Next Article in Journal
Introduction to the Special Issue: Feminist Solidarity, Resistance, and Social Justice
Previous Article in Journal
Use of Space and Safety Perceptions from a Gender Perspective: University Campus, Student Lodging, and Leisure Spots in Concepción (Chile)
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Mediation (Emotional Self-Control) and Moderation (Fun) of the Relationship Between Receiving and Perpetrating Hate Speech Among Boys and Girls in Almeria (Spain)

by
Francisco Villegas Lirola
1 and
Pilar Rodriguez Martinez
2,*
1
Departamento de Educación, Universidad de Almería, La Cañada, 04120 Almería, Spain
2
Departamento de Geografía, Historia y Humanidades, Universidad de Almería, La Cañada, 04120 Almería, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Soc. Sci. 2025, 14(6), 349; https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci14060349
Submission received: 28 March 2025 / Revised: 23 May 2025 / Accepted: 25 May 2025 / Published: 29 May 2025
(This article belongs to the Section Childhood and Youth Studies)

Abstract

:
This article presents an analysis of the mediation and moderation of the suffering and perpetration of hate speech among adolescents in Almería (Spain), taking into account gender differences. For this purpose, data from a questionnaire completed by secondary school students (N = 535) in four schools in Almería (Spain) was analyzed. For the analysis, an index of emotional self-control was constructed and introduced as a mediator between receiving hate speech and perpetrating it. In addition, the moderating role of revenge and fun motives in this relationship was analyzed, finding that the revenge motive is not significant and the fun motive is significant (for boys). The results suggest that receiving hate speech does not mean that one commits hate speech. It occurs when there is a low level of self-control and increases (for boys) when hate speech is perceived as fun. Therefore, we can say that for both boys and girls, it is necessary to improve emotional self-control. In addition, toxic masculinity is related to low emotional self-control, but is loaded with the view that hate speech is funny.

1. Introduction

Hate speech can be defined as “any form of expression intended to incite, promote, disseminate or justify violence, hatred or discrimination against a person or group of persons on the grounds of their personal characteristics or status, such as “race”, colour, language, religion, nationality, national or ethnic origin, age, disability, gender, gender identity and sexual orientation” (Council of Europe 2022). In other words, hate speech is defined (a) as expressions of hostility directed at people; (b) justified by factors of identity or belonging (real or perceived) to social groups, gender, ethnicity, ideology, or physical appearance, among others (Sponholz 2023; Kansok-Dusche et al. 2023); and (c) it also seeks dissemination and publicity (Schwertberger and Rieger 2021).
The interpretation of hate speech can be clearer or more ambiguous based on its explicit or implied content, as well as its symbolic character (Weber et al. 2024), and its intentionality. Hate speech is intended to harm, humiliate, or belittle a person or group of people. It is also intended to be seen by an audience and incite more hatred (Rodriguez Ramos 2022; Brown 2018).
Currently, the Internet is the main source of hate speech (Blaya 2019; Paz et al. 2020). At least 75% of people who use social networks or instant messaging have witnessed or experienced insults, threats, or hate speech online (EU Commission 2016). Hate speech occurs in two main contexts: social media (Castaño-Pulgarin et al. 2021) and online gaming (Costa et al. 2024). In addition, there is the “massive cyberattack”, in which a large number of users post comments directed at an individual or small group, using language intended to shame, humiliate, marginalize, or insult, in a digital bombardment that is difficult to escape (Cover 2023).
As the authors point out, the material basis of hate speech is disinformation (Di Fátima 2024, p. 12). Considered a communication phenomenon in the post-truth era, these discourses contribute to the promotion of an ‘informational disorder’—disinformation—that aims to cause harm (Lilleker and Pérez-Escolar 2023; Wardle and Derakhshan 2017; Barrientos Rastrojo 2022), using insults, threats, and abuse to incite violence, reinforce stereotypes, and create a climate of intolerance (Wachs and Wright 2019). The harm is compounded by the sense of security of those who perpetrate and spread hate speech through social networks, shielded by anonymity, which creates particular helplessness in victims (Poland 2016), especially when those victims are adolescents (Kolotouchkina et al. 2023). Marinoni et al. (2024a) found a positive relationship between the possibility of using a fake profile and the perpetration of cyberbullying.
According to the authors, there is a relationship between victimization and the perpetration of hate speech (Wachs et al. 2022b). In fact, Walrave and Heirman (2011) found that online victimization was the most important predictor of future cyberbullying perpetration. In other words, being the recipient of hate speech is related to perpetrating, disseminating, issuing, or promoting hate speech (Ramón Ortiz and Guizado 2023). Musante and DeWalt (2011) and Pinker (2011) argue that the most common factors driving violent behavior are revenge, power, entertainment, and ideology. Ballaschk et al. (2021) consider hate speech to be a form of violence.
Regarding revenge, Kowalski and Limber (2007) found that students exposed to cyberbullying tend to experience anger afterward (p. 2). This anger is related to the perception of being on the receiving end of an unfair situation (Van Dorn 2018). Some adolescents respond by using hate speech as a means of revenge or to express a desire to harm the other (Ballaschk et al. 2021).
Pinker (2011) posits that violence is associated with the pursuit of power and the perception of exerting control over others. Ballaschk et al. (2021) found a correlation between the desire for power and the perpetration of hate speech.
The idea that seeking enjoyment justifies the expression of hate speech is based on two main principles. First, humor serves to minimize the severity of hate speech. Second, the content of the messages may cause recipients to view the senders as overly sensitive or lacking a sense of humor (Jane 2015).
However, ideology is not necessarily a motivation for hate speech (Fluck 2017). The search for identity and belonging to a group can create pressure to align with ideological opinions that are compatible with hate speech (Gradinger et al. 2012; Ballaschk et al. 2021; Van de Bongardt et al. 2015).
Tanrikulu and Erdur-Baker (2021) found that the two main motives for adolescents to engage in hate speech were revenge and entertainment. Regarding revenge, Kowalski and Limber (2007) found that students exposed to cyberbullying tend to experience feelings of anger after the bullying (p. 2). Thus, a correlation would be expected between those who receive hate speech and those who perpetrate hate speech, which could be explained as reactive aggression motivated by revenge (Fluck 2017; Runions et al. 2018; Tanrikulu and Erdur-Baker 2021; Wachs and Wright 2019). Other authors also note that some youth may use hate speech as a means of revenge when they feel threatened, treated unfairly, frustrated, or angry (Wachs et al. 2022b, p. 10). In addition, Wachs et al. (2022a) found that the revenge subscale of the Motivations to Engage in Hate Speech Perpetration Scale (MHATE) had the highest frequency (p. 17). Marinoni et al. (2024b) found that, particularly in online games, the normalization of teasing and aggressive behavior is common as part of the socialization process.
Second, we have the entertainment or fun motive. Hate speech is often expressed in the form of jokes to gain power and fit in with groups within youth peer networks (Wachs et al. 2022a). Hate speech that is masked by intentional humor may be perceived as culturally acceptable, creating an added sense of helplessness (Cover 2023). Schmid et al. (2024) identified fun and entertainment as one of the most common motives for justifying and perpetrating hate speech. The trivialization of hate speech, treated as a humorous expression, seeks to provide justification and be interpreted as less serious and even innocent (Schmid et al. 2024). This further contributes to dehumanizing and creating helplessness in victims, while establishing moral disengagement processes or mechanisms that make it easier for perpetrators to legitimize their aggressive behavior (Bandura 2016) and assign blame to the victim (Álvarez-Turrado et al. 2024).
Finally, the relationship between hate speech and emotional self-control should be highlighted. Teruel et al. (2019, p. 2) define emotional intelligence as “the knowledge and/or competencies to manage emotions effectively in order to regulate social and emotional behaviors”. Emotional self-control is defined as the ability to control impulses when faced with ethical, moral, legal, social, or other constraints that make an impulse inappropriate or harmful (West and Thomson 2025).
As the authors point out, adolescence is a period characterized by strong feelings and emotions (O′Brien 2014). In this sense, Delgado et al. (2019) demonstrated that the likelihood of being a victim or aggressor of cyberbullying increases with the level of aggressiveness, which implies a lower capacity for emotional self-control. Self-control is based on changes in motivation that depend on one’s identity and situational context (Berkman et al. 2017). Regarding identity, self-control exhibits both a cognitive–structural component situated in the inferior frontal gyrus and anterior insula and a socio-emotional component located in the middle frontal gyrus and posterior cingulate (Hofmann et al. 2009; Li et al. 2021; Steinberg 2008). Together, these components predispose individuals to respond differently to different situations (Baumeister et al. 2007). The manner in which thoughts and feelings (or emotions) are adapted is influenced by two factors. The first is personal preferences, and the second is social situations (Koole 2008; Paschke et al. 2016). There is a substantial degree of overlap between self-control and emotional self-regulation (Musante and DeWalt 2011). Gross and Thompson (2007) posits that emotional self-regulation is a form of emotional self-control.
Recent studies also show that the emotional intelligence profile is a variable that can explain to a large extent the variability of cybervictimization, so those who show a higher level of emotional self-regulation have a lower risk of cybervictimization (Villegas-Lirola 2024). At the other extreme, those who have received hate speech and have low emotional self-control would be precisely those who perpetrate the most hate speech (Turner et al. 2022). In social dynamics, the need to seek the approval of like-minded friends (Walther 2022) leads to the development of situations such as those described among incels, one of the main groups of the manosphere. These individuals end up in a spiral of self-pity and self-loathing, which forms the basis of the fraternal bonds within their community (Scotto di Carlo 2023). The development of such toxic masculinity shares ideas with the authoritarian personality (Erjavec and Kovačič 2012).
The innovation of this research is that it jointly examines (1) the relationship between experiencing hate speech and perpetrating it; (2) the influence that self-control has on this relationship; and (3) whether self-control moderates the relationship between experiencing hate speech and perpetrating it by motives such as revenge and amusement. In addition, a gender analysis of these relationships is presented.

2. Method

Our research has four aims: (1) to construct an index of emotional self-control; (2) to identify gender differences in receiving and/or perpetrating hate speech; (3) to analyze the mediating role of emotional self-control between receiving and perpetrating hate speech; and (4) to analyze the moderating role of motives of (a) revenge and (b) viewing hate speech as fun.

2.1. Participants and Instrument

Non-probability accessibility sampling was used to select the participants (Arrogante 2022). The sample consisted of 535 boys and girls from four secondary schools in Almería (Spain). Of the total sample, 48.8% were girls (261), 46.7% were boys (250), 2.6% were non-binary (14), and the rest (10) were considered non-valid (did not respond). Students ranged in age from 12 to 19 years (M = 14.88; SD = 5.39). Eighty-nine percent (476) were in compulsory secondary education, 7.9% (42) were in bachelor’s programs, and the rest were in vocational programs (17).
All missing cases for the variables of analysis were eliminated from the statistical analysis. In total, the analysis sample consisted of 413 participants, of which 50.1% (207) were girls, 46.5% (192) were boys, and the rest (14) were non-binary. The mean age was 14.47 years (SD = 1.53). The non-response rate was 22.8%, which allows us to consider the survey to be of good quality (Biemer and Lyberg 2003).
An ad hoc questionnaire was developed and is available at http://hdl.handle.net/10835/18556 (accessed on 7 February 2025), together with the database we used in this study (Rodríguez-Martínez et al. 2025).

2.2. Procedure

For data collection, four secondary schools were contacted and provided with information about the project and the questionnaire to be administered to the students. Each school agreed to participate in the project through its school council, and consent was obtained from the students’ legal custodians. Interviewers were informed of the ethical principles of the Declaration of Helsinki regarding confidentiality, anonymity, and voluntary participation. The questionnaire was completed by the students during school hours in their regular classroom (March to June 2024). They completed it on laptops or tablets accessed via a QR code, and it took approximately thirty minutes to complete.

2.3. Data Analysis

(1) Variables
The dependent variable used was perpetrated hate speech, based on the question “In the past 12 months, how often have you experienced perpetrated hate speech at your school?”, with three response options: (1) never; (2) once or twice in the same month; (3) three times a month; (4) about once a week; (5) several times a week. The variable “perpetrated hate speech (PerpDO)” was created as a dichotomous variable (1: yes perpetrated hate speech; 0: no perpetrated hate speech).
The independent variable was having been confronted with hate speech (1: yes; 0: no). The mediating variable (emotional self-control index) was constructed from four items. The question in the questionnaire was as follows:
“All people are different. We would like to know to what extent you agree with the following statements:
L1. I think about how I feel, about my emotions.
L2. I can control my behavior.
L3. I feel responsible for my behavior.
L4. I care about how others feel”.
The motives that participants used to justify hate speech were revenge (J1) and fun (J2). Responses were recorded on a Likert scale from 1 to 5, with 1 being strongly disagree and 5 being strongly agree.
J1. It is okay to send hateful or degrading messages to someone on the Internet if they attack you, your friends, or your family first.
J2. It is okay to send hateful or degrading messages against someone on the Internet because hate speech is fun.
The gender variable (S1) refers to an individual’s self-identification as male, female, or non-binary.
(2) Construction of the Emotional Self-Control Index
Exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was performed on items L1, L2, L3, and L4 using the SPSS dimension reduction function (v. 29) [Analysis > Dimension Reduction > Factor]. The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) test of sampling adequacy and Barlett’s test of sphericity were performed, as well as Cronbach’s alpha and McDonald’s w-value. The maximum likelihood method was used and a scatterplot was generated. A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was then performed to verify its construct validity (Amos, v. 28). The interpretation was supported by the utility of Gaskin and Lim (2016). As cutoff criteria, those proposed by Hu and Bentler (1999) were used (Table 1).
Once it was verified that the four items allowed the formation of a self-control index with good reliability and validity, it was categorized, taking into account the percentile distribution. This was done using the visual grouping utility (SPSS) [Transform > Visual Grouping], with four cut-off points (20% per group). This resulted in five groups (very high, high, medium, low, medium, low, and very low).
(3) Identifying gender differences
Descriptive data were calculated for the different variables, as well as percentages for the different profiles resulting from suffering/perpetrating hate speech. Four mean difference analyses were conducted using one-factor ANOVA with gender (boy, girl, non-binary) as the comparison variable and the following as the dependent variable:
(a) Receiving hate speech;
(b) Perpetrating hate speech;
(c) Justifying hate speech as funny;
(d) Believing that it is legitimate to engage in hate speech out of revenge.
(4) Analysis of the mediating role of emotional self-control and the moderating role of (a) revenge motives and (b) viewing hate speech as humorous motives in the relationship between receiving hate speech and perpetrating hate speech.
It was based on model 7 (moderated mediation) of the PROCESS macro for SPSS (Hayes 2013). We took perpetrating hate speech (PerpDO) as the dependent variable (Y), receiving hate speech (E1_1L) as the independent variable (X), the emotional self-control index as the mediating variable (M), and the motivation for revenge and then the motivation to consider hate speech as humorous as the moderating variable (W) first (Figure 1).

3. Results

3.1. Construction of the Emotional Self-Control Index

As a result of the KMO test, a value of 0.684 was obtained, indicating that the correlations between pairs of variables can be explained by the rest of the variables. Barlett’s test of sphericity (chi-square: 244.97; df: 6) had a p-value of less than 0.001, which means that the variables are related to each other, thus making it possible to proceed with the exploratory factor analysis. A Cronbach’s alpha of 0.66 [CI95%: 0.58, 0.72] and a McDonald’s omega of 0.662 [CI95%: 0.58, 0.72] were obtained.
When drop contrast was used as the decision criterion (Figure 2), the optimal number of factors was found to be 2, with the model explaining 69.88% of the variance.
A percentile scale of the Emotional Self-Control Index was constructed. If the sum of items L1, L2, L3, and L4 is less than 12, it is considered very low emotional self-control; scores between 13 and 15 are considered moderately low; between 17 and 18, moderately high; and more than 19, very high (Table 2).
The theoretical model (Figure 3), to be verified by Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) (Figure 4), grouped items L1 and L4 under the Emotional Self-perception factor (F1), and items L2 and L3 under the Emotional Self-regulation factor (F2).
In terms of model fit, the various indices were rated as excellent (Table 3).

3.2. Identification of Gender Differences

Receiving hate speech
For the variable receiving hate speech, the means and standard deviations were very similar for boys [mean: 0.31 (CI95%: 0.25; 0.38), SD: 0.46] and girls [mean: 0.29 (CI95%: 0.22; 0.35), SD: 0.45]. For the non-binary category, the mean was higher than for the other two categories [mean: 0.50 (CI95%: 0.20; 0.80), SD: 0.52]. The small number of cases for this category (14) is noteworthy.
The sample size analysis showed that the study has 80% power to detect the effect size in the process of identifying differences between groups, which is likely to lead to a slight underestimation of the effect size for the values of the group of subjects who identify as non-binary.
In the homogeneity of variances tests, a p-value of Levene’s statistic > 0.05 was obtained assuming the null hypothesis (homogeneity of variances), using the ANOVA F test and Tuckey’s test for post hoc analysis as the reference for the analysis. The ANOVA F value was not significant (p-value = 0.230) because it was greater than 0.05 (Table 4).
Perpetrating Hate Speech
For the perpetrating hate speech variable, the means and standard deviations were also very similar for boys [mean: 0.58 (CI95%: 0.51; 0.65), SD: 0.50] and girls [mean: 0.55 (CI95%: 0.48; 0.61), SD: 0.50]. For the non-binary category, the mean was higher than for the other two categories [mean: 0.71 (CI95%: 0.44; 0.98), SD: 0.47].
In tests for homogeneity of variances, a p-value of Levene’s statistic < 0.05 was obtained, leading to the assumption of the alternative hypothesis (no homogeneity of variances). Therefore, Welch’s test and the Games–Howel test were used for post hoc analysis.
The p-value of Welch’s statistic was not significant (p-value = 0.412 > 0.05). The difference between the means was not significant for any of the groups (Table 5).
Perpetrating hate speech is fun
For the variable perpetrating hate speech is fun (J2), the means and standard deviations were as follows: for boys [mean: 1.77 (CI95%: 1.60; 1.94), SD: 1.19] and for girls [mean: 1.34 (CI95%: 1.22; 1.47), SD: 0.89). For the non-binary category, the mean was higher than for the other two categories [mean: 2.79 (CI95%: 1.48; 1.70), SD: 1.11].
In the homogeneity of variances tests, a p-value of Levene’s statistic < 0.05 was obtained, leading to the assumption of the alternative hypothesis (no homogeneity of variances). Therefore, Welch’s test and Games–Howel test were used for post hoc analysis.
The p-value of Welch’s statistic was significant (p-value < 0.001). The mean difference was significant (Table 6).
Revenge justification for perpetration of hate speech
When asked whether it is justified to commit hate speech if we are attacked first (revenge), the means and standard deviations were very similar for boys [mean: 2.69 (CI95%: 2.47; 2.90), SD: 1.50], girls [mean: 2.49 (CI95%: 2.28; 2.70), SD: 1.51], and those who identified as non-binary [mean: 3.00 (CI95%: 2.07; 3.93), SD: 1.62].
In the homogeneity of variances tests, a p-value of Levene’s statistic > 0.05 was obtained, leading to the assumption of the null hypothesis (homogeneity of variances). Therefore, the ANOVA F test and Tuckey’s test were used for post hoc analysis.
The ANOVA F-value was not significant (p-value = 0.253) (Table 7).
Emotional Self-Control
For emotional self-control, the means and standard deviations were similar for boys [mean: 15.23 (CI95%: 14.75; 15.71), SD: 3.35] and girls [mean: 15.54 (CI95%: 15.13; 15.95), SD: 2.99], and slightly lower for the non-binary category [mean: 12.21 (CI95%: 9.90; 14.52), SD: 4.00].
In tests for homogeneity of variances, a p-value of Levene’s statistic > 0.05 was obtained, leading to the assumption of homogeneity of variances.
The ANOVA F value was significant (p-value < 0.001), with a small effect [(eta-squared of 0.034 (CI95%: 0.006; 0.072)] (Table 8).
Comparison of hate speech profiles by gender
The non-binary group is very small, making comparisons difficult. Among non-victims and non-perpetrators, girls (36.7%) show a higher percentage than boys (29.2%). Among those who are not victims but are perpetrators of hate speech, the situation is very similar (39.6% for boys and 34.8% for girls), as well as among those who are both victims and perpetrators (18.2% for boys and 19.8% for girls). On the contrary, boys are more likely than girls to be victims and non-perpetrators of hate speech (boys: 13.0%, girls: 8.7%) (Table 9).

3.3. Moderated Mediation Analysis

Taking the mediating variable (Emotional Self-Control Index) as the dependent variable, suffering hate speech as the independent variable, and considering hate speech as funny as the moderating variable, we found a significant relationship between the different variables (Figure 5).
The regression analysis between the mediating variable and the dependent variable was significant, while the relationship between receiving hate speech and perpetrating hate speech (excluding the effects of the mediating variable) was not significant (Figure 6).

4. Discussion

Our results indicate that receiving hate speech does not imply perpetrating hate speech (c’ = 0.40, p-value > 0.05—not significant). This result contrasts with the findings of Wachs et al. (2022a) and Walrave and Heirman (2011). However, the two variables (receiving and perpetrating hate speech) are related, as receiving and perpetrating hate speech occur significantly when low levels of self-control are present for both boys and girls. The relationship between receiving hate speech and perpetrating hate speech is determined by the presence of low levels of emotional self-regulation.
One of the goals of this work was to construct an index of emotional self-control that would allow us to know its role in the relationship between receiving and perpetrating hate speech. A two-factor structure was found, with a good internal consistency (0.66) and an accumulated variance of 69.88%. The results of the confirmatory factor analysis demonstrated excellent validity of the instrument (Gaskin and Lim 2016).
The moderated mediation analysis showed that the variable of emotional self-control is necessary to explain the relationship between receiving hate speech and perpetrating it. It resulted in a full mediation (Hayes 2013) (Figure 7).
In this regard, our analysis confirms the results obtained by Villegas-Lirola (2024) regarding cybervictimization and by Turner et al. (2022) regarding self-control in the exposure and sharing of hate content online by adolescents.
Regarding hate speech motivations, we found that revenge motivation was not significant as a moderating variable (Table 10), which contradicts the findings of Kowalski and Limber (2007), Fluck (2017), Runions et al. (2018), Tanrikulu and Erdur-Baker (2021), Wachs and Wright (2019), and Wachs et al. (2022a).
However, the motive of fun turned out to be significant as a moderating variable in our sample. Regarding the perception of hate speech as fun, the analysis of the moderating effect (indirect effect conditional) showed us that boys who do not perceive hate speech as fun are not more likely to perpetrate speech hate, even when they are recipients of hate speech (effect: 0.08 (CI95%: 0.01, 0.19). Thus, among boys, not finding hate speech funny is a protective factor against its dissemination (Figure 8).
Regarding the analysis of gender differences, no significant differences were found in terms of receiving and/or perpetrating hate speech, nor in the motivation to justify hate speech as revenge. Significant differences were found in the perception of hate speech as fun (Marinoni et al. 2024b) and in the scores obtained for the emotional self-control index. Therefore, we can say that, in the case of boys, perpetrating hate speech is mediated by low self-control and moderated by the consideration that hate speech is fun. In the case of girls, the variable emotional self-control significantly mediates the relationship between receiving and perpetrating hate speech, but we do not find that the variable fun moderates the likelihood of perpetrating hate speech.
It is conceivable that, in the case of boys, perpetrating hate speech is related to the need to seek approval from like-minded friends (Walther 2022) or the search for fraternal bonds from their gender community (Scotto di Carlo 2023).
In conclusion, (1) the relationship between receiving hate speech and perpetrating hate speech was not significant, but when the emotional self-regulation variable was added, the indirect effects were significant. A situation of total mediation was found; (2) although the revenge variable was a widespread justification for perpetrating hate speech, its moderating role between receiving hate speech and perpetrating it was not significant; and (3) for the variable considering hate speech as funny, it was found to have a significant moderating role in this relationship. This was particularly true in the case of boys, given that girls do not tend to consider hate speech as funny.

5. Limitations

The main limitation of this study is that the sample belongs to a very specific region of Spain and is relatively small, so although the results can be considered for other contexts (hate speech is generalized in all countries of the world), it would be convenient to conduct cross-cultural studies to compare the results with other cultures, lifestyles, or political regimes. Furthermore, this study was conducted at one point in time, so the results of emotional self-control mediation should be considered as associations, which requires longitudinal studies that allow for causal explanations.
Another limitation of this study is the difficulty in understanding the girls’ hate speech. Although they share the fact that receiving and perpetrating hate speech is mediated by emotional self-control, motives of revenge or fun do not significantly moderate this relationship. In this sense, it should be noted that we need new studies to help us better understand the relationship between receiving and perpetrating hate speech in the case of girls.
However, despite its limitations, this work provides evidence for an intervention to address hate speech among adolescents that may help reduce its prevalence. Improving adolescents’ emotional self-control can help prevent them from engaging in hate speech, even if they receive it. In the case of adolescents, it is necessary to promote the idea that those who perpetrate hate speech must accept their responsibility and not excuse themselves with the supposedly playful nature of their aggression—because hate speech is not fun.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, F.V.L. and P.R.M.; methodology, F.V.L. and P.R.M.; software, F.V.L. and P.R.M.; validation, F.V.L. and P.R.M.; formal analysis, F.V.L. and P.R.M.; investigation, F.V.L. and P.R.M.; resources, F.V.L. and P.R.M.; data curation, F.V.L.; writing—original draft preparation, F.V.L. and P.R.M.; writing—review and editing, F.V.L. and P.R.M.; visualization, F.V.L. and P.R.M.; supervision, F.V.L. and P.R.M.; project administration, F.V.L. and P.R.M.; funding acquisition, P.R.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

Ministry of Science and Innovation, Government of Spain: PID2021-127113OB-I00.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Comisión de Bioética of the Universidad. de Almería. (Ref:UALBIO2024/025).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data available in https://repositorio.ual.es/handle/10835/18556 (accessed on 27 February 2025).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Arrogante, Oscar. 2022. Sampling techniques and sample size calculation: How and how many participants should I select for my research? Intensive Nursing 33: 44–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Álvarez-Turrado, Blanca, Daniel Falla, and Eva M. Romera. 2024. Peer Presure and Cyberaggression in Adolescents: The Mediating Effects of Moral Disengagement Strategies. Youth & Society, ahead of print. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Ballaschk, Cindy, Sebastian Wachs, Norman Krause, Friederike Schulze-Reichelt, Julia Kansok-Dusche, Ludwig Bilz, and Wilfried Schubarth. 2021. Then everyone just goes along with it. A qualitative study on reasons and motives of hate speech among students. Discourse. Journal of Childhood and Adolescence Research 16: 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Bandura, Albert. 2016. Moral Disengagement: How People Do Harm and Live with Themselves. New York: Worth Publishers and McMillan. [Google Scholar]
  5. Barrientos Rastrojo, José. 2022. Philosophy for Children and Youth as a prevention and antidote to hate speech. ISEGORY Journal of Moral and Political Philosophy 67: 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Baumeister, Roy F., Kathleen D. Vohs, and Dianne M. Tice. 2007. The Strength Model of Self-Control. Current Directions in Psychological Science 16: 351–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Berkman, Elliot T., Jordan L. Livingston, and Lauren E. Kahn. 2017. Finding the “self” in self-regulation: The identity-value model. Psychological Inquiry 28: 77–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Biemer, Paul P., and Lars E. Lyberg. 2003. Introduction to Survey Quality. New York: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  9. Blaya, Catherine. 2019. Cyberhate: A review and content analysis of intervention strategies. Aggression and Violent Behavior 45: 163–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Brown, Alexander. 2018. What is so special about online (as compared to offline) hate speech? Ethnicities 18: 297–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Castaño-Pulgarín, Sergio Andrés, Natalia Suárez-Betancur, Luz Magnolia Tilano Vega, and Harvey Mauricio Herrera López. 2021. Internet, social media and online hate speech. Systematic review. Aggression and Violent Behavior 58: 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Costa, Susana, Bruno Mendes Da Silva, Ana Filipa Martins, and Alexandre Martins. 2024. Perceptions about online hate speech in games and gaming communities: Results from a survey in Portugal. Journal of Cyberspace Studies 8: 145–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Council of Europe. 2022. Combating Hate Speech. Available online: https://rm.coe.int/prems-083822-gbr-2018-recommendation-on-combating-hate-speech-memorand/1680a710c9 (accessed on 1 March 2025).
  14. Cover, Rob. 2023. Digital hostility, subjectivity and ethics: Theorising the disruption of identity in instances of mass online abuse and hate speech. Convergence 29: 308–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Delgado, Beatriz, Raquel Escortell, María Carmen Martínez-Monteagudo, Alicia Ferrández-Ferrer, and Ricardo Sanmartín. 2019. Cyberbullying, Self-concept and Academic Goals in Childhood. The Spanish Journal of Psychology 22: E46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Di Fátima, B., ed. Disinformation and Polarization in the Algorithmic Society: Online Hate Speech Trilogy. Covilhã: LabCom-University of Beira Interior, Cali: Universidad Icesi, vol. 1.
  17. Erjavec, Karmen, and Melita Poler Kovačič. 2012. “You Don’t Understand, This is a New War!” Analysis of Hate Speech in News Web Sites’ Comments. Mass Communication and Society 15: 899–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. EU Commission. 2016. Eurobarometer, S. 452, Media Pluralism and Democracy. Available online: https://ec.europa.eu (accessed on 7 February 2025).
  19. Fluck, Julia. 2017. Why Do Students Bully? An Analysis of Motives Behind Violence in Schools. Youth & Society 49: 567–87. [Google Scholar]
  20. Gaskin, J., and J. Lim. 2016. Model Fit Measures, AMOS Plugin. Gaskination’s StatWiki. [Google Scholar]
  21. Gradinger, Petra, Dagmar Strohmeier, and Christiane Spiel. 2012. Motives for bullying others in cyberspace: A study on bullies and bully-victims in Austria. In Cyberbullying in the Global Playground: Research from International Perspectives. Edited by Qing Li, Donna Cross and Peter K. Smith. Hoboken: Wiley Blackwell, pp. 263–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Gross, James, and Ross A. Thompson. 2007. Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In Handbook of Emotion Regulation. Edited by James J. Gross. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 3–24. [Google Scholar]
  23. Hayes, Andrew F. 2013. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis. New York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  24. Hofmann, Wilhelm, Malte Friese, and Fritz Strack. 2009. Impulse and Self-Control From a Dual-Systems Perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science 4: 162–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Hu, Li-tze, and Peter M. Bentler. 1999. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 6: 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Jane, Emma A. 2015. Flaming? What flaming? The pitfalls and potentials of researching online hostility. Ethics and Information Technology 17: 65–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Kansok-Dusche, Julia, Cindy Ballaschk, Norman Krause, Anke Zeißig, Lisanne Seemann-Herz, Sebastian Wachs, and Ludwig Bilz. 2023. A systematic review on hate speech among children and adolescents: Definitions, prevalence, and overlap with related phenomena. Trauma, Violence & Abuse 24: 2598–615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Kolotouchkina, Olga, Celia Rangel, and Patricia Núñez Gómez. 2023. Digital Media and Younger Audiences. Media and Communication 11: 124–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Koole, Sander L. 2008. The psychology of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Cognition and Emotion 23: 4–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Kowalski, Robin M., and Susan P. Limber. 2007. Electronic bullying among middle school students. Journal of Adolescent Health 41: 22–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Li, Qingqing, Guangcan Xiang, Shiqing Song, Mingyue Xiao, and Hong Chen. 2021. Trait self-control mediates the association between resting-state neural correlates and emotional well-being in late adolescence. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 16: 632–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Lilleker, Darren, and Marta Pérez-Escolar. 2023. Demonising migrants in contexts of extremism: Analysis of hate speech in UK and Spain. Politics and Governance 2: 127–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Marinoni, Carlo, Marco Rizzo, and Maria Assunta Zanetti. 2024a. Fake profiles and time spent online during the COVID 19 pandemic: A real risk for cyberbullying? Current Psychology 43: 26639–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Marinoni, Carlo, Marco Rizzo, and Maria Assunta Zanetti. 2024b. Social Media, Online Gaming, and Cyberbullying during the COVID-19 Pandemic: The Mediation Effect of Time Spent Online. Adolescents 4: 297–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Musante, Kathleen, and Billie R. DeWalt. 2011. Participant Observation: A Guide for Fieldworkers. Plymouth: AltaMira Press. [Google Scholar]
  36. O′Brien, Ed. 2014. Feeling connected to younger versus older selves: The asymmetric impact of life stage orientation. Cognition and Emotion 29: 678–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Paschke, Lena M., Denise Dörfel, Rosa Steimke, Ima Trempler, Amadeus Magrabi, Vera U. Ludwig, Torsten Schubert, Christine Stelzel, and Henrik Walter. 2016. Individual differences in self-reported self-control predict successful emotion regulation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 11: 1193–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Paz, María Antonia, Julio Montero-Díaz, and Alicia Moreno-Delgado. 2020. Hate Speech: A Systematized Review. Sage Open 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Pinker, Steven. 2011. Los Ángeles que Llevamos Dentro: El Declive de la Violencia y sus Implicaciones [The Better Angels of Our Nature: Why Violence Has Declined]. Barcelona: Paidós. [Google Scholar]
  40. Poland, Bailey. 2016. Haters Harassment, Abuse, and Violence on Line. Sterling: Potomac Books. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Ramón Ortiz, Julia Ángela, and Jesús Vilchez Guizado. 2023. Hate speech in secondary education: Intellectual and emotional effects. Visual Review. International Visual Culture Review 14: 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Rodriguez Ramos, Marta. 2022. La Fiscalía General del Estado y los delitos de odio: ¿Una falta de respeto al Derecho Internacional? [The Attorney General’s Office and hate crimes: A lack of respect for international law?]. Revista Penal 49: 146–59. Available online: https://revistapenal.tirant.com/index.php/revista-penal/article/view/134 (accessed on 10 February 2025).
  43. Rodríguez-Martínez, Pilar, Juan Manuel Bellido-Cáceres, Lucia Martínez-Joya, Joaquín J. Sánchez-Espinosa, Antonio Segura-Sánchez, Francisco Villegas-Lirola, Christian Roith, Antonia Lozano-Diaz, Antonia Sánchez-Villanueva, Pilar Rios-Campos, and et al. 2025. DIS-ODIO Questionnaire Database. DIS-ODIO Project (PID2021-127113OB-100). University of Almería; Base de Datos Discursos de odio DIS-ODIO (PID2021-127113OB-I00). Available online: https://repositorio.ual.es/handle/10835/18556 (accessed on 15 February 2025).
  44. Runions, Kevin C., Christina Salmivalli, Therese Shaw, Sharyn Burns, and Donna Cross. 2018. Beyond the reactive-proactive dichotomy: Rage, revenge, reward, and recreational aggression predict early high school bully and bully/victim status. Aggressive Behavior 44: 501–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Schmid, Ursula Kristin, Anna Sophie Kümpel, and Diana Rieger. 2024. Social media users’ motives for (not) engaging with hate speech: An explorative investigation. Social Media & Society 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Schwertberger, Ulrike, and Diana Rieger. 2021. Hass und seine vielen Gesichter: Eine sozial- und kommunikationswissenschaftliche Einordnung von Hate Speech. In Hate Speech—Multidisziplinäre Analysen und Handlungsoptionen. Edited by Sebastian Wachs, Barbara Koch-Priewe and Andreas Zick. Wiesbaden: Springer VS. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Scotto di Carlo, Giuseppina. 2023. An analysis of self-other representations in the incelosphere: Between online misogyny and self-contempt. Discourse & Society 34: 3–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Sponholz, Liriam. 2023. Hate Speech. In Challenges and Perspectives of Hate Speech Research. Edited by Christian Strippel, Sünje Paasch-Colberg, Martin Emmer and Joachim Trebbe. Berlin: Deutsche Gesellschaft für Publizistik- und Kommunikationswissenschaften, Fachgruppe Digitale Kommunikation, vol. 12, pp. 143–63. Available online: https://www.ssoar.info/ssoar/handle/document/86292 (accessed on 15 February 2025).
  49. Steinberg, Laurence. 2008. A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review 28: 78–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Tanrikulu, Ibrahim, and Özgür Erdur-Baker. 2021. Motives Behind Cyberbullying Perpetration: A Test of Uses and Gratifications Theory. Journal of Interpersonal Violence 36: 13–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Teruel, Pilar, Carlos Salavera, Pablo Usán, and José L. Antoñanzas. 2019. Self- and other-centered emotional intelligence: Rotterdam Emotional Intelligence Scale (REIS). Universitas Psychologica 8: 1–12. [Google Scholar]
  52. Turner, Noah, Thomas J. Holt, Russell Brewer, Jesse Cale, and Andrew Goldsmith. 2022. Exploring the Relationship between Opportunity and Self-Control in Youth Exposure to and Sharing of Online Hate Content. Terrorism and Political Violence 35: 1604–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Van de Bongardt, Daphne, Ellen Reitz, Theo Sandfort, and Maja Deković. 2015. A meta-analysis of the relations between three types of peer norms and adolescent sexual behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review 19: 203–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Van Dorn, Janne. 2018. Anger, Feelings of Revenge, and Hate. Emotion Review 10: 321–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Villegas-Lirola, Francisco. 2024. Emotional Intelligence Profiles and Cyber-Victimization in Secondary School Students: A Multilevel Analysis. Education Sciences 14: 971. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Wachs, Sebastian, Alexander Wettstein, Ludwig Bilz, and Manuel Gámez-Guadix. 2022a. Motives for hate speech in adolescence and their relation to social norms. Scientific Journal of Educommunication. Comunicar 71: 9–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Wachs, Sebastian, and Michelle F. Wright. 2019. The Moderation of Online Disinhibition and Sex on the Relationship Between Online Hate Victimization and Perpetration. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking 22: 300–06. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Wachs, Sebastian, Juan Manuel Machimbarrena, Michelle F. Wright, Manuel Gámez-Guadix, Soeun Yang, Ruthaychonnee Sittichai, Ritu Singh, Ramakrishna Biswal, Katerina Flora, Vassiliki Daskalou, and et al. 2022b. Associations between Coping Strategies and Cyberhate Involvement: Evidence from Adolescents across Three World Regions. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19: 6749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Walrave, Michel, and Wannes Heirman. 2011. Cyberbullying: Predicting Victimization and Perpetration. Children & Society, 25, 59–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Walther, Joseph B. 2022. Social media and online hate. Current Opinion in Psichology 45: 101298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Wardle, Claire, and Hossein Derakhshan. 2017. Information Disorder: Toward an Interdisciplinary Framework for Research and Policymaking; Council of Europe. Available online: https://bit.ly/3CThDbf (accessed on 1 March 2025).
  62. Weber, Ina, Heidi Vandebosch, Karolien Poels, and Sara Pabian. 2024. The ecology of online hate speech: Mapping expert perspectives on the drivers for online hate perpetration with the Delphi method. Aggressive Behavior 50: e22136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. West, Samuel J., and Nicholas D. Thomson. 2025. Clarifying the relationship between trait aggression and self-control using random item slope regression. Journal of Personality 93: 581–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Conceptual model of moderated mediation.
Figure 1. Conceptual model of moderated mediation.
Socsci 14 00349 g001
Figure 2. Scree plot showing extraction of two factors for Emotional Self-Control Index.
Figure 2. Scree plot showing extraction of two factors for Emotional Self-Control Index.
Socsci 14 00349 g002
Figure 3. Emotional Self-Control Index.
Figure 3. Emotional Self-Control Index.
Socsci 14 00349 g003
Figure 4. Standardized solution of the confirmatory factor analysis of the emotional self-regulation index.
Figure 4. Standardized solution of the confirmatory factor analysis of the emotional self-regulation index.
Socsci 14 00349 g004
Figure 5. Statistical diagram of regression result 1. Mediation and moderation (indirect effects). p < 0.05 * means that the probability of obtaining the observed (or more extreme) results if the null hypothesis were true is less than 5%. This is considered a statistically significant result, indicating that the results are unlikely to be due to chance. p < 0.01 ** means that the p-value (probability) of a result is less than 0.01 (or 1%). This indicates that, if the null hypothesis were true, there is only a 1% probability of observing the results (or more extreme results) obtained in the study. In simple terms, it means that the results are highly significant and unlikely if the null hypothesis is correct, which leads to rejecting the null hypothesis and accepting the alternative hypothesis.
Figure 5. Statistical diagram of regression result 1. Mediation and moderation (indirect effects). p < 0.05 * means that the probability of obtaining the observed (or more extreme) results if the null hypothesis were true is less than 5%. This is considered a statistically significant result, indicating that the results are unlikely to be due to chance. p < 0.01 ** means that the p-value (probability) of a result is less than 0.01 (or 1%). This indicates that, if the null hypothesis were true, there is only a 1% probability of observing the results (or more extreme results) obtained in the study. In simple terms, it means that the results are highly significant and unlikely if the null hypothesis is correct, which leads to rejecting the null hypothesis and accepting the alternative hypothesis.
Socsci 14 00349 g005
Figure 6. Statistical diagram of regression result 2. p < 0.05 * means that the probability of obtaining the observed (or more extreme) results if the null hypothesis were true is less than 5%. This is considered a statistically significant result, indicating that the results are unlikely to be due to chance.
Figure 6. Statistical diagram of regression result 2. p < 0.05 * means that the probability of obtaining the observed (or more extreme) results if the null hypothesis were true is less than 5%. This is considered a statistically significant result, indicating that the results are unlikely to be due to chance.
Socsci 14 00349 g006
Figure 7. Statistical Diagram (Moderated Mediation—model 7 PROCESS v4.2). p < 0.05 * means that the probability of obtaining the observed (or more extreme) results if the null hypothesis were true is less than 5%. This is considered a statistically significant result, indicating that the results are unlikely to be due to chance. p < 0.01 ** means that the p-value (probability) of a result is less than 0.01 (or 1%). This indicates that, if the null hypothesis were true, there is only a 1% probability of observing the results (or more extreme results) obtained in the study. In simple terms, it means that the results are highly significant and unlikely if the null hypothesis is correct, which leads to rejecting the null hypothesis and accepting the alternative hypothesis.
Figure 7. Statistical Diagram (Moderated Mediation—model 7 PROCESS v4.2). p < 0.05 * means that the probability of obtaining the observed (or more extreme) results if the null hypothesis were true is less than 5%. This is considered a statistically significant result, indicating that the results are unlikely to be due to chance. p < 0.01 ** means that the p-value (probability) of a result is less than 0.01 (or 1%). This indicates that, if the null hypothesis were true, there is only a 1% probability of observing the results (or more extreme results) obtained in the study. In simple terms, it means that the results are highly significant and unlikely if the null hypothesis is correct, which leads to rejecting the null hypothesis and accepting the alternative hypothesis.
Socsci 14 00349 g007
Figure 8. Protective role of considering that hate speech is not funny.
Figure 8. Protective role of considering that hate speech is not funny.
Socsci 14 00349 g008
Table 1. Cutoff criteria for fit indices in covariance structure analysis (Hu and Bentler 1999).
Table 1. Cutoff criteria for fit indices in covariance structure analysis (Hu and Bentler 1999).
MeasureTerribleAcceptableExcellent
IFC<0.90<0.95>1
RMSEA>0.08>0.06<0.06
PClose<0.01<0.05>0.05
Table 2. Emotional self-control index scale by percentiles.
Table 2. Emotional self-control index scale by percentiles.
Level of Self-ControlScoreFrequency (%)
Very Low≤1290 (21.8%)
Low-Medium13–15115 (27.8%)
Medium1645 (10.9%)
High-Medium17–1889 (21.5%)
Very High>1974 (17.9%)
413 (100%)
Table 3. Model fit measures in Confirmatory Factor Analysis (Gaskin and Lim 2016).
Table 3. Model fit measures in Confirmatory Factor Analysis (Gaskin and Lim 2016).
MeasureEstimateThresholdInterpretation
IFC1.00<0.95Excellent
RMSEA0.00>0.06Excellent
PClose0.714>0.05Excellent
Table 4. Mean comparison test for the variable receiving hate speech by gender.
Table 4. Mean comparison test for the variable receiving hate speech by gender.
Tests for Homogeneity of Variances
Levene’s statisticSg
Based on the trimmed mean2.098 0.124
ANOVASum of squaresdfRoot mean squareFSig
Between groups0.62620.3131.4760.230
Within groups86.9344100.212
Total87.559412
HSD Tuckey
Gender (I)Gender (J)Mean difference (I–J)SigCI95% LowerCI 95% Higher
ManWoman0.0270.823−0.080.14
ManNon-binary−0.1870.306−0.490.11
WomanMan−0.0270.823−0.140.08
WomanNon-binary−0.2150.210−0.510.08
Non-binaryMan0.1880.306−0.110.49
Non-binaryWoman0.2150.210−0.080.51
Table 5. Mean comparison test for the variable perpetrating hate speech by gender.
Table 5. Mean comparison test for the variable perpetrating hate speech by gender.
Tests for Homogeneity of Variances
Levene’s statisticSg
Based on the trimmed mean9.887 <0.001
WelchStatisticianDf 1Df2 Sig
0.908236.07 0.412
Games–Howel
Gender (I)Gender (J)Mean difference (I–J)SigCI 95% LowerCI 95% Higher
ManWoman0.0320.794−0.0850.149
ManNon-binary−0.1360.561−0.4740.202
WomanMan−0.0320.794−0.1490.085
WomanNon-binary−0.1680.419−0.5060.169
Non-binaryMan0.1360.561−0.2020.474
Non-binaryWoman0.1680.419−0.1690.506
Table 6. Mean comparison test for the variable perpetrating hate speech is fun by gender.
Table 6. Mean comparison test for the variable perpetrating hate speech is fun by gender.
Tests for Homogeneity of Variances
Levene’s statisticSg
Based on the trimmed mean21.883 <0.001
WelchStatisticianDf 1Df2 Sig
12.113234.281 <0.001
Games–Howel
Gender (I)Gender (J)Mean difference (I–J)SigCI 95% LowerCI 95% Higher
ManWoman0.423<0.001 *0.170.67
ManNon-binary−1.0200.088−2.180.14
WomanMan−0.423<0.001 *−0.67−0.17
WomanNon-binary−1.4430.015 *−2.60−0.29
Non-binaryMan1.0200.443−0.142.18
Non-binaryWoman1.4430.015 *0.292.60
* Significant p-value (<0.05).
Table 7. Mean comparison test for the variable revenge justifies perpetrating hate speech by gender.
Table 7. Mean comparison test for the variable revenge justifies perpetrating hate speech by gender.
Tests for Homogeneity of Variances
Levene’s statisticSg
Based on the trimmed mean0.022 0.978
ANOVASum of squaresdfRoot mean squareFSig
Between groups6.30923.1541.3800.253
Within groups936.9704102.285
Total943.278412
HSD Tuckey
Gender (I)Gender (J)Mean difference (I–J)SigCI 95% LowerCI 95% Higher
ManWoman0.2000.386−0.160.56
ManNon-binary−0.3120.736−1.300.67
WomanMan−0.2000.386−0.560.16
WomanNon-binary−0.5120.438−1.490.47
Non-binaryMan0.3130.736−0.671.30
Non-binaryWoman0.5120.438−0.651.67
Table 8. Mean comparison test for the variable emotional self-control by gender.
Table 8. Mean comparison test for the variable emotional self-control by gender.
Tests for Homogeneity of Variances
Levene’s statisticSg
Based on the trimmed mean0.922 0.399
ANOVASum of squaresdfRoot mean squareFSig
Between groups146.074273.0377.123<0.001
Within groups4204.21141010.254
Total4350.286412
HSD Tuckey
Gender (I)Gender (J)Mean difference (I–J)SigCI 95% LowerCI 95% Higher
ManWoman−0.3070.605−1.060.45
ManNon-binary3.020.002 *0.935.10
WomanMan0.3070.605−0.441.06
WomanNon-binary3.33<0.001 *1.245.40
Non-binaryMan−3.020.002 *−5.88−0.15
Non-binaryWoman−3.33<0.001 *−5.41−0.47
* Significant p-value (<0.05).
Table 9. Percentages of hate profiles by gender.
Table 9. Percentages of hate profiles by gender.
Hate Speech ProfilesBoysGirlsNon-BinaryTotal
Non-victims—non-perpetrators56 (29.2%)76 (36.7%)2 (14.3%)134 (32.4%)
No victims—yes perpetrators76 (39.6%)72 (34.8%)5 (35.7%)153 (37.0%)
Yes victims—no perpetrators25 (13.0%)18 (8.7%)2 (14.3%)45 (10.9%)
Yes victims—yes perpetrators35 (18.2%)41 (19.8%)5 (35.7%)81 (19.6%)
Totals19220714413
Table 10. Analysis of revenge moderation in the relationship between hate speech and emotional self-control index.
Table 10. Analysis of revenge moderation in the relationship between hate speech and emotional self-control index.
coefftotpLLCIULCI
Constant15.730.36742.630.00015.0016.45
E1_1L (independent variable)−1.530.72−2.120.03−2.95−0.11
Revenge (moderating variable)−0.070.13−0.590.55−0.320.17
Int (E1_1L * Revenge)0.250.231.100.27−0.190.71
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Villegas Lirola, F.; Rodriguez Martinez, P. The Mediation (Emotional Self-Control) and Moderation (Fun) of the Relationship Between Receiving and Perpetrating Hate Speech Among Boys and Girls in Almeria (Spain). Soc. Sci. 2025, 14, 349. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci14060349

AMA Style

Villegas Lirola F, Rodriguez Martinez P. The Mediation (Emotional Self-Control) and Moderation (Fun) of the Relationship Between Receiving and Perpetrating Hate Speech Among Boys and Girls in Almeria (Spain). Social Sciences. 2025; 14(6):349. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci14060349

Chicago/Turabian Style

Villegas Lirola, Francisco, and Pilar Rodriguez Martinez. 2025. "The Mediation (Emotional Self-Control) and Moderation (Fun) of the Relationship Between Receiving and Perpetrating Hate Speech Among Boys and Girls in Almeria (Spain)" Social Sciences 14, no. 6: 349. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci14060349

APA Style

Villegas Lirola, F., & Rodriguez Martinez, P. (2025). The Mediation (Emotional Self-Control) and Moderation (Fun) of the Relationship Between Receiving and Perpetrating Hate Speech Among Boys and Girls in Almeria (Spain). Social Sciences, 14(6), 349. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci14060349

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop