (F(1) = 381.978; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.012), subjects who do not consider themselves religious or who consider themselves atheists have lower values of concern (2.65) than those who consider themselves religious (2.96). The same happens when analyzing the item about the importance of religion (F(3) = 406.842; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.039): Participants for whom religion is not definitely important present the lowest values of concern for terrorist attacks (2.44), followed by subjects for whom religion is not important (2.64), and then followed by subjects for whom religion is important (2.81) and very important (3.11). Subjects who believe in God have higher values of concern (2.89) than those who do not believe in God (2.39; F(1) = 442.451; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.014). Participants who consider that God is very important (3.09) have significantly higher values than those who consider that God is not important (2.44), little important (2.58), or just important (2.69; F(3) = 466.066; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.044).

2.4. Religiosity—Religious Practice

Concerning the religious practice, only 30% of the sample are an active member of the church and 40% attend religious services at least once a month; most of the sample (68%) prays at least once a day (see Table 5).
Regarding worries about a terrorist attack, significant differences in religious practice were found. Concerning the first item active/inactive membership of a church or religious organization, the participants who are active members (2.97) have higher values than those who are not members (2.89) or those who are inactive members (2.84; F(2) = 31.402; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.002). Regarding the question How often do you attend religious services, subjects who attend very little or never attend religious services present less concern about the terrorist attacks (2.72) than the participants who go once a year (2.76), on special days (2.81), once a month (2.94), and at least once a week (3.05; F(4) = 146.351; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.016). With regard to the item How often to you pray, subjects who never pray have lower values (2.51) than those who pray once a year (2.78), on special days (2.74), several days a week (2.95), and every day (3.09; F(4) = 197.584; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.021).

3. Discussion

This study aimed to understand if religious identity protects individuals from feeling concerned about the possibility of terrorist attacks. The study is justified by (1) the high number of terrorist attacks between 1970 and 2015, (2) the psychological negative impact of terrorist attacks, and (3) the results of studies that point to the religion protective effect on people’s lives. Data used in the study were collected from wave 6 (2010–2014) of the World Values Survey (World Values Survey 2016). The innovative attractiveness of this article emerges from the results that mostly contradict the literature.
Most respondents reported high levels of worry about terrorist attacks, confirming the first hypothesis, which predicted that most people worry about being targeted by a terrorist attack. This can be explained by the overall numbers of terrorism, mentioned above, that are quite impressive (over 150,000 terrorist attacks between 1970 and 2015 around the world) and whose mass circulation is widespread through the media. Although there are few opportunities for the individual to become a victim of terrorism, most people feel threatened when being reminded about it through newspaper articles, pictures of attacks, etc. (Fischer et al. 2010). As internet-based contact is not regulated by the same standards and ethics as those of traditional print and television journalism, it may be possible to be inferred that this kind of information is mostly misinformation (Comer 2019), which may contribute to the perception of threat. Disruptive events, such as terrorist attacks, have many impacts both at the psychosocial and societal level; post-traumatic stress disorder, depression, fear, and anxiety are frequently associated with terrorist attacks (Oksanen et al. 2018). Also, danger perception of terror attacks was reported as being associated with both psychological distress and with severe stress symptoms (Mahat-Shamir et al. 2018).
Based on the data in this study, and regarding sociodemographic characteristics, countries where terrorist attacks seem to be most feared are unstable countries from a political point of view and with a history of social violence. This is in accordance with Shechory-Bitton and Cohen-Louck (2018), who reported that this fear of terrorism is perceived differently than other types of fear, namely, because of the geographic location. Participants who are male, older, not married/not living together, with higher education, and professionally non-active revealed less worries than participants who are women, younger, married/living together, less educated, and professionally active. Lerner et al. (2003) stated that females experienced more pessimistic risk than males, which may explain the results in relation to gender. Also, Cohen-Louck and Levy (2018) found that men reported higher perceptions than women regarding risk perception of terrorism. Contrary to the results in this study, Williamson et al. (2019) found a positive association between fear of terrorist attacks and age and a negative one with married participants (less fear). However, the same authors reported that as educational achievement increases, the fear of terrorism decreases, in agreement with the results in this study. Subjects who are married/living together present higher values than those who are not married/not living together, possibly due to the fact that having a family makes them feel more concerned about their future in general and with terrorist threats, in particular. Respondents with no formal education report significantly higher levels of concern than those with more education, relating to the fact that less educated people may be less aware of the significance of the probability of a terrorist attack. Professionally active respondents tend to report significantly more worry than non-active ones, possibly because non-active people have less to lose than active individuals.
Most of the sample considers itself religious, with being religion very important. Also, this majority believe in God and consider that God is very important in their lives. These results seem to contradict those of other authors (Voas 2008; Voas and Chaves 2018), who state that there is a religious decline in all countries, mainly in religious practice (Molteni and Biolcati 2018), supporting the ‘believing without belonging’ theory (Davie 1990; Flanagan and Davie 1995), which suggests a widening gap between stable or increasing private and intimate religiosity and decreasing public religiosity.
Subjects who do not consider themselves religious or who consider themselves atheists have less concern with terrorist attacks than those who consider themselves religious. Also, participants for whom religion is not important present the lowest values of concern for terrorist attacks than subjects for whom religion is important. These results do not confirm the supposed second hypothesis, which predicts that more religious people feel less worry about terrorist attacks. These results also seem to contradict those found by Fischer et al. (2006), who claim that intrinsic religiosity helps to cope with the increased salience of terrorism; and even that high probability of terrorism only negatively affects the mood of non-religious participants but not of intrinsically religious persons. However, Adamczyk and LaFree (2015) showed that more religious respondents are more likely to express concerns about terrorism, although this relationship were mediated by their level of conservatism. Also, Haner et al. (2019) found that gender, religiosity, and psychological distress were most consistently associated with fear of terrorism and worry about being a victim of a terrorist attack.
Subjects who believe in God and consider that God is important present more concern than those who do not believe in God and consider that God is not important. These results may be in accordance with Jong and Halberstadt (2016), who found that the narrative reporting that non-religious people fear death more than religious people is not necessarily true. According to Pennycook et al. (2016), there is evidence that atheists and agnostics are more reflective than religious believers, which may explain the results obtained in this study.
Almost a third of the sample are an active member of the church, about two in five attend religious services at least once a month, and the majority prays at least once a day. Active members present higher values of worry than those who are not members or inactive members. Subjects who attend very little or never attend religious services and that never pray present less concern about the terrorist attacks than the participants that do it. These results clearly contradict those of Kupor et al. (2015), as well as those of Jonas and Fischer (2006), who consider that religion has a protective effect on people’s lives. Although attending religious services has been shown to be one of the strongest religious predictors of well-being (George et al. 2002), it only happens with those who internalize their religious beliefs and try to live out their religion on a daily basis; otherwise, anxiety may emerge (Steffen et al. 2017), contributing to the fear of terrorism.

3.1. Conclusion

The results of this study seem to question the idea that religion has a protective effect on people’s lives, in the sense that the more religious believers and assiduous persons in religious practices are not protected from feeling more concern about terrorist attacks. It was found that religiosity is not protective of concern created by violence, which is the primary target of terrorism (Kupor et al. 2015). This study suggests, in accordance with the one from Brañas-Garza et al. (2013), that religiosity impacts on individual decisions and behavior, and its intensity affects social interactions and attitudes. Thus, it is possible to state that the more religious participants are other victims of terrorist attacks, although not the immediate ones (Matusitz 2013). This study contributes to the body of knowledge about religion and terrorism as it sheds light on the relationship between religion and fear, showing that religion does not protect religious people from feeling fear and or concern about terrorism, as is mostly claimed in the literature.

3.2. Limitations

The main limitation of this study concerns the fact that all the sample belong to a single religion, Christianity. However, it was a methodological option to avoid that the type of religion influences the variables studied, namely, religious practice; this would certainly vary according to the type of religion and not because of the concern about terrorist attacks. Another limitation concerns the selection of countries being mostly Christian: Countries with a Christian majority were chosen; however, this majority varies greatly from countries, with a percentage of 51% of Christians to countries with 90% of Christians. Finally, this study was a cross-sectional one, preventing the establishment of causal relationships between the variables under study, and thus allowing only the establishment of differences and associations.

3.3. Research Implications

Future research should include all religions, especially the most significant ones. In addition, countries whose percentage of religious majority is equivalent should be sought. At last, longitudinal research designs can best fit the subject under analysis.

4. Materials and Methods

4.1. Participants and Procedure

Data used in the study were collected from wave 6 of the WVS (2016). These data were obtained between 2010 and 2014 and include basic information on demographics, value and attitude measures, and organizations’ affiliation of more than 85,000 respondents in 57 countries. WVS has a well-structured sample of respondents and is not limited to special groups of individuals. It contains a question regarding the concern about terrorist attacks and a set of questions, not only about religious affiliation, but also on the intensity of individual beliefs.
The participating countries in this study were all countries included in WVS wave 6 whose dominant religion is Christianity (Pew Research Center’s Forum on Religion & Public Life 2012): Argentina, Australia, Armenia, Brazil, Belarus, Chile, Colombia, Cyprus, Ecuador, Estonia, Georgia, Germany, Ghana, Mexico, Netherlands, New Zealand, Peru, Philippines, Poland, Romania, Russia, Rwanda, Slovenia, South Africa, Zimbabwe, Spain, Sweden, Trinidad and Tobago, Ukraine, United States, and Uruguay. Of these countries, only subjects who considered themselves Christian were selected, comprising a total of 31,749 citizens. This selection sought to avoid differences between religions (identities and practices) determining the level of concern about terrorist attacks. Since the statistical analysis was limited to observations without outliers and without missing data, the final sample resulted in 30,446 respondents. The socio-demographic variables used were gender, age (in years and in categories), marital status (whose response modalities were converted into only two: Not married/not living together and married/living together), highest education level (these response modalities were converted into four: No formal education, low education, medium education and high education), and employment status (whose response modalities were converted into two: Inactive and active).

4.2. Measures

4.2.1. Worry of Terrorist Attacks

The question about the worry of terrorist attacks used is part of a list of situations. It was measured with one 4-point item and participants were asked “To what degree are you worried about the following situations?” (1 very much–4 not at all). The situation was “a terrorist attack”. Responses were recorded, with higher scores indicating higher levels of worries.

4.2.2. Religiosity

To identify the religious profile, a set of religious indicators was used. Response scales were adjusted so that the higher values corresponded to higher religiosity levels. Religiosity was indicated by two topics: Religious identity and religious practice.
  • Religious identity. Religious identity was measured with the following four items: (a) “Independently of whether you attend religious services or not, would you say you are” (1 a religious person, 2 not a religious person, 3 an atheist). This item has been recommended as the best single measure of personal piety (Stark 2002). We combined the second and third response ratings as the non-religious group (0), which was compared to the first group, the religious one (1). (b) “How important is religion in your life?” (1 not at all important, 2 not very important, 3 rather important, 4 very important). (c) “Do you believe in God” (1 yes, 2 no). This item has been commonly used to assess religious beliefs (Norenzayan and Hansen 2006). We recoded the responses as 1 (yes) and 0 (no). (d) “How important is God in your life?”. Participants answered this question on a 10-point scale (1 not at all important–10 very important). The responses were recoded as 1 not at all important to 4 very important.
  • Religious practice. The measure of religious practice contained three items: (a) “Apart from weddings and funerals, about how often do you attend religious services these days?” Participants answered this question on a 7-point scale (1 more than once a week–7 never, practically never). (b) “Apart from weddings and funerals, about how often do you pray?” Participants responded to this question on an 8-point scale (1 several times a day–8 never, practically never). The 2 items have shown good validity to estimate religious actions (Sethi and Seligman 1993). The responses were recoded to the 2 items in a 5-point scale (1 never–5 often), with higher scores indicating more religious practice. (c) “Active/Inactive membership: Church or religious organization” (1 not a member, 2 inactive member and 3 active member).

4.3. Data Analyses

A univariate analysis was applied to characterize the sample. Chi-square analyses were used to examine changes in the demographic characteristics and the religious profile in relation to the concern felt about terrorist attacks. To identify the profile of groups based on the worry of terrorist attacks, contingency tables and one-way ANOVA tests were computed. Significance was set at p ≤ 0.05. Data analysis was conducted using SPSS software, Version 25.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, Â.L. and A.R., data analysis, Â.L. and A.R. and writing, Â.L., A.R., M.A.P.D. and H.F.P.e.S.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Adamczyk, Amy, and Gary LaFree. 2015. Religiosity and reactions to terrorism. Social Science Research 51: 17–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Berko, Anat. 2007. The Path to Paradise: The Inner World of Suicide Bombers and Their Dispatchers. Greenwood: Publishing Group. [Google Scholar]
  3. Brañas-Garza, Pablo, Antonio Espín, and Shoshana Neuman. 2013. Effects of Religiosity on Social Behaviour: Experimental Evidence from a Representative Sample of Spaniards. London: CEPR. [Google Scholar]
  4. Caruso, Raul, and Andrea Locatelli. 2014. Understanding Terrorism: A Socio-Economic Perspective. Bingley: Emerald Group Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  5. Cohen-Louck, Keren, and Inna Levy. 2018. Risk perception of a chronic threat of terrorism: Differences based on coping types, gender and exposure. International Journal of Psychology. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Comer, Jonathan S. 2019. Media Literacy, Terrorism, and Fear. In The International Encyclopedia of Media Literacy. Hoboken: Wiley, pp. 1–6. [Google Scholar]
  7. Cornwall, Marie, Stan L. Albrecht, Perry H. Cunningham, and Brian L. Pitcher. 1986. The dimensions of religiosity: A conceptual model with an empirical test. Review of Religious Research 27: 226–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Davie, Grace. 1990. Believing without Belonging: Is This the Future of Religion in Britain? Social Compass 37: 455–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. De Mesquita, Ethan Bueno. 2008. The political economy of terrorism: A selective overview of recent work. The Political Economist 10: 1–12. [Google Scholar]
  10. Esteban, Joan Maria, Gilat Levy, and Laura Mayoral. 2015. Liberty, Religiosity, and Effort. London: CEPR. [Google Scholar]
  11. Feyyaz, Muhammad. 2016. Religion, ethnicity, social organizations and terrorists’ behavior—A case of Taliban movement in Pakistan. Behavioral Sciences of Terrorism and Political Aggression 8: 111–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Fischer, Peter, and Amy L. Ai. 2008. International terrorism and mental health: Recent research and future directions. Journal of Interpersonal Violence 23: 339–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Fischer, Peter, Tobias Greitemeyer, Andreas Kastenmüller, Eva Jonas, and Dieter Frey. 2006. Coping with terrorism: The impact of increased salience of terrorism on mood and self-efficacy of intrinsically religious and nonreligious people. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 32: 365–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Fischer, Peter, Tobias Greitemeyer, Andreas Kastenmüller, Dieter Frey, and Silvia Oßwald. 2007. Terror salience and punishment: Does terror salience induce threat to social order? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 43: 964–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Fischer, Peter, Julia Fischer, Silke Weisweiler, and Dieter Frey. 2010. Selective exposure to information: How different modes of decision making affect subsequent confirmatory information processing. British Journal of Social Psychology 49: 871–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Fischer, Peter, Tom Postmes, Julia Koeppl, Lianne Conway, and Tom Fredriksson. 2011. The meaning of collective terrorist threat: Understanding the subjective causes of terrorism reduces its negative psychological impact. Journal of Interpersonal Violence 26: 1432–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Flanagan, Kieran, and Grace Davie. 1995. Religion in Britain Since 1945. Believing without Belonging. The British Journal of Sociology 46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. George, Linda K., Christopher G. Ellison, and David B. Larson. 2002. TARGET ARTICLE: Explaining the Relationships between Religious Involvement and Health. Psychological Inquiry 13: 190–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Haner, Murat, Melissa M. Sloan, Francis T. Cullen, Teresa C. Kulig, and Cheryl Lero Jonson. 2019. Public Concern about Terrorism: Fear, Worry, and Support for Anti-Muslim Policies. Socius: Sociological Research for a Dynamic World 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Hoffman, Bruce. 1995. “Holy terror”: The implications of terrorism motivated by a religious imperative. Studies in Conflict & Terrorism 18: 271–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Horgan, John. 2012. Interviewing the terrorists: Reflections on fieldwork and implications for psychological research. Behavioral Sciences of Terrorism and Political Aggression 4: 195–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Huddy, Leonie, Stanley Feldman, Charles Taber, and Gallya Lahav. 2005. Threat, anxiety, and support of antiterrorism policies. American Journal of Political Science 49: 593–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Huq, Aziz Z. 2013. The political psychology of counterterrorism. Annual Review of Law and Social Science 9: 71–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. IEP. 2016. Global Terrorism Index 2016: Measuring and Understanding the Impact of Terrorism. Sydney: Institute for Economics & Peace. [Google Scholar]
  25. Ireland, William W. 2018. The Varieties of Religious Experience: A Study in Human Nature. Being the Gifford Lectures on Natural Religion, delivered at Edinburgh in 1901–1902 by William James, LL. D., etc. London, 1902: Longmans. Royal octavo, pp. 534. Journal of Mental Science 49: 146–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Jamal, Muhammad Ahsan. 2014. What Went Wrong in the War on Terrorism in Afghanistan? Available online: www.academia.edu/17777394/What_Went_Wrong_in_the_War_on_Terrorism_in_Afghanistan (accessed on 15 July 2016).
  27. Jefferis, Jennifer L. 2009. Religion and Political Violence: Sacred Protest in the Modern World. Abingdon: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  28. Jing, Xiaojuan. 2014. Nonbelievers’ Beliefs About Religion in China. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal 42: 1221–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Jonas, Eva, and Peter Fischer. 2006. Terror management and religion: evidence that intrinsic religiousness mitigates worldview defense following mortality salience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 91: 553–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Jong, Jonathan, and Jamin Halberstadt. 2016. Death Anxiety and Religious Belief: An Existential Psychology of Religion. London: Bloomsbury Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  31. Kaitz, Marsha, Mindy Levy, Richard Ebstein, Stephen V. Faraone, and David Mankuta. 2009. The intergenerational effects of trauma from terror: A real possibility. Infant Mental Health Journal 30: 158–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Kastenmüller, Andreas, Tobias Greitemeyer, Amy L. Ai, Gabriele Winter, and Peter Fischer. 2011. In the Face of Terrorism: Evidence that Belief in Literal Immortality Reduces Prejudice Under Terrorism Threat. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 50: 604–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Kastenmüller, Andreas, Nilüfer Aydin, Dieter Frey, Eva Traut-Mattausch, Claudia Peus, and Peter Fischer. 2014. Terrorist threat and employees’ perceived ability to cope with organizational change. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 44: 423–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. King, Michael, and Donald M. Taylor. 2011. The Radicalization of Homegrown Jihadists: A Review of Theoretical Models and Social Psychological Evidence. Terrorism and Political Violence 23: 602–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Kingsley, Okoro. 2010. Religion and terrorism: A socio-historical re-consideration. Journal of Alternative Perspectives in the Social Sciences 2: 550–76. [Google Scholar]
  36. Kluch, Sofia Pinero, and Alan Vaux. 2017. Culture and Terrorism: The Role of Cultural Factors in Worldwide Terrorism (1970–2013). Terrorism and Political Violence 29: 323–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Kosmin, Barry A., Ariela Keysar, Ryan Cragun, and Juhem Navarro-Rivera. 2009. American Nones: The Profile of the No Religion Population, a Report Based on the American Religious Identification Survey 2008. Quezon City: Trinity College. [Google Scholar]
  38. Küçükcan, Talip. 2000. Can religiosity be measured? Dimensions of religious commitment: Theories revisited. Uludağ Üniversitesi İlahiyat Fakültesi Dergisi 9: 1–5. [Google Scholar]
  39. Kupor, Daniella M., Kristin Laurin, and Jonathan Levav. 2015. Anticipating divine protection? Reminders of god can increase nonmoral risk taking. Psychological Science 26: 374–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Laqueur, Walter. 2017. A History of Terrorism. Abingdon: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  41. Leonhard, Robert R. 2005. The Evolution of Strategy in the Global War on Terror. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Applied Physics Laboratory, pp. 1–26. [Google Scholar]
  42. Lerner, Jennifer S., and Dacher Keltner. 2001. Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81: 146–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Lerner, Jennifer S., Roxana M. Gonzalez, Deborah A. Small, and Baruch Fischhoff. 2003. Effects of fear and anger on perceived risks of terrorism: a national field experiment. Psychological Science 14: 144–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Lin, Patrick K. F., Eddie M. W. Tong, Li Neng Lee, Andre H. M. Low, and Danielle Gomes. 2016. The prosocial impact of God concept priming on God believers. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice 3: 93–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Lindeman, Marjaana, and Jari Lipsanen. 2016. Diverse Cognitive Profiles of Religious Believers and Nonbelievers. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 26: 185–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Mahat-Shamir, Michal, Yaakov Hoffman, Shani Pitcho-Prelorentzos, Yaira Hamama-Raz, Osnat Lavenda, Lia Ring, Uzia Halevi, Eytan Ellenberg, Ishay Ostfeld, and Menachem Ben-Ezra. 2018. Truck attack: Fear of ISIS and reminder of truck attacks in Europe as associated with psychological distress and PTSD symptoms. Psychiatry Research 267: 306–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Matusitz, Jonathan. 2013. Terrorism and Communication: A Critical Introduction. Beijing: Sage. [Google Scholar]
  48. McCauley, Clark, and Sophia Moskalenko. 2008. Mechanisms of Political Radicalization: Pathways toward Terrorism. Terrorism and Political Violence 20: 415–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. McDaniel, Stephen W., and John J. Burnett. 1990. Consumer religiosity and retail store evaluative criteria. Journal of the Academy of marketing Science 18: 101–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Midlarsky, Manus I. 2011. Origins of Political Extremism: Mass Violence in the Twentieth Century and Beyond. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  51. Mink, Charles. 2015. It’s about the group, not God: Social causes and cures for terrorism. Journal for Deradicalization 2015: 63–91. [Google Scholar]
  52. Molteni, Francesco, and Ferruccio Biolcati. 2018. Shifts in religiosity across cohorts in Europe: A multilevel and multidimensional analysis based on the European Values Study. Social Compass 65: 413–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Norenzayan, Ara, and Ian G. Hansen. 2006. Belief in supernatural agents in the face of death. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 32: 174–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. O’Brien, Joanne, and Martin Palmer. 2000. Atlas del Estado de las Religions. [Atlas of the state of religions]. Madrid: Ediciones AKAL, vol. 4. [Google Scholar]
  55. O’Dea, Thomas F., and Janet O’Dea Aviad. 1983. The Sociology of Religion. Upper Saddle River: Prentice Hall. [Google Scholar]
  56. Oksanen, Atte, Markus Kaakinen, Jaana Minkkinen, Pekka Räsänen, Bernard Enjolras, and Kari Steen-Johnsen. 2018. Perceived Societal Fear and Cyberhate after the November 2015 Paris Terrorist Attacks. Terrorism and Political Violence, 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Ömer, Taşpınar. 2009. Fighting Radicalism, Not “Terrorism”: Root Causes of an International Actor Redefined. SAIS Review of International Affairs 29: 75–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Pennycook, Gordon, Robert M. Ross, Derek J. Koehler, and Jonathan A. Fugelsang. 2016. Atheists and Agnostics Are More Reflective than Religious Believers: Four Empirical Studies and a Meta-Analysis. PLoS ONE 11: e0153039. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Perliger, Arie, Gabriel Koehler-Derrick, and Ami Pedahzur. 2016. The Gap Between Participation and Violence: Why We Need to Disaggregate Terrorist ‘Profiles’. International Studies Quarterly 60: 220–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Pew Research Center’s Forum on Religion & Public Life. 2012. The Global Religious Landscape. A Report on the Size and Distribution of the World’s Major Religious Groups as of 2010. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center. [Google Scholar]
  61. Pisoiu, Daniela. 2014. Subcultures, violent radicalization and terrorism. Behavioral Sciences of Terrorism and Political Aggression 7: 1–2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Post, Jerrold M., Farhana Ali, Schuyler W. Henderson, Steven Shanfield, Jeff Victoroff, and Stevan Weine. 2009. The psychology of suicide terrorism. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes 72: 13–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Poulin, Michael J., Roxane C. Silver, Virginia Gil-Rivas, E. Alison Holman, and Daniel N. McIntosh. 2009. Finding social benefits after a collective trauma: Perceiving societal changes and well-being following 9/11. Journal of Traumatic Stress 22: 81–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Rapoport, David C. 2013. Inside Terrorist Organizations. Abingdon: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  65. Ross, Jeffrey Ian. 2015. Religion and Violence: An Encyclopedia of Faith and Conflict from Antiquity to the Present. Abingdon: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  66. Sensales, Gilda, Alessandra Areni, Lauren Boyatzi, Alessandra Dal Secco, and Arie Kruglanski. 2014. Perceived impact of terrorism and the role of the media: Representations by Italian citizens differing in political orientation and need for closure. Behavioral Sciences of Terrorism and Political Aggression 6: 41–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Sethi, Sheena, and Martin E. P. Seligman. 1993. Optimism and fundamentalism. Psychological Science 4: 256–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Shaffer, Ryan. 2015. Unconventional Views of Terrorism: Culture, Objectives, and the Future. Terrorism and Political Violence 27: 970–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Shariff, Azim F., and Mijke Rhemtulla. 2012. Divergent effects of beliefs in heaven and hell on national crime rates. PLoS ONE 7: e39048. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Shechory-Bitton, Mally, and Keren Cohen-Louck. 2018. Does Fear of Terrorism Differ from Fear of Crime and Sexual Assault: A Question of Geographical Location and Residential Area. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology 62: 806–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Stark, Rodney. 2002. Physiology and Faith: Addressing the “Universal” Gender Difference in Religious Commitment. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 41: 495–507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. START. 2016. Global Terrorism Database. National Consortium for the Study of Terrorism and Responses to Terrorism. College Park: START. [Google Scholar]
  73. Steffen, Patrick R., Kevin S. Masters, and Scott Baldwin. 2017. What Mediates the Relationship between Religious Service Attendance and Aspects of Well-Being? Journal of Religion and Health 56: 158–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Twemlow, Stuart W. 2005. Psychoanalytic controversies: The relevance of psychoanalysis to an understanding of terrorism. The International Journal of Psychoanalysis 86: 957–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Updegraff, John A., Roxane Cohen Silver, and E. Alison Holman. 2008. Searching for and finding meaning in collective trauma: results from a national longitudinal study of the 9/11 terrorist attacks. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 95: 709–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  76. Voas, David. 2008. The Rise and Fall of Fuzzy Fidelity in Europe. European Sociological Review 25: 155–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  77. Voas, David, and Mark Chaves. 2018. Even Intense Religiosity Is Declining in the United States: Comment. Sociological Science 5: 694–710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Walsh, James Igoe. 2010. Media attention to terrorist attacks: Causes and consequences. Institute for Homeland Security Solutions, Research Brief, 1–15. [Google Scholar]
  79. Waxman, Dov. 2011. Living with terror, not living in terror: The impact of chronic terrorism on Israeli society. Perspectives on Terrorism 5: 4–26. [Google Scholar]
  80. Williamson, Harley, Suzanna Fay, and Toby Miles-Johnson. 2019. Fear of terrorism: media exposure and subjective fear of attack. Global Crime 20: 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. World Values Survey. 2016. Wave 6. Official Aggregate v.20150418. World Values Survey Association. Madrid: Aggregate File Producer, Asep/JDS, SPAIN, Available online: www.worldvaluessurvey.org (accessed on 24 September 2017).
Table 1. Socio-demographic sample characterization (N = 30,446).
Table 1. Socio-demographic sample characterization (N = 30,446).
VariableN%
Country
Argentina6702.20
Australia7442.44
Armenia9953.27
Brazil11633.82
Belarus12634.15
Chile7102.33
Colombia11633.82
Cyprus4611.51
Ecuador9143.00
Estonia4811.58
Georgia11373.73
Germany10043.30
Ghana12244.02
Mexico16225.33
Netherlands5081.67
New Zealand4921.62
Peru10333.39
Philippines9963.27
Poland8762.88
Romania14004.60
Russia15415.06
Rwanda11803.88
Slovenia7152.35
South Africa20026.58
Zimbabwe13324.37
Spain8662.84
Sweden7372.42
Trinidad and Tobago6092.00
Ukraine12103.97
United States10943.59
Uruguay3041.00
Gender
Male13,31443.73
Female17,132 56.27
Age
16–24466015.31
25–4411,62238.17
45–64933230.65
>65483215.87
Education
No formal education4941.62
Low education524917.24
Medium education17,04255.97
High education766125.16
Marital Status
Not married/not living together11,93739.21
Married/living together18,50960.79
Employment Status
Inactive14,93449.05
Active15,51250.95
Table 2. Sample distribution relating the concern about the terrorist attacks.
Table 2. Sample distribution relating the concern about the terrorist attacks.
Not at All (N = 4717) Not Much (N = 6086) A Great Deal (N = 7053) Very Much (N = 12,587)χ2(df)p
N (%)N (%)N (%)N (%)
Country 10,267.23 (90) <0.001
Argentina(5) 263 (5.56%)
Australia (5) 336 (5.52%)
Belarus (4) 400 (5.67%)
Chile(4) 264 (5.58%)
Colombia (5) 785 (6.24%)
Germany (1) 456 (7.49%) (4) 801 (6.37%)
Ghana (3) 825 (6.56%)
Mexico (1) 1281 (10.18%)
Romania(3) 269 (5.69%)
Russia (1) 683 (9.68%)
Rwanda (2) 1106 (8.79%)
South Africa(1) 485 (10.26%)(2) 444 (7.30%)(2) 507 (7.19%)
Zimbabwe(2) 306 (6.47%)
Sweden (4) 343 (5.64%)
Ukraine (5) 399 (5.66%)
United States (3) 377 (6.19%)(3) 425 (6.03%)
Gender 36.697 (3)<0.001
Male2184 (46.20%)2790 (45.84%)3036 (43.05%)5304 (42.16%)
Female2543 (53.80%)3296 (54.16%)4017 (56.95%)7276 (57.84%)
Age 727.784 (240)<0.001
M (SD)45.63 (18.85)46.08 (17.97)46.07 (17.81)41.94 (16.72)
16–24758 (16.04%)834 (13.70%)965 (13.68%)2103 (16.72%)
25–441640 (34.69%)2159 (35.47%)2445 (34.67%)5378 (42.75%)
45–641396 (29.53%)1982 (32.57%)2362 (33.49%)3592 (28.55%)
>65933 (19.74%)1111 (18.26%)1281 (18.16%)1507 (11.98%)
Education 414.258 (9)<0.001
No formal education52 (1.10%)46 (0.76%)69 (0.98%)327 (2.60%)
Low education921 (19.48%)893 (14.67%)1076 (15.26%)2359 (18.75%)
Medium education2596 (54.92%)3277 (53.84%)4029 (57.12%)7140 (56.76%)
High education1158 (24.50%)1870 (30.73%)1879 (26.64%)2754 (21.89%)
Marital Status 50.768 (3)<0.001
Not married/not living together2061 (43.60%)2300 (37.79%)2789 (39.54%)4787 (38.05%)
Married/living together2666 (54.40%)3786 (62.21%)4264 (60.46%)7793 (61.95%)
Employment Status 42.182 (3)<0.001
Inactive2481 (52.49%)2850 (46.83%)3543 (50.23%)6060 (48.17%)
Active2246 (47.51%)3236 (53.17%)3510 (49.77%)6520 (51.83%)
Religious identity
Religious person 453.274 (3)<0.001
Not a religious person/an atheist1117 (23.63%)1532 (25.17%)1448 (20.53%)1725 (13.71%)
A religious person3610 (76.37%)4554 (74.83%)5605 (79.47%)10,855 (86.29%)
Important in life: Religion 1579.229 (9)<0.001
Not at all important455 (9.63%)526 (8.64%)442 (6.27%)411 (3.27%)
Not very important955 (20.20%)1503 (24.70%)1225 (17.37%)1528 (12.15%)
Rather important1363 (28.83%)1912 (31.42%)2312 (32.78%)3020 (24.01%)
Very important1954 (41.34%)2145 (35.24%)3074 (43.58%)7621 (60.58%)
Believe in: God 519.597 (3)<0.001
No443 (9.37%)662 (10.88%)438 (6.21%)380 (3.02%)
Yes4284 (90.63%)5424 (89.12%)6615 (93.79%)12,200 (96.98%)
How important is God in your life 1743.741 (9)<0.001
1—Not at all important409 (8.65%)530 (8.71%)393 (5.57%)389 (3.09%)
2592 (12.52%)920 (15.12%)800 (11.34%)789 (6.27%)
31288 (27.25%)1837 (30.18%)1943 (27.55%)2163 (17.19%)
4—Very Important2438 (51.58%)2799 (45.99%)3917 (55.54%)9239 (73.44%)
Religious practice
Active/Inactive membership of a Church or religious organization 221.536 (6)<0.001
Not a member2192 (46.37%)2822 (46.37%)3684 (52.23%)5624 (44.71%)
Inactive member1135 (24.01%)1682 (27.64%)1499 (21.25%)2808 (22.32%)
Active member1400 (29.62%)1582 (25.99%)1870 (26.51%)4148 (32.97%)
How often do you attend religious services 746.575 (12)<0.001
Less often or never1276 (26.99%)1614 (26.52%)1588 (22.52%)2269 (18.04%)
Once a year274 (5.80%)438 (7.20%)425 (6.03%)582 (4.63%)
Only on special holy days829 (17.54%)1191 (19.57%)1462 (20.73%)1833 (14.57%)
Once a month637 (13.48%)904 (14.85%)1152 (16.33%)1895 (15.06%)
At least once a week1711 (36.20%)1939 (31.86%)2426 (34.40%)6001 (47.70%)
How often do you pray 885.098 (12)<0.001
Never, practically never572 (12.10%)838 (13.77%)694 (9.84%)733 (5.83%)
At least once a year417 (8.82%)642 (10.55%)631 (8.95%)899 (7.15%)
Only on holy days and when attending religious services793 (16.78%)1046 (17.19%)1181 (16.74%)1448 (11.51%)
Once a day and several times each week1906 (40.32%)2457 (40.37%)2998 (42.51%)5661 (45.00%)
Several times a day1039 (21.98%)1103 (18.22%)1549 (21.96%)3839 (30.52%)
Note: numbering in countries data refers to the importance of each country; bold values are the highest observed in the answer modality.
Table 3. Distribution of frequencies relating worries on terrorist attack.
Table 3. Distribution of frequencies relating worries on terrorist attack.
N%
Not at all (1)472715.53
Not much (2)608619.99
A great deal (3)705323.17
Very much (4)12,58041.32
Table 4. Distribution of frequencies relating to religious identity.
Table 4. Distribution of frequencies relating to religious identity.
N%
Religious person
Not a religious person/an atheist582219.12
A religious person24,62480.88
Important in life: Religion
Not at all important (1)18346.02
Not very important (2)521117.12
Rather important (3)860728.27
Very important (4)14,79448.59
Believe in: God
No19236.32
Yes28,52393.68
How important is God in your life
1—Not at all important17215.65
2310110.19
3723123.75
4—Very Important18,39360.41
Table 5. Distribution of frequencies relating to religious practice.
Table 5. Distribution of frequencies relating to religious practice.
N%
Active/Inactive membership: Church or religious organization
Not a member14,32247.04
Inactive member712423.40
Active member900029.56
How often do you attend religious services
Less often or never (1)674722.16
Once a year (2)17195.65
Only on special holy days (3)531517.46
Once a month (4)458815.07
At least once a week (5)12,07739.67
How often do you pray
Never, practically never (1)28379.32
At least once a year (2)25898.50
Only on holy days and when attending religious services (3)446814.68
Once a day and several times each week (4)13,02242.77
Several times a day (5)753024.73

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Leite, Â.; Ramires, A.; Dinis, M.A.P.; e Sousa, H.F.P. Who Is Concerned about Terrorist Attacks? A Religious Profile. Soc. Sci. 2019, 8, 316. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci8110316

AMA Style

Leite Â, Ramires A, Dinis MAP, e Sousa HFP. Who Is Concerned about Terrorist Attacks? A Religious Profile. Social Sciences. 2019; 8(11):316. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci8110316

Chicago/Turabian Style

Leite, Ângela, Ana Ramires, Maria Alzira Pimenta Dinis, and Hélder Fernando Pedrosa e Sousa. 2019. "Who Is Concerned about Terrorist Attacks? A Religious Profile" Social Sciences 8, no. 11: 316. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci8110316

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop