1. Introduction
The majority of parents in Taiwan consistently expect their children to behave well, perform well in school, and achieve great things in the future. Students who implicitly comprehend the self-worth measured by competence in this achievement-oriented environment may strive to be the best (
Mofield and Peters 2018). They might hold the idea that if they succeed or are perfect, it means that they have been valued. The thoughts of a perfectionist might reinforce the beliefs about ability (beliefs about whether the ability is malleable vs. static). If pupils’ great abilities are demonstrated, they further solidify their idea that their intelligence and talent are fixed. Many gifted students have good cognitive abilities and quickly achieve high educational performance. Therefore, they execute their idealized plan in order to achieve the best performance and win the respect, and be identified by their parents and teachers. Due to this, gifted students experience greater pressure from their parents, instructors, and their own expectations than average students do. Boys feel more pressure from their parents if they are given more attention than girls.
Perfectionism has long been recognized as a psychological factor that can enhance or interfere with the healthy adjustment of young students who are academically gifted (
Grugan et al. 2021). Most studies conclude that perfectionism has both advantages and disadvantages (
Silverman 1999). According to
Hamachek (
1978), perfectionism should be classified into two types, normal perfectionism and neurotic perfectionism, and considered in a continuous form ranging from excellent to nervous. For example, positive perfectionism tends to lead to a healthy pursuit of high standards and being excellent, whereas maladaptive perfectionism results in anxiety and unsatisfactory results.
Mofield and Peters (
2018) confirmed that the perfectionism of gifted students is related to the various concepts of adaptive results (
Mofield and Peters 2018). Accordingly, there is growing agreement that perfectionism is a multidimensional concept made up of two larger dimensions: perfectionism concerns and perfectionism strivings (
Stricker et al. 2020).
Aspects of perfectionistic concerns have a negative impact on academic effectiveness, grade point average (GPA), life satisfaction, and happiness among gifted students (
Stricker et al. 2020). However, aspects of perfectionistic strivings are connected to academic success, GPA, and life satisfaction in a favorable way (
Stricker et al. 2020). According to the findings of a meta-analytic study, students who were intellectually gifted showed higher levels of perfectionistic striving but similar levels of perfectionistic concerns as compared to non-gifted students (
Stricker et al. 2020). For students who are academically gifted, perfectionistic concerns (PC) are likely to be uniformly crippling, but perfectionistic strivings (PS) are linked to more inconsistent results (
Grugan et al. 2021). Evidently, the perfectionist striving of gifted students may lead to greater test scores than non-gifted students and different outcomes. Moreover, gifted-individuals’ perfectionism concerns have detrimental effects and are identical to those of non-gifted students.
According to Dweck, people’s beliefs about the fixedness and malleability of their personal attributes, such as their intelligence, are expressed in their self-theories (
Dweck and Molden 2005). Do people think that their intelligence is a fixed attribute or something that they can develop with study and effort? Self-theories can be described as either having a fixed mindset or a growth mindset. If gifted students experience a form of perfectionism that conceives of their abilities as outstanding, they might be afraid of failure, have a decreased tolerance for frustration, and even develop maladaptive behaviors. If they feel that their efforts improved their performance, they might have the courage to accept a challenge in order to achieve greater success. In contrast to gifted achievers, gifted underachievers in grades 6–8 demonstrated poorer levels of organization, self-regulation, and motivation, according to research by
Mofield and Peters (
2019). They also exhibited stronger fixed-mindset beliefs about intelligence (
Mofield and Peters 2019). The fixed mindset of unhealthy perfectionists was significantly higher than healthy perfectionists and nonperfectionists, indicating that targeting mindset adjustment may be a feasible method for unhealthy perfectionists (
Chan 2012). Additionally, for the whole sample of gifted students, fixed-mindset beliefs predicted both aspects of evaluative-concerns perfectionism (concern over mistakes and doubt of action), whereas growth-mindset beliefs predicted both aspects of positive-striving perfectionism (personal standards and organization)
Mofield and Peters (
2019). Apparently, gifted students may convert maladaptive perfectionism into adaptive perfectionism if they have good coping skills or constructive thinking.
In addition to being directly associated with bad results, perfectionism has also been connected to the specific ways in which people try to deal with their daily issues and the resulting emotions of distress (
Flett et al. 1994). Several forms of perfectionism were connected to either a positive or negative coping style. Constructive thinking is a method for resolving everyday issues and a procedure for easing the pressure that leads to subpar performance (
Burns and Fedewa 2005). Poor constructive thinkers experienced the helplessness pattern noted by Dweck and her colleagues when faced with coping situations (
Flett et al. 1994). The Constructive Thinking Inventory (CTI) offers several measures, such as summary scores for global constructive thinking, emotional coping, behavioral coping, categorical thinking, personal superstitious thinking, naive optimism, and esoteric thinking (
Flett et al. 1994). According to
Flett et al. (
1994), socially prescribed perfectionism was linked to less constructive thinking and more negative coping on the majority of CTI subscales. These correlations remained even after adjusting for levels of depression symptoms. Self-oriented perfectionism was healthy in that it was linked to proactive behavioral coping strategies, but it was unhealthy in that it was linked to emotional coping strategies that involved lower levels of self-acceptance (
Flett et al. 1994). It seems that whatever types of perfectionist gifted students there are, if they hold inappropriate perfectionism beliefs, they might have negative effects.
Most developmental theories have been specifically concerned with children and the scientific understanding of age-related changes in experience and behavior (
Sharma n.d.). The analyses in
Ogurlu (
2020) also focused on the two moderators of perfectionism dimensions and grade level. In addition, gender is one of the first categories children learn, and the categorization of people into men and women affects almost every aspect of our lives, especially our self-concept and our perceptions of others (
Morgenroth and Ryan 2018). In psychology, age and gender are the vital factors that affect students’ cognitive and mental changes. Therefore, the study aimed to investigate the differences in the perfectionism, cognitive mindset, constructed thinking, and emotional intelligence of primary gifted students by grade and gender.
5. Discussion
The dimensions of perfectionism varied significantly by grade. Students in the third grade tended to value ego-syntonic perfections such as order, satisfaction, and contingent self-worth more. Students in the fourth grade exhibit hypercriticism and other-oriented perfectionism as well as satisfaction and contingent self-worth. Gifted children in the sixth grade had more critical qualities than students in earlier grades. It was shown that the perfectionist features of students could alter as they become older, and educators should be cautious about this issue. The outcome was consistent with those of
Chen (
1996),
Uz Baş (
2011), and
DeKryger (
2005). Children’s perfectionist tendencies appeared to be more noticeable and advantageous when they were in the third or fourth grade. They tended to become more critical as they grew older. The majority of gifted students had a tendency for self-oriented perfectionism, organization, and hypercriticism, which was the cause of this. They may put a lot of pressure on themselves to perform well because of demands from their parents, professors, or classmates, while also making requests of others to maintain their good work.
The dimensions of perfectionism varied significantly according to gender. The findings were consistent with
Uz Baş’s (
2011) study, which found that girls were more sensitive to errors than boys. It seems that girls are more prone to judge themselves depending on their performance; thus, they will hold themselves to high standards and worry about making mistakes. Boys often had an entitlement attitude and exhibited narcissism. The perception that men predominate in Eastern society may also have an impact on this phenomenon. For the majority of the subscales, there were no significant differences according to gender; this finding was, likewise, similar to those of
Siegle and Schuler (
2000),
Chen (
1996), as well as
Rice et al. (
2004). Furthermore, gifted boys and girls shared the same ideals of self-centered perfection and satisfaction. Girls endorsed organization, setting high goals, and planning as well. The cause for this was that gifted perfectionists in elementary school receive high grades for their academic work, which encourages them to pursue perfectionism and feel content with their results.
The mindset did not vary by grade. Gifted students in third through sixth grades all slightly disagreed with a fixed mindset and a growth mindset. However, it appears that third graders tend to believe that intelligence and talent are fixed, whereas fourth graders believe that these traits are malleable. While the findings of
Dweck (
2006,
2007) and
Schroder et al. (
2017) were not replicated here, fixed-mindset messages predominated among students in the achievement range, while older children tended to support the growth mindset more (
Boaler 2013). There were no differences in cognitive mindset between the genders. These results were different from those of
Dweck (
2006,
2007) and
Boaler (
2013), but similar to those of
Macnamara and Rupani (
2017). The cause for this is that gifted students in elementary school have not yet cultivated a fixed mindset or growth mindset. Educators could be concerned with their affective development, to develop a growth mindset.
According to the reverse counting of mistrust of others, third- and fifth-grade students clearly have a higher tendency to trust individuals than fourth-grade students. There were no obvious differences in the level of constructive thinking by grade, with the exception of mistrust of others. This conclusion did not corroborate the study of
Park et al. (
1997) nor
Epstein and Meier’s (
1989) assumption that older students’ constructive-thinking skills were superior to those of younger students. There were no apparent differences in constructive thinking between genders. The findings of
Li (
2003),
Pihet et al. (
2011), and
Roothman et al. (
2003) could not be compared to those of the study. Third through sixth graders shared a common understanding of dealing with frustration, mistrusting others, and dichotomous thinking, despite the fact that levels of mistrust differed dramatically by grade. Third through fifth graders agreed on optimistic action. Between third and fifth graders, there was agreement on superstitious thinking as well. It appears that the majority of gifted students have developed the coping skills of frustration and proactive action. In addition, educators must be aware of the detrimental effects that distrusting others, thinking in dichotomies, and superstitious thinking have on gifted students.
There were no variations in any of the emotional-intelligence subscales by grade. The outcome was consistent with those of
Çelik and Deniz (
2008),
Birks et al. (
2009),
Faisal and Ghani (
2015), and
Nasir and Masrur (
2010), but it did not correspond to the hypothesis of
Bar-On (
2006). The emotional intelligence of gifted children is medium and, moving forward, it will be essential to enhance these emotional-intelligence knowledge and skills. There was a significant difference in the dimensions of emotional intelligence by gender. Boys keep themselves in a pleasant mood and have nice introspection. Girls are also quite good at interacting with others. The findings of
Li (
2003),
Katyal and Awasthi (
2005), and
Harrod and Scheer (
2005) results, that grade-school girls scored much higher on emotional capacities than boys (
Nasir and Masrur 2010), did not align with this study’s findings. This was in contrast to the research of
Roothman et al. (
2003), which found no appreciable gender differences in emotional intelligence. In addition, boys and girls all agreed on interpersonal relationships. It may be too much pressure for apparently brilliant girls to maintain a positive attitude because they place so much emphasis on the performance of interpersonal interactions, self-oriented perfectionism, and high standards. Boys also favored self-oriented perfection and satisfaction so they could be happy and reflect.
6. Conclusions
The study found that younger pupils exhibited more pleasant perfectionist characteristics such as contingent self-worth, order, and satisfaction. Students in sixth grade outperformed third and fifth graders only in hypercriticism. When kids were in the third or fourth grade, their perfectionist tendencies seemed to be more apparent and positive. As they grew older, they tended to become more critical. Girls also tended to judge themselves more harshly and worry about making mistakes because they were more likely to evaluate themselves based on their performance. Boys frequently exhibited narcissistic traits and placed expectations on other people. This behavior might also be influenced by the idea that men predominate in Eastern society. Instructors must be aware that gifted students may exhibit perfectionist traits that can vary with age and gender.
According to grade or gender, there were no variations in cognitive mindset. The outcome differed from that of
Dweck (
2006,
2007), where older children tended to support the growth mindset more, while fixed-mindset messages predominated among students throughout the achievement spectrum (
Boaler 2013). According to the study, gifted primary-school pupils did not acquire a fixed or growth attitude. They still have the ability to develop a growth mindset by gaining relevant knowledge. It is crucial for educators to be aware of this and consult with pupils who have fixed mindsets as soon as possible.
Only in mistrust of others did the third- and fifth-grade pupils do considerably better than the fourth-grade students; there were no grade-specific differences in the other subscales. Each subscale of constructive thinking showed no gender differences. Teachers must be concerned about the trust that gifted third and fifth graders feel in them. There were gender differences in introspection, interpersonal interactions, and general mood, but not in any of the emotional-intelligence subscales by grade. Boys are adept at reflecting on themselves and maintaining a positive mood. Girls also have excellent interpersonal relationships. Teachers could observe gifted girls’ inner feelings and gifted boys’ interpersonal relationships.
There were two limitations to the study. The study only explored the differences in grade and gender levels of perfectionism, mindset, constructive thinking, and emotional intelligence. In addition, the study did not conduct further interviews with gifted pupils to make sure of their inner thinking. There were some discrepancies with previous studies; to further analyze the data in the future, researchers could broaden the research subjects, select alternative variables, or use quantitative research techniques. Educators could gain a better understanding of the affective straits of gifted students as a result of the study and they could be more concerned about these issues in the future.