Next Article in Journal
Using Mixed Reality to Support Inclusive Teaching Strategies in General and Special Education Preparation Programs
Previous Article in Journal
Power and Precarity: First Generation Students Compose Digital Stories of Class Mobility
Previous Article in Special Issue
Dating Conflict-Resolution Tactics and Exposure to Family Violence: University Students’ Experiences
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Sibling Violence and Position in Sibling Dyad in a Sample of Adolescents: How Does It Relate to Self-Esteem?

by
Catarina Pinheiro Mota
1,2,*,
Joana Rita Sousa
1 and
Inês Carvalho Relva
1,3,4
1
Department of Education and Psychology, University of Trás-os-Montes and Alto Douro, Quinta de Prados, 5000-801 Vila Real, Portugal
2
Center for Psychology, University of Porto, 4200-135 Porto, Portugal
3
Research Center in Sports Sciences, Health Sciences and Human Development (CIDESD), University of Trás-os-Montes and Alto Douro, 5001-801 Vila Real, Portugal
4
Centre for Research and Intervention in Education (CIIE), University of Porto, 4200-135 Porto, Portugal
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Soc. Sci. 2023, 12(11), 595; https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci12110595
Submission received: 31 July 2023 / Revised: 19 October 2023 / Accepted: 21 October 2023 / Published: 26 October 2023

Abstract

:
(1) Background: Research regarding sibling violence is still scarce, although it is the most common type of intrafamily violence. Every sibling’s position in the sibling dyad seems to influence this type of violent conduct since every status has its characteristics. Siblings involved in aggressive behavior seem to be described as having low self-esteem. This study intends to test the predictive effect of self-esteem, sibling position and sex on sibling violence development. (2) Method: The sample consists of 286 students, aged between 12 and 17 years, from both sexes. A social demographic questionnaire and the Revised Conflict Tactics Scales—the Portuguese Sibling Version (CTS2-SP) and the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale were used for data collection. (3) Results: The results show an association between self-esteem in sibling violence, as well as an association between sibling position on negotiation and sexual coercion’s perpetration and victimization. Sex also predicts the negotiation of psychological aggression’s perpetration and psychological and physical aggression’s victimization. (4) Discussion: the results will be discussed according to the attachment theory, considering the importance of affective bonds with siblings as adaptive development facilitators.

1. Introduction

1.1. Attachment and the Development of Sibling Violence and Self-Esteem

According to Bowlby (1988), in infancy, the child begins to elaborate conscious and unconscious mental representations about himself and the external world in which he is involved. These mental representations are based on the subject’s experiences with significant figures and, more specifically, on the availability and responsiveness demonstrated. Given this availability and responsiveness of primary caregivers, the child, when asked, develops a model of himself as someone deserving of affection and a model of the other as someone available to meet his needs. All these elaborated perceptions about oneself, others and the surrounding world are characterized by their ability to adapt to all life cycle phases, calling themselves dynamic internal models. These dynamic internal models are responsible for modeling the cognitions, affects and interpersonal behaviors that emerge in later relationships (Bowlby [1969] 1982). According to Bowlby (1988), all behaviors aimed at establishing or maintaining a close relationship with a representative person are called attachment behaviors. The secure base is characterized by its permanent availability and is only used when strictly necessary, as in distress situations (Bowlby 1988).
In adolescence, the so-called separation–individuation process begins, through which the young person becomes independent of his parents and extends his support network to other important people. At this stage, siblings are seen as a source of emotional support and are sought out for advice in the adversities that arise during this process (Seginer 1998). It should be noted that this expansion of attachments is facilitated by the sense of security and trust provided by primary caregivers, their secure base (Fraley and Davis 1997; Hazan and Zeifman 1994; Mayseless 2004; Meeus et al. 2002; Nickerson and Nagle 2005). In turn, siblings become safe havens, and this relationship is characterized by its symmetry as they also provide and receive help (Mota and Rocha 2012).
Although there are few studies on sibling attachment (Kiang and Furman 2007; Geraldes et al. 2013; Mota et al. 2017), the possibility that older siblings can become caregivers for youngest siblings, especially in stressful situations or in the absence of the primary caregiver, and even become their safe base, was first explored. In the relationship between siblings, a secure attachment is synonymous of a positive relationship, provided there is a relationship of trust characterized by reciprocity, symmetry and cooperation. However, some relationships are characterized by ambivalence, which does not mean that lasting affective bonds are not formed (Ainsworth 1989).
Assuming that family dynamics, particularly social interactions with siblings, teach conflict resolution techniques that are later applied to peer relationships and the more diverse the sibling dyad is, the better prepared siblings are for these later relationships (Fernandes 2002; Mota et al. 2017). In a study of 374 families, Yeh and Lempers (2004) found evidence that healthy sibling relationships promote high levels of self-esteem and improved relationships with peers. Conversely, sibling dynamics may involve negativity, as this is often associated with conflict situations.
Sibling violence is often overlooked, resulting in a shortage of the literature on the subject (Linares 2006). Some authors use the term bullying interchangeably with sibling violence when defining this type of aggression (Monks et al. 2009; Toseeb and Wolke 2022; Wolke et al. 2015). Sibling violence is often characterized by a power imbalance between the victim and the aggressor and it is uncommon for siblings to have identical physical and/or psychological characteristics, such as age, height and physical strength (Monks et al. 2009). Moreover, according to Hoffman and Edwards (2004), the aggressors in sibling dynamics may analyze each other’s characteristics to identify strengths and weaknesses, using this information to exert superiority over the victim before executing violent behaviors. Additionally, the lack of parental or adult supervision allows abusers to carry out their aggression at their convenience due to the extensive amount of time siblings spend together (Monks et al. 2009), as well as harsh parenting behavior (Tippett and Wolke 2015) or the use of the aggressive discipline (Relva et al. 2019). However, Krienert and Walsh (2011) emphasize that roles can be interchangeable in sibling violence and the aggressors may also be victims of violence between siblings.
Sibling violence can take the form of physical, psychological, sexual or relational violence (Relva et al. 2012). According to Wiehe (1997), physical violence is a deliberate act involving physically hurting someone with or without using an object. Psychological violence occurs when an aggressor uses intimidating language to scare, ridicule or belittle a sibling. There is still no consensus on the distinction between normal and abusive sexual contact (Relva et al. 2012). For Wiehe (1997), abusive sexual contact is considered inappropriate and can involve inappropriate touching, attempted penetration and using violence to force someone to have sex. Relational violence is a form of social violence that includes spreading negative rumors and publishing sensitive information online. In relational violence, the victim may not be aware of the aggression and the indirect nature of the perpetrator’s actions makes it easy for them to deny involvement (Caspi 2012), but with probable consequences for the adolescents’ adjustment (Gallagher et al. 2018).
Several studies on sibling violence (e.g., Khan and Rogers 2015; Kiselica and Morrill-Richards 2007; Relva et al. 2014; Relva et al. 2013; Simonelli et al. 2002) investigate the traits of both perpetrators and victims. This study only considers sex, social class and position in the sibling dyad. Sex stereotypes predetermined by society, as noted by Kiselica and Morrill-Richards (2007) and Seixas (2009), are relevant here. Males are expected to be aggressive, while females are expected to show subtlety, fragility, and greater concern for others, as found by several authors (Kiselica and Morrill-Richards 2007; Kindlon and Thompson 2000; Simões et al. 2015). According to Rubia (2007), these characteristics present in females also seem to be due to their innate capacity for greater and faster aptitude in the development of verbal skills. Thus, according to recent empirical conceptions, males mostly assume the role of aggressor in sibling relationships (Dantchev and Wolke 2019; Kiselica and Morrill-Richards 2007; Relva et al. 2014; Relva et al. 2013). There is no consensus regarding the sex that faces the most victimization; however, females are the most affected in terms of experiencing psychological aggression (Khan and Rogers 2015; Relva et al. 2013; Simonelli et al. 2002). Other factors can contribute to this. A recent study conducted by Walters et al. (2020) intends to explore a mediational model of the effects and consequences of sibling victimization in a sample of 355 adolescents (165 females) with ages ranging between 10 and 15 years. According to the authors, the results indicated a negative association between parental monitoring and sibling victimization in girls. Regarding males, they predominantly perpetrate acts of aggression that result in serious physical harm (Relva et al. 2013; Relva et al. 2014).
Regarding social class, Hoffman and Edwards (2004) recognize this analysis as a gap in the literature because the study samples mostly include elements of the average socioeconomic level. However, Green (1984) found an effect of financial resources on the severity of the aggressions perpetrated and victimized by siblings and this severity increased when economic conditions worsened. Also, Tippett and Wolke (2015), in a sample of 4237 participants (aged between 10 and 15), intended to identify factors associated with sibling violence, namely the socioeconomic background. The authors found that sibling victimization was related to families who experienced poverty or financial difficulties. However, the results regarding social class are inconsistent. In a large sample of 6838 children from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, a prospective United Kingdom birth-cohort, Dantchev and Wolke (2019) found that social class was not associated with sibling bullying, suggesting that “social conditions matter less or not at all” (p. 1068).
Menesini et al. (2010) state that since the early 1980s, special importance has been attached to the relationship between siblings in their healthy development. Despite growing up and developing in the same family context, siblings may experience different practices, as well as experience differentiated treatment by the primary attachment figures (Dunn and Plomin 1990).
Several studies focus on the position occupied by siblings in their sibling dyad (e.g., Bowes et al. 2014; Menesini et al. 2010). Menesini et al. (2010), by aiming to examine individual and relational factors in the function of violence between siblings, as well as its relationship with bullying in the school context, with a sample of 195 subjects aged between 10 and 12 years, found that victimization occurs more frequently in individuals with an older sibling. In addition, the authors also found that older and male siblings engage more in sibling violent behavior when the youngest sibling exists. They also found that females give more importance to the quality of the relationship established between siblings rather than conceiving importance to their birth order. Finkelhor et al. (2006) point in the same direction in a sample of 2030 children and adolescents aged between two and 17 years of age, as they found that compared to the rest, the older siblings attack the most. According to Fernandes (2000, 2005) and Fernandes et al. (2007), there are characteristics common to siblings who occupy the same fraternal position. In this way, the older siblings are seen as the leaders and those to whom the beliefs and values of the parents are bequeathed are seen as the most responsible. The younger brothers are free from any burden, they are considered the most fragile and, therefore, the most protected. The middle brothers are characterized by the poorly defined role they experience in their siblinghood, incessantly searching for the position they should occupy, which gives them particularities such as low self-esteem and aggressiveness, revealing themselves to be quite centered in themselves, evidencing a scant concern for others.
Regarding the association between sibling relationships and self-esteem, sibling relationships as a safe haven can be associated with self-esteem in adolescents (Mota and Matos 2015; Mota et al. 2017). Self-esteem is the subject’s appreciation of him/herself, considering his/her attributes and virtues (Serra 1988). In fact, the positive sibling relationship is associated with lower levels of internalizing symptoms in adolescents during stressful events (Waite et al. 2011). However, maintaining the relationship between siblings is not always positive. Relationships between siblings can be damaging when primary care or the lack of it have not made it possible to organize young people’s emotional experiences within a framework of emotional stability (Mota 2021). In these cases, sibling separation can be beneficial, protecting them from excessive rivalry, blame, abuse and/or violence in the relationship (Mota 2021). According to the literature, self-esteem exerts an effect on the involvement in aggressive behaviors between siblings since siblings involved in this dynamic evidence the presence of low levels of self-esteem (Avanci et al. 2007; Wiehe 1997). However, the results are not consistent. Laopratai et al. (2023), in a study that aims to examine sibling bullying and its association with self-esteem and depression during the pandemic in a sample of 352 participants (30.4% female) where 92 (26.1%) were victims and 49 (13.9%) were bullies of sibling bullying, did not observe an association between sibling bullying and low self-esteem. Possibly other variables may be associated, such as the context or individual aspects of young people.
According to Rosenberg et al. (1995), global self-esteem is related to psychological well-being, while specific self-esteem is related to the behavioral area. This behavioral scope is associated with the subject’s feelings in relation to a specific characteristic of the self, such as academic or sexual self-esteem. In this sense, Gentile et al. (2009), in a meta-analysis in which they wanted to verify the differences by sex according to 10 specific domains of self-esteem using 115 studies, were able to verify that when assessing overall self-esteem, there are fewer significant differences found about sex, which does not occur for specific self-esteem. However, this conception is not consensual since there is literature that attributes males to higher levels of self-esteem (Quatman and Watson 2001). Also, Ruiz et al. (2009), when analyzing the relationship between social reputation, bullying, psychosocial adjustment, loneliness, self-esteem and satisfaction with life in 1319 adolescents aged between 11 and 16 years, found that it was not possible to ascertain significant differences in variable self-esteem between females and males. Although this conclusion regarding family socioeconomic differences is not straightforward, it would also be expected that young people with higher socio-economic levels would have access to greater support and consistent care, promoting greater self-esteem. Espínola (2010) supports this idea as, in a sample of 593 students aged between 9 and 13 years, they found that adolescents belonging to the middle social class show higher rates of self-esteem when compared to adolescents belonging to a lower social class. However, we know that many other personal and particularly relational factors are inherent in a socio-economic status and its relationship with young people’s self-esteem, so we will try to fill in some of the gaps in the literature.

1.2. Objectives and Hypotheses

The main objective of this study is to analyze the role of self-esteem, position in the sibling dyad and sex in the development of sibling violence behaviors in adolescents (both from a victimization and perpetration perspective) (see Figure 1). First, we intend to analyze the associations between self-esteem and sibling violence. In this follow-up, the differences in self-esteem and violence between siblings will be analyzed according to such sociodemographic variables as sex, social class and position in the sibling dyad.
According to the objectives, self-esteem is expected to correlate positively with the negotiation dimension and negatively with psychological aggression, physical aggression, sexual coercion and injury dimensions, also related to violence between siblings. It is also expected that self-esteem and violence between siblings present statistically significant differences in relation to sex, social class and position in the sibling dyad. Regarding self-esteem, it is expected that males present higher rates of it, as well as it is expected that respondents of a low social class have lower self-esteem. Regarding position in the sibling dyad, the middle sibling is expected to be the holder of a less positive appreciation of himself. Thus, it is expected that there is a greater involvement of females in negotiation behaviors, as well as in indirect aggressions. As for social class, a greater involvement in aggressive conduct between siblings by elements of lower social classes is expected. Regarding the position occupied in the sibling dyad, due to his low self-esteem and characteristic aggressiveness, the greater involvement of the middle brother in violent acts is observed. Finally, it is also expected that the negotiation dimension will be positively predicted by self-esteem, just as a negative prediction of self-esteem is expected in the face of the effectiveness of aggressive behaviors between siblings. Regarding the position in the sibling dyad, the greater involvement of the middle sibling in aggressive behaviors that aim at the resolution of conflicts between siblings, as well as the greater involvement of females in the negotiation technique and males in the effectiveness of aggressive behaviors of the direct type are observed.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sample

In the present study, 286 adolescents aged between 12 and 17 years participated (M = 13.55; SD = 1.12), of which 115 (40.2%) are male and 171 (59.8%) females. Regarding schooling (M = 8.11; SD = 0.82), 83 participants (29%) attend the 7th grade, 89 (31.1%) attend the 8th grade and 114 (39.9%) the 9th grade. This sample includes 163 (57%) individuals who occupy the position of youngest sibling in the sibling dyad, 35 (12.2%) are middle siblings and 88 (30.8%) are older siblings.

2.2. Instruments

Sociodemographic questionnaire—questionnaire designed for the appropriate purposes, which included variables considered pertinent to the investigation. It allowed access to information regarding the student (e.g., age, schooling), his/her parents (e.g., age, education, socioeconomic level, marital status) and his/her sibling dyad (e.g., number of siblings, age).
The Revised Conflict Tactics Scales (sibling Portuguese version)—CTS2-SP (Straus et al. 1996; adapted by Relva et al. 2013). This self-report questionnaire is addressed to subjects with one or more siblings, questioning them about potential aggressive behaviors experienced within their siblings. This scale consists of 76 items, subdivided equally by two subscales: victimization and perpetration. Each of these subscales consists of five dimensions: negotiation with 6 items (e.g., “I showed that I cared about this brother/sister, even if we disagreed”), psychological aggression with 7 items (e.g., “I insulted or swore at this brother/sister”), physical aggression with 12 items (e.g., “I threw something at this brother/sister that could hurt him”), sexual coercion with 7 items (e.g., “I made this brother/sister have sex without a condom”) and injury with 6 items (e.g., “This brother/sister had a fracture due to a fight with me”). These items appear paired, that is, first questioning whether the individual was the author of a given behavior and later questioning whether he was a victim of the same type of behavior. The response options of this scale vary between 1 (once in the last year), 2 (twice in the last year), 3 (three to five times in the last year), 4 (six to ten times in the last year), 5 (eleven to twenty times in the last year), 6 (more than twenty times in the last year), 7 (not in that year, but it happened before) and 8 (it never happened) and it is, therefore, an 8-point Likert scale, corresponding to the frequency with which the behavior happened in a given period of time. It should be noted that for the present investigation, it was decided to recode points 7 and 8 in 0 because they were not related to the last year, which was not relevant to the current investigation. The internal consistency analysis for the present research showed Cronbach’s alpha values of 0.78 for perpetration and 0.77 for victimization. Regarding the constituent dimensions of the scale of perpetration and victimization, Cronbach’s alpha values of 0.79/0.84 for negotiation, 0.87/0.79 for psychological aggression, 0.84/0.88 for physical aggression, 0.67/0.58 for sexual coercion and 0.55/0.50 for injury were met, respectively. Through the confirmatory factor analyses, it was verified that the questionnaire, for the perpetration and victimization scales presents adequate adjustment indexes: Perpetration—SRMR = 0.07, CFI = 0.92, RMSEA = 0.09, χ2 (75) = 246.65, p < 0.001, χ2/gl = 3.29—and victimization—SRMR = 0.06, CFI = 0.93, RMSEA = 0.08, χ2 (75) = 227.60, p < 0.001, χ2/gl = 3.03. According to the authors, the Cronbach’s alphas ranged, for the scale of perpetration/victimization, between 0.65 and 0.80/0.66 and 0.84, respectively (negotiation = 0.79/0.77, psychological aggression = 0.76/0.75, physical assault = 0.80/0.80, sexual coercion = 0.77/0.84 and injury = 0.65/0.66) (Relva et al. 2013).
Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale—RSE (Rosenberg 1965; adapted by Rocha and Matos 2003, cited in Rocha 2008). This questionnaire, characterized by being a self-report instrument, aims to assess global personal self-esteem. Consisting of 10 items, five are positively worded (e.g., “I feel I have some good qualities”) and the rest are negatively worded (e.g., “Sometimes I feel like I don’t pay attention”), and the latter are quoted in reverse (items 3, 5, 8, 9 and 10). Originally, the response scale is of the six-point Likert type (from strongly agree to strongly disagree). In the present study, it was adapted aiming at standardization and simplifying the entire protocol’s response. This thus appears in four points, namely: 1 (Strongly disagree), 2 (Disagree), 3 (Agree) and 4 (Strongly agree). The internal consistency analysis for the present research showed Cronbach’s alpha values of 0.84, which is like the one found by the author (0.85) of which the adaptation was used (Rocha 2008). Through the confirmatory factor analysis, it was verified that the questionnaire presents adequate adjustment indexes: SRMR = 0.05, CFI = 0.98, RMSEA = 0.06, χ2 (32) = 66.104, p < 0.001, χ2/gl = 2.07.

2.3. Procedures

The procedures in the research followed the General Regulation on Data Protection of the European Union and the Code of Ethics and Deontology of the Portuguese Psychologists Association for Research.
The present study sample was collected from middle schools in the northern zone of Portugal. The requests for authorization were sent to these establishments through letter and e-mail. When obtaining these authorizations, meetings were scheduled with the school directors to present the protocol and clarify possible doubts. It requested the informed consent of parents. The application was carried out with the collaboration of educational institutions and the standard instructions for applying the research protocol were given to the class teacher. Data collection occurred in the classroom in the presence of the supervising researcher. Students were informed that information was confidential and their participation in the study was voluntary.

2.4. Data Analysis Strategies

The present research assumes a cross-sectional character since all the data were collected only in a single moment, so there was no follow-up of the sample subjects during a time interval. Data processing was performed using SPSS (Statistical Package for Social Sciences) version 28 for Windows. This process began with a database cleanup intended to identify and exclude missing data and possible outliers. The Mahalanobis distance analysis was used to identify multivariate outliers, since this allows the use of the means and variances of the variables, through which it is possible to identify subjects who present inadequate values (values that deviate significantly from the mean) (Field 2005).
Subsequently, normality was tested by analyzing the values of skewness and kurtosis, complementing this process with several statistical analyses that aim to inform about the distribution of the data: Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, histogram graphs, Q-QPlots, Scatterplots and Boxplots (Marôco 2007; Pallant 2020). The sample admitted all the assumptions of normality (−1 and 1). We performed 1st order Confirmatory Analyses of the instruments. Finally, correlation analyses (Pearson’s correlations), differential analyses (student t-test, ANOVA and MANOVA) and prediction analyses (hierarchical multiple regression) were performed. The correlational analyses considered reference values stipulated by Cohen (1988), indicating correlation magnitudes between 0.10 and 0.29 as low, from 0.30 to 0.49 as moderate and values above 0.50 as corresponding to strong associations.
The same author’s reference values for analyses of variance were also taken into account, considering the size of the magnitude of the effect through eta squared with values: 0.01 small effect, 0.06 moderate effect and 0.14 large effect (Cohen 1988).

3. Results

Correlational analyses were used to analyze the associations between self-esteem and sibling violence (perpetration and victimization). The results of the inter-scale correlations and the respective means and standard deviations are presented in Table 1.
Regarding the association between self-esteem and the perpetration of sibling violence, there is a positive and significant correlation of a low magnitude for negotiation (r = 0.21, p < 0.001) and negative and significant correlations of a low magnitude for psychological aggression (r = −0.19, p < 0.001), for physical assault (r = −0.18, p < 0.001), for sexual coercion (r = −0.15, p < 0.05) and for injury (r = −0.18, p < 0.001).
Regarding the association between self-esteem and the victimization of sibling violence, there is a positive and significant correlation of a low magnitude of negotiation (r = 0.24, p < 0.001), with psychological aggression (r = −0.19, p < 0.001), physical assault (r = −0.18, p < 0.001), and injury (r = −0.21, p < 0.001) which is correlated negatively and significantly with a low magnitude. Sexual coercion is the only dimension that is not statistically significantly correlated.

3.1. Variance of Self-Esteem and Sibling Violence According to Sex, Social Class and Position in the Sibling Dyad

Aiming at analyzing self-esteem and sibling violence as a function of sociodemographic variables (sex, social class and position in the sibling dyad), we proceeded with univariate and multivariate differential analyses of variance (ANOVA and MANOVA), as well a Student’s t-test.
To explore differences for the variable self-esteem as a function of sex, a student’s t-test was used and the results suggest the absence of significant differences t(284) = 0.90; p = 0.37; IC 95% [−0.08; 0.20] ƞ2 = 0.04.
Regarding the perpetration of sibling violence, significant differences are found in relation to sex for the dimensions of negotiation t(284) = −2.05; p = 0.04; IC 95% [−0.81;−0.02] ƞ2 = 0.20, psychological aggression t(284) = −2.02; p = 0.05; IC 95% [−0.57;−0.01] ƞ2 = 0.30 and sexual coercion t(152.52) = 2.58; p = 0.01; IC 95% [0.02;0.18] ƞ2 = 0.50. Thus, it is verified that females use more negotiation (M = 3.20, SD = 1.59), but also psychological aggression (M = 1.21, SD = 1.25), while males more often perform acts of sexual coercion (M = 0.14, SD = 0.39) (Table 2). Finally, physical assault t(284) = −0.22; p = 0.83; IC 95% [−0.17;0.14] ƞ2 = 0.05 and injury t(214.59) = 1.31; p = 0.19; IC 95% [−0.03;0.16] ƞ2 = 0.02 do not present significant differences regarding sex (Table 2).
Regarding the victimization of sibling violence according to sex, there are significant differences for psychological aggression t(269.90) = −2.21; p = 0.03; IC 95% [−0.58;−0.03] ƞ2 = 0.40 and injury t(182.86) = 2.05; p = 0.04; IC 95% [0.00;0.22] ƞ2 = 0.60. Thus, it is concluded that females are the ones who suffer more psychological aggression (M = 1.14, SD = 1.26), while males are the ones who suffer the most acts of injury (M = 0.24, SD = 0.52) (Table 2). The dimensions of negotiation t(284) = −1.54; p = 0.13; IC 95% [−0.70;0.09] ƞ2 = 0.02, physical assault t(284) = 0.52; p = 0.60; IC 95% [−0.12;0.21] ƞ2 = 0.07 and sexual coercion t(284) = 0.74; p = 0.46; IC 95% [−0.04;0.22] ƞ2 = 0.07 do not demonstrate statistically significant differences regarding sex (Table 2).
Social class was classified according to the average monthly income of the parents. This categorization was made in three groups: low (up to and including the minimum wage), medium (from the minimum wage to 1000 euros) and high (more than 1000 euros per month). This division was based on the five-level classification on the Graffar household income scale. In the present study, these five levels were adapted into only three (low, medium and high) to simplify and be better understood by the respondents.
A univariate differential analysis (ANOVA) was carried out to investigate self-esteem as a function of social class. The results suggest that there are no significant differences: F(2, 221) = 2.13, p = 0.12.
Regarding sibling violence in relation to social class, there are no significant differences in both the perpetration F(10, 436) = 0.99, p = 0.45, ƞ2 = 0.53 or victimization subscales F(10, 436) = 0.63, p = 0.79, ƞ2 = 0.33.
By analyzing self-esteem as a function of position in the sibling dyad, it is evident that there are no significant differences F(2, 283) = 0.29, p = 0.75.
Regarding the subscale perpetration of sibling violence, no statistically significant differences were found F(10, 560) = 1.36, p = 0.20, ƞ2 = 0.70 faced with the position in the sibling dyad.
Regarding the subscale victimization of sibling violence F(10, 560) = 2.71, p = 0.003, ƞ2 = 0.97, there are significant differences in the position in the sibling dyad only for the dimension of physical assault F(2, 283) = 5.42, p = 0.005, ƞ2 = 0.84. Middle siblings (M = 0.72, SD = 0.98) experience more physical assault (victimization subscale) when compared to youngest sibling (M = 0.40, SD = 0.70) and older sibling (M = 0.28, SD = 0.45) (Table 3).

3.2. Predictive Role of Sex, Position in Sibling Dyad and Self-Esteem in Sibling Violence

Hierarchical multiple regression analyses were performed to ensure the response to the present study’s objectives.
In the performance of each hierarchical multiple regression, the dimensions of violence between siblings were analyzed separately. All analyses introduced the following three blocks: sex, position in the sibling dyad and self-esteem. Note that the variables sex and position in the sibling dyad were dummy coded to ensure the analysis of sex (0—male, 1—female) and position in the sibling dyad (1—Youngest sibling; 2—Middle sibling; 3—Eldest sibling).
In the hierarchical multiple regression analysis regarding the negotiation (perpetration), sex makes a significant contribution, F(1, 284) = 4.21, p = 0.041, explains 2% of the total variance (R2 = 0.02) and contributes individually to 1% of the variance for the model (R2change = 0.01). The position in the sibling dyad makes a significant contribution, F(3, 282) = 3.02, p = 0.03, explains 3% of variance (R2 = 0.03) and contributes individually to 2% of the variance for the model (R2change = 0.02). Self-esteem makes a significant contribution, F(4, 281) = 5.96, p = 0.000, explains 8% of variance (R2 = 0.08) and contributes individually to 7% of the variance for the model (R2change = 0.07).
By analyzing the individual contribution of each independent variable, it is observed that three have a significant contribution (p < 0.05) as an effect of negotiation (perpetration), presenting itself according to its importance: self-esteem (β = 0.22), position in sibling dyad—youngest (β = −0.20)—and females (β = 0.13) (Table 4).
In the hierarchical multiple regression analysis regarding psychological aggression (perpetration), sex makes a significant contribution, F(1, 284) = 4.06, p = 0.045, explains 1% of variance (R2 = 0.01) and contributes individually to 1% of the variance for the model (R2change = 0.01). The position in the sibling dyad makes a significant contribution, F(3, 282) = 3.13, p = 0.026, explains 3% of variance (R2 = 0.03) and individually contributes 2% to the model (R2change = 0.02). No que concerns self-esteem as it makes a significant contribution F(4, 281) = 4.76, p = 0.001, explains 6% of variance (R2 = 0.06) and individually contributes 5% to the model (R2change = 0.05).
Analyzing the individual contribution of each independent variable, it is observed that two make a significant contribution (p < 0.05) as an effect of psychological aggression (perpetration), presenting itself according to its importance: position in the sibling dyad—older sibling (β = −0.20)—and self-esteem (β = −0.18) (Table 4).
In the hierarchical multiple regression analysis regarding physical assault (perpetration), sex does not make a significant contribution, F(1, 284) = 0.05, p = 0.827, does not explain the variance (R2 = 0.00) and does not present an individual contribution to the variance of the model (R2change = 0.00). The position in the sibling dyad does not make a significant contribution, F(3, 282) = 0.89, p = 0.448, explains 1% of variance (R2 = 0.01) and does not contribute individually to the template (R2change = 0.00). Self-esteem makes a significant contribution, F(4, 281) = 3.04, p = 0.018, explains 4% of variance (R2 = 0.04) and contributes individually to 3% to the model (R2change = 0.03).
By analyzing the individual contribution of the independent variable, it is observed that self-esteem (β = −0.18) has a significant contribution (p < 0.05) associated with physical assault (perpetration) (Table 4).
In the hierarchical multiple regression analysis regarding sexual coercion (perpetration) sex makes a significant contribution, F(1, 284) = 8.46, p = 0.004, explains 3% of variance (R2 = 0.03) and contributes individually to 3% of the variance for the model (R2change = 0.03). The position in the sibling dyad makes a significant contribution F(3, 282) = 3.12, p = 0.027, explains 3% of variance (R2 = 0.03) and contributes individually to 2% of the model (R2change = 0.02). Self-esteem also makes a significant contribution, F(4, 281) = 4.12, p = 0.003, explains 6% of variance (R2 = 0.06) and contributes individually to 4% of the model (R2change = 0.04).
By analyzing the individual contribution of each independent variable, it is observed that two make a significant contribution (p < 0.05) as the effect of sexual coercion (perpetration), males (β = −0.17) and self-esteem (β = −0.15) (Table 4).
In the hierarchical multiple regression analysis regarding injury (perpetration), sex does not make a significant contribution, F(1, 284) = 1.085, p = 0.175, explains 1% of the variance (R2 = 0.01) and does not present an individual contribution to the variance of the model (R2change = 0.00). The position in the sibling dyad does not make a significant contribution, F(3, 282) = 1.37, p = 0.253, explains 1% of variance (R2 = 0.01) and does not contribute individually to the template (R2change = 0.00). Regarding self-esteem, it makes a significant contribution, F(4, 281) = 3.46, p = 0.009, explains 5% of variance (R2 = 0.05) and contributes, individually, 3% to the model (R2change = 0.03).
By analyzing the individual contribution of each independent variable, it is observed that only self-esteem (β = −0.18) makes a significant contribution (p < 0.05) as the effect of injury (perpetration) (Table 4).
In the hierarchical multiple regression analysis regarding negotiation (victimization) sex (dummy) does not make a significant contribution, F(1, 284) = 2.37, p = 0.125, explains 1% of the total variance (R2 = 0.01) and contributes individually to 1% of the variance for the model (R2change = 0.01). Regarding the position in the sibling dyad (dummy), it does not make a significant contribution, F(3, 282) = 1.69, p = 0.170, explains 1% of variance (R2 = 0.01) and contributes individually to 1% of the variance for the model (R2change = 0.01). With regard to self-esteem, it makes a significant contribution, F(4, 281) = 5.75, p = 0.000, explains 6% of variance (R2 = 0.06) and contributes individually to 6% of the variance for the model (R2change = 0.06).
By analyzing the individual contribution of each independent variable, it is observed that self-esteem (β = 0.24) makes a significant contribution (p < 0.05) as the effect of negotiation (Table 5).
In the hierarchical multiple regression analysis of psychological aggression (victimization), sex makes a significant contribution, F(1, 284) = 4.57, p = 0.033, explains 2% of the variance (R2 = 0.02) and contributes individually to 1% of the variance for the model (R2change = 0.01). The position in the sibling dyad makes a significant contribution, F(3, 282) = 2.98, p = 0.0232, explains 3% of the variance (R2 = 0.03) and individually contributes 2% to the model (R2change = 0.02). Regarding self-esteem, it makes a significant contribution, F(4, 281) = 4.57, p = 0.001, explains 6% of the variance (R2 = 0.06) and contributes individually to 5% of the model (R2change = 0.05).
By analyzing the individual contribution of each independent variable, it is observed that two make a significant contribution (p < 0.05) as the effect of psychological aggression (victimization), with both presenting the same importance: position in the sibling dyad—eldest sibling (β = −0.18)—and self-esteem (β = −0.18) (Table 5).
In the hierarchical multiple regression analysis of physical assault (victimization), sex does not make a significant contribution, F(1, 284) = 0.27, p = 0.602, does not explain the variance (R2 = 0.00) and does not present an individual contribution to the variance of the model (R2change = 0.00). Regarding the position in the sibling dyad, it makes a significant contribution, F(3, 282) = 3.73, p = 0.012, explains 4% of variance (R2 = 0.04) and contributes individually to 3% of the model (R2change = 0.03). Regarding self-esteem, it makes a significant contribution, F(4, 281) = 5.08, p = 0.001, explains 7% of variance (R2 = 0.07) and contributes individually to 5% of the model (R2change = 0.05).
By analyzing the individual contribution of each independent variable, it is observed that three make a significant contribution (p < 0.05) as the effect of physical assault (victimization), presenting themselves according to their importance: position in the sibling dyad—youngest sibling (β = −0.29)—position in the sibling dyad—Eldest sibling (β = −0.24)—and self-esteem (β = −0.17) (Table 5).
In the hierarchical multiple regression analysis regarding sexual coercion (victimization), sex does not make a significant contribution, F(1, 284) = 0.55, p = 0.459, does not explain the variance (R2 = 0.00) and does not individually contribute variance to the model (R2change = 0.003). With regard to the position in the sibling dyad, it does not make a significant contribution, F(3, 282) = 0.37, p = 0.772, does not explain the variance (R2 = 0.00) and contributes individually to −1% of the model (R2change = −0.01). Regarding self-esteem, it makes no significant contribution, F(4, 281) = 0.31, p = 0.871, does not explain the variance (R2 = 0.00) and contributes individually to −1% of the model (R2change = −0.01).
By analyzing the individual contribution of each independent variable, it is observed that none make a significant contribution (p < 0.05) as an effect of sexual coercion (victimization) (Table 5).
In the hierarchical multiple regression analysis regarding injury (victimization), sex makes a significant contribution, F(1, 284) = 4.85, p = 0.028, explains 2% of variance (R2 = 0.02) and contributes individually to 1% for model variance (R2change = 0.01). Regarding the position in the sibling dyad, it does not make a significant contribution, F(3, 282) = 2.22, p = 0.086, explains 2% of variance (R2 = 0.02) and individually contributes 1% to the model (R2change = 0.01). Regarding self-esteem, it makes a significant contribution F(4, 281) = 5.08, p = 0.001, explains 7% of variance (R2 = 0.07) and individually contributes 5% to the model (R2change = 0.05).
Analyzing the individual contribution of each independent variable, it is observed that two have a significant contribution (p < 0.05) as the effect of injury (victimization), presenting itself according to its importance: self-esteem (β = −0.21) and males (β = −0.14) (Table 5).

4. Discussion

The main objective of the present study was to test the effect of sex, position in the sibling dyad and self-esteem in the development of behaviors of violence between siblings. The results suggest that self-esteem is positively associated with negotiation on the perpetration and victimization of sibling violence. On the contrary, it is negatively associated with psychological aggression, physical assault and injury, also concerning the perpetration and victimization of sibling violence. Only about sexual coercion is self-esteem negatively associated with perpetration and there is no significant relationship with victimization. Thus, self-esteem seems to play an important role in the dynamics of violence between siblings. The presence of a positive perception of oneself (Serra 1988) seems to favor the establishment of dialogues between brothers and, consequently, to benefit the capacity to resolve conflicts that may arise between them. Self-esteem it also seems to be associated with aggressive conduct between siblings playing a protective role. When there is high self-esteem, there is a lower frequency of aggressive acts perpetrated and suffered by siblings. Knowing that self-esteem consists in the appreciation that the subject makes of himself, contemplating his attributes and qualities, having himself as capable or incapable of successfully executing what he proposes (Serra 1988), an individual with high self-esteem, demonstrating high levels of happiness and satisfaction with himself, will not have the need to develop any rivalry, which assumes a violent character, towards any of his siblings (Mota et al. 2017). The results of the present study are in accordance with previous studies. Yeh and Lempers (2004), in a sample of 374 families, found that a healthy relationship between siblings was predictive of high self-esteem because, by relating positively and adjusted, they developed positive feelings and perceptions about themselves. Also, in their research, Avanci et al. (2007) and Wiehe (1997) realized that low self-esteem characterizes siblings involved in violent behavior. Avanci et al. (2007), in a sample of 266 students aged between 11 and 19 years, also found that adolescents with high self-esteem engage less in victimizing behaviors in sibling violence. Wiehe (1997), in addition to verifying this assumption, also found that brother aggressors in their dyad have low levels of self-esteem.
Regarding self-esteem, there were no significant differences according to sex. It is suggested that the results obtained in the present research are attributed to the fact of evaluating global self-esteem and not considering the various specificities of it (e.g., sexual, physical and academic) because an adolescent, in general, may not evaluate himself positively; however, he can be considered effective in some specificities (e.g., academic self-esteem). The study of Gentile et al. (2009) points in the same direction. The authors consider that to obtain significant results in this analysis and properly examining self-esteem, the specific self-esteem should always be considered. Although the results of the present investigation do not generate consensus in the literature (e.g., Quatman and Watson 2001; Ruiz et al. 2009), they are corroborated by Ruiz et al. (2009) in a sample of 1319 adolescents aged between 11 and 16 years and they, in their study, also verified the absence of significant differences in self-esteem in relation to sex.
Regarding negotiation, psychological aggression and sexual coercion in the perpetration of violence between siblings, there were significant differences according to sex. Female subjects showed a greater predisposition to establish conversations with their siblings and the elements most resort to psychological aggressions. Therefore, males are the one who most use sexual assaults. In view of these results, it was expected that the female was more involved in negotiation behaviors. This can be explained based on empirical conceptions about sex differences. According to Rubia (2007), females, when compared to males, seem to have a greater capacity in relation to the performance of verbal skills. In addition, it is also known that the development of these same communicative skills progresses later in males, unlike females, who previously acquire a greater maturation of social skills (Legato 2009). In the follow-up, sex stereotypes created by society also seem to influence the greater involvement of females in negotiation behaviors. From its birth, the baby is exposed to certain behaviors taken to the detriment of its sex, according to Seixas (2009). From an early age, parents show different behaviors towards babies based on their sex. When faced with a female baby, parents tend to show more concern for others and engage in longer conversations than a male baby. This behavior is believed to contribute to aggressive behavior in boys (Kindlon and Thompson 2000). Females tend to adopt psychological aggression behaviors more often than males, suggesting that societal sex stereotypes play a significant role. While males are expected to display harsh and aggressive behavior, the same is not expected from females.
Females are expected to display indirect behavior characterized by subtlety (Simões et al. 2015). Therefore, knowing that psychological aggression is an indirect form of being aggressive toward a sibling, it was expected that individuals identifying as female would choose this type of aggression. However, the current results contradict the existing literature on the subject, which assigns the role of aggressor in violence between siblings to males (Kiselica and Morrill-Richards 2007). Kiselica and Morrill-Richards (2007) justify that such results are due to sex stereotypes, in which aggressiveness is once again associated with males and subtlety with females. Thus, the father figures infer that the damage caused by males is the most serious. This is because they see it according to sex stereotypes, in which males are the most physically aggressive and these physical damages are visible. As for females, they infer that they do not have sufficient strength to cause serious harm to their siblings. According to Kiselica and Morrill-Richards (2007), it is only by changing attitudes and accepting that it is that females perpetrate aggression against a sibling that this type of family violence can be tackled. Additionally, sex stereotypes already mentioned in this discussion also justify the greater involvement of males as an aggressor in sexual coercion. However, attention should also be paid to the inequality of power mentioned in the definition of sibling violence. It is understood that this inequality of power can be related to several characteristics of the actors; among them age, height, physical strength and superiority are specific to the aggressor (Monks et al. 2009). In this sense, physical superiority is seen as belonging to males when compared to females, which corresponds to sex stereotypes (Kiselica and Morrill-Richards 2007; Simões et al. 2015). It was expected that sexual assaults would be perpetrated primarily by males. The present results corroborate the existing literature on the subject, through which it can be observed that males more frequently maintain sexual acts (with siblings) without consent (e.g., Relva et al. 2013; Relva et al. 2014).
Regarding psychological aggression and injury in the victimization of sibling violence in relation to sex, there were significant differences, with females showing a greater propensity to victimize psychological aggressions. In contrast, the males showed greater victimization of aggressive behaviors causing serious physical harm. Regarding the victimization of psychological aggression, experienced mostly by females, it is suggested that this is justified based on the conception that there is reciprocity in sibling violence (Krienert and Walsh 2011). In the present research, females are the ones that most perpetrate violence of the psychological type because, considering males are seen often with a greater physical robustness (Wiehe 1997), females are expected to opt for psychological violence. In addition, as already mentioned, according to Krienert and Walsh (2011), there is a reciprocity in sibling violence; in a way, they are both victims and perpetrators of aggression. Thus, and verified, this duality would be expected to be attributed to the same sex the same type of violence suffered and perpetrated. The present results are corroborated by the literature (e.g., Khan and Rogers 2015; Relva et al. 2013; Simonelli et al. 2002) when other researchers verified a predominance of females being victims of psychological aggression. The results of being a victim of injury (physical aggression with severe physical damage) seem to involve males exercising them, mostly in aggressive behaviors towards those of the same sex (Relva et al. 2014). Also, the existing literature on the subject points results in the same outcome. Several studies show that male siblings are more victimized by assaults with serious physical harm (Relva et al. 2013; Relva et al. 2014).
In view of the results obtained in the present study, it was also found that there were no significant differences in self-esteem as a function of social class. According to the existing literature on the subject, such results were unexpected. Espínola (2010), in a sample of 593 students aged between 9 and 13 years, found that compared to individuals belonging to the low social class, subjects who were included in the average socioeconomic level have higher rates of self-esteem. However, these results are due to the particularities of the sample of the present investigation. Notably, 44.4% of the respondents belong to the middle social class, making it impossible to have a proportional distribution across all levels (low, medium and high) and an exact theme analysis.
The results also point to the absence of significant differences in victimization and the perpetration of sibling violence regarding the parents’ social class. As mentioned, social class does not seem to predict sibling bullying (Dantchev and Wolke 2019). Thus, it is suggested that the results are related to the size of the sample and its distribution relative to the socioeconomic level of the parents. In the present study, 44.4% of the adolescents in the sample were in the middle class, 20.3% in the high social class and the remainder in the so-called low social class. It should be noted that a larger sample that contemplates, in equal ways, different social classes should obtain a better analysis of this theme. Hoffman and Edwards (2004) recognize the gap in the literature on the subject. According to the authors, the studies that contemplate the parents’ social class in their analyses are scarce because, as in the present investigation, they present a limited sample and mostly belong to the middle social class, thus opting for the analysis of variables such as sex and age. However, Green (1984), aiming to detect and analyze the characteristics of children, as well as their parents, who assaulted siblings, causing them serious damage, in a sample of five children and adolescents, could infer that the severity of injuries increased when there were fewer financial resources. Indeed, in 2015, Tippet and Wolke found that greater rates of sibling aggression were associated with financial difficulties. In this follow-up, it is also suggested that because the sample of this study includes few elements of low social class, more obstacles are denoted to the existence of significant differences.
Regarding self-esteem in relation to the position in the sibling dyad, there were no significant differences. It is suggested that such conceptions may be explained based on the present study’s sample. It should be noted that the distribution of adolescents by the position occupied in their phratry is quite divergent since 57% of the respondents are characterized by being older siblings, only 12.2% are from the middle and the rest are the youngest. According to the literature, it would be expected that there will be differences in self-esteem inherent to the position occupied in the phratry. According to Fernandes (2005), the middle sibling is the one who presents lower self-esteem due to the poorly defined role occupied in his sibling dyad and consequent feelings of inferiority in relation to his siblings.
There were no significant differences in the perpetration of sibling violence in the face of their position in the sibling dyad. It is suggested that the achievement of such results is due to the non-association of the position occupied by the aggressor sibling with the position occupied by the victim of these same aggressions. The existing literature (Menesini et al. 2010) attaches importance to this analysis, verifying the presence of significant differences in sibling violence due to the position occupied by the sibling in the dyad. However, these analyses contemplate the position occupied by the two actors (aggressor and victim) in this dynamic, which does not occur in the present investigation. Another explanation is given for the lack of significant differences in the present analysis. Menesini et al. (2010) suggested that females, instead of highlighting importance to the position occupied by each of the siblings in their dyad, attach greater importance to the quality of the relationship established between them. Knowing that the sample of the present study consists mostly of female elements (59.8%), the position in the sibling dyad is expected to not acquire a prominent position. However, the existing literature does not corroborate the conceptions evidenced in the present study (Finkelhor et al. 2006). Thus, Finkelhor et al. (2006), in a sample of 2030 children and adolescents aged between 2 and 17 years of age, could verify that older siblings attack more frequently when compared to the others. The same was found by Tippett and Wolke (2015), where they found an association between the perpetration of sibling aggression and being the eldest child.
As for the victimization of sibling violence, there were significant differences regarding physical assault compared to the position in the sibling dyad. The middle sibling, as victims, were more often involved in acts of physical assault when compared to the youngest or the oldest. It should be noted that the middle sibling is, according to the literature, the only one who does not have his role well defined in his phratry because the eldest is considered as the successor of the parents, while the youngest is the protected son, the one who is free from any responsibility (Fernandes 2005; Fernandes et al. 2007). It is thus suggested that, due to this condition, the middle sibling feels that his interaction with his parents is insufficient, considering that they do not give him the necessary attention. Thus, they are inherently aware of a sense of abandonment, guided by feelings of inferiority towards their siblings, which in turn leads to the presence of low levels of self-esteem (Fernandes 2005), thus making them more vulnerable to the victimization of violent behaviors. Similar conclusions are described in the study by Bowes et al. (2014) with a sample of 2002 young adults aged 18 years. The authors found that victims of violence between siblings are characterized by belonging to a sibling dyad where an older brother is present. However, according to Tippett and Wolke (2015), the eldest siblings are also victims of sibling violence. The authors suggest that the youngest siblings desire the resources of the eldest siblings and, therefore, behave more aggressively.
The results of the present study also allow us to observe a predictive effect of self-esteem on the development of behaviors of both negotiation and sibling violence. This predictive effect is observed for both acts of perpetration and victimization, except for the victimization of sexual assaults; the existence of a positive role of self-esteem in negotiation behaviors is noteworthy. It is thus suggested, as already mentioned in the present discussion, that high self-esteem is associated with a greater capacity to resolve intra-family conflicts, thus favoring dialogue and communication between brothers and sisters. On the other hand, self-esteem is negatively associated with violent behaviors between siblings. In the face of such results, it seems that siblings who have high self-esteem, that is, who show high levels of satisfaction with themselves and the outside world, do not seem to have the need to develop any rivalry with any other sibling. Empirical conceptions present in the literature corroborate the results explained in that they argue that relationships between siblings influence self-esteem and a positive and healthy relationship between siblings benefits the quality of self-esteem, thus developing positive feelings and perceptions about oneself (Yeh and Lempers 2004). Also, about the possible presence of aggressive behaviors in the dynamics between siblings, Avanci et al. (2007) and Wiehe (1997) show that elements with high self-esteem less frequently victimize aggressions perpetrated by their siblings. Concomitantly, Wiehe (1997) also postulates that in addition to the victims, aggressors in sibling violence are also characterized by low levels of self-esteem. Additionally, Dantchev and Wolke (2019) argue that high self-esteem seems to be protective of the victim status.
The position’s role in the sibling dyad was only significant for the perpetration of negotiation and psychological aggression and the victimization of psychological aggression and physical assault. In this follow-up, the middle brother reveals a greater predisposition to converse with the other siblings when compared to the youngest brother. It is suggested that this capacity for dialogue with others, present in the dyad, is because both the middle brother and the youngest one experience, from birth, social relations with the brothers. However, when these relationships are associated with the different fraternal roles that the middle brother can assume (Fernandes 2005), they give him characteristics such as cooperation and negotiation. In addition to the relational networks experienced since its birth, it is also noted that this adaptability to situations gives them prosocial characteristics such as the capacity for dialogue. However, empirical conceptions present in the literature contradict the present result; according to Fernandes (2005) and Fernandes et al. (2007), the middle brother presents a personality marked by aggressiveness and little concern for others, focusing essentially on himself. Thus, the presence of capacities relative to the middle brother is not expected, such as the predisposition to establish conversations to resolve conflicts arising in the fraternal dyad. Also, when equated with the eldest, the middle brother reveals a greater involvement in behaviors of the perpetration of psychological aggression. The characteristics inherent to the older brother in the literature are suggested to assume special importance in this relationship. Especially in the case of being male, the eldest son is seen by the parents as their successor, the one who will continue with their legacy (Fernandes 2005). Derived from this assumption, characteristics are inherent in the older brother that differentiate him from the middle brother. It is known that older people are revealed to be more conservative (Fernandes 2005) to the extent that they are instilled, by their parents, with the values and beliefs adopted by them. For this reason, the older one presents personality characteristics such as integrity, responsibility, greater concern for others and lower aggressiveness (Fernandes 2005; Fernandes et al. 2007). Conversely, the middle brother is given characteristics such as competitiveness and aggressiveness (Fernandes 2005; Fernandes et al. 2007). However, the present results are inconsistent with the existing literature on this problem. Finkelhor et al. (2006), using a sample of 2030 individuals aged between 2 and 17 years, found that older siblings most often play the role of aggressor in sibling violence. The middle brother reveals being more the victim of psychological aggression than the older brother. Regarding being victims of physical assault, the middle sibling reveals greater involvement when compared to the youngest and oldest. These results align with those reported by Dantchev and Wolke (2019) where the firstborn children were more likely to be perpetrators. The results can also be explained based on the assumptions already evidenced in the current discussion. Knowing that characteristics such as low self-esteem (Fernandes 2005) are found in the personality of the middle brother, this fact seems to predispose him to greater involvement in sibling violence (Wiehe 1997), more specifically, assuming the role of victim. The same is no longer true for the other siblings (older brother and younger brother) because, as already mentioned, they are at the extremes of the fraternal constellation and have their fraternal roles very well defined, which gives them security (Fernandes 2005), protecting them in situations demarcated by violence. These results point in the same direction as the empirical conceptions present in the literature, stating that high levels of self-esteem are inherent to a good relationship between siblings; on the contrary, in the face of a relationship demarcated by violence, it fosters the subject of a negative self-evaluation (Yeh and Lempers 2004).
Finally, there was a sex role in the perpetration of sexual coercion and the victimization of injury, that is, of aggression causing serious physical harm. Thus, females, when compared to males, revealed a greater propensity to establish conversations and use this method to resolve conflicts arising within the sibling dyad. As already mentioned in the present discussion, it is suggested that, based on sex stereotypes, the adoption of negotiation behaviors by females was an expected result. Due to the belief that females should assume a behavior connoted by delicacy (Simões et al. 2015), the primary attachment figures interact early with the baby according to these same convictions (Seixas 2009). According to Kindlon and Thompson (2000), parents, in front of a female baby, maintain a longer conversation, always showing a concern for other individuals, which is no longer the case with a male child. In addition, the predisposition to a greater development of communicative skills also belongs to females (Rubia 2007), as they achieve, before males, a maturation of social skills (Legato 2009).
Regarding the perpetration of sexual assaults, compared to females, males stand out in the adoption of abusive conduct of this type. Sex stereotypes, as already mentioned in this discussion, once again seem to have a strong effect on this association. The behavioral mode, determined by a society that must be employed by males, is connoted by aggressiveness and frontality, contrasting with the subtlety and delicacy particular to females (Simões et al. 2015). An inequality of power and a central characteristic of violence between siblings is also highlighted in this analysis. Noting that, normatively, males are physically stronger, the perpetration of sexual abuse is more achievable for them when compared to females (Monks et al. 2009). The existing literature on the subject in question denotes evidence that follows the same direction as the results obtained in this study, clarifying the greater propensity of males to engage as aggressors in sexual violence when compared to the females (e.g., Relva et al. 2013; Relva et al. 2014). Regarding the victimization caused by physical aggressions that cause serious physical harm, males also show greater involvement. It is suggested that these results can be explained by showing that males, recurrently, exert aggressions on individuals of the same sex (Relva et al. 2014). In this sense, there are several studies that corroborate the results presented here, demonstrating that males have a greater propensity to be involved, as a victim, in behaviors of physical aggressions that cause serious physical damage (e.g., Relva et al. 2013; Relva et al. 2014).

Practical Implications, Limitations and Suggestions for Future Studies

Regarding practical implications, the presence of high self-esteem levels in adolescents’ psychosocial development is highlighted, protecting them when they are in situations of sibling violence. This result is extremely important since self-esteem works as a protective factor. Additionally, developing programs that help children and adolescents improve the quality of sibling interaction can contribute to reducing sibling conflict. In this way, it is intended to highlight the importance of the adjusted development of self-esteem and alert to sibling violence. In addition, the sex of adolescents and the sibling position occupied seem to be relevant variables in the study of aggression between siblings. Teaching siblings to regulate emotions is also important to promote good sibling relationships. The current findings also suggest that siblings, where negotiation strategies are reduced, can be helped to develop these skills and contribute to reduced sibling violence. Parents can be taught to help children to acquire prosocial competencies.
The present study has several limitations. The first limitation is the cross-sectional character, not allowing the establishment of cause–effect relationships. The present investigation also uses self-report instruments. A convenience sample was also a limitation. Additionally, some participants presented a reluctance to answer the questionnaire regarding the problem of sibling violence since it is a theme that is still little accepted. Also, the small age range assumes great prominence, hindering the analyses of different ages. Finally, we only have the perspective of one sibling. Future studies should explore both perspectives and parents’ perceptions of the quality of the sibling relationships.
Expanding the sample to other age sets is suggested for future investigations to compare groups. Additionally, using longitudinal studies and covering other types of intrafamily violence to provide cause–effect connections.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.P.M. and J.R.S.; methodology, C.P.M. and J.R.S.; software, C.P.M. and J.R.S.; validation, C.P.M. and J.R.S.; formal analysis, C.P.M. and J.R.S.; investigation, C.P.M. and J.R.S.; resources, C.P.M. and J.R.S.; data curation, C.P.M.; writing—original draft preparation, C.P.M., J.R.S. and I.C.R.; writing—review and editing, C.P.M. and I.C.R.; visualization, C.P.M. and I.C.R.; supervision, C.P.M.; project administration, C.P.M.; funding acquisition, C.P.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

National funds support Catarina Pinheiro Mota through Center for Psychology (CPUP)–FCT- Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology (UIDB/00050/2020); and Inês Relva works through the FCT—Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology under the project the Scientific Employment Stimulus-Institutional Call–CEECINST/00127/2018 and under the following project UIDB04045/2020.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Ethics approval was not sought for this study because there was no Ethics Committee in the University of Trás-os-Montes and Alto Douro when the project began. The project was submitted to a scientific board of the university.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in this study.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data, analysis code and materials used in this manuscript are not openly available due to privacy and ethical restrictions but can be obtained from the corresponding author. No aspects of this study were pre-registered.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Ainsworth, Mary Dinsmore. 1989. Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist 44: 709–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Avanci, Joviana, Simone Assis, Nilton César Santos, and Rachel Oliveira. 2007. Adaptação transcultural da escala de auto-estima para adolescentes. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica 20: 397–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Bowes, Lucy, Dieter Wolke, Carol Joinson, Tanya Lereya, and Glyn Lewis. 2014. Sibling bullying and risk of depression, anxiety, and self-harm: A prospective cohort study. Pedriatics 134: e1032–e1038. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Bowlby, John. 1982. Attachment and Loss: Attachment, 2nd ed. New York: Basic Books, vol. 1. First published 1969. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bowlby, John. 1988. A Secure Base: Parent-Child Attachment and Healthy Human Development. New York: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
  6. Caspi, Jonathan. 2012. Sibling Aggression: Assessment and Treatment. Berlin and Heidelberg: Springer Publishing Company. [Google Scholar]
  7. Cohen, Jacob. 1988. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. [Google Scholar]
  8. Dantchev, Slava, and Dieter Wolke. 2019. Trouble in the nest: Antecedents of sibling bullying victimization and perpetration. Developmental Psychology 55: 1059–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Dunn, Judy, and Robert Plomin. 1990. Separate Lives: Why Siblings Are So Different. New York: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
  10. Espínola, Solange Rodriguez. 2010. Relacion entre nível socioeconomico, apoyo social percibido, género y depresion en ninos. Interdisciplinaria 27: 261–75. [Google Scholar]
  11. Fernandes, Otília Monteiro. 2000. Fratria e Personalidade. Vila Real: Tese de doutoramento não publicada, Universidade de Trás-os-Montes e Alto Douro. [Google Scholar]
  12. Fernandes, Otília Monteiro. 2002. Semelhanças e Diferenças Entre Irmãos. Lisbon: Climepsi Editores. [Google Scholar]
  13. Fernandes, Otília Monteiro. 2005. Ser único ou ser Irmão. Lisbon: Oficina do Livro. [Google Scholar]
  14. Fernandes, Otília Monteiro, Madalena Alarcão, and José Vasconcelos Raposo. 2007. Position in the sibling dyad e personalidade. Estudos da Psicologia 24: 297–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Field, Andy. 2005. Discovering Statistics Using SPSS. London: Sage Publications. [Google Scholar]
  16. Finkelhor, David, Heather Turner, and Richard Ormrod. 2006. Kid’s stuff: The nature and impact of peer and sibling violence on younger and older children. Child Abuse & Neglect 30: 1401–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Fraley, Chris, and Keith Davis. 1997. Attachment formation and transfer in young adults’ close relationships and romantic relationships. Personal Relationships 4: 131–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Gallagher, Annabella, Kimberly Updegraff, Jenny Padilla, and Susan McHale. 2018. Longitudinal associations between sibling relational aggression and adolescent adjustment. Journal of Youth Adolescence 47: 2100–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Gentile, Brittany, Shelly Grabe, Brenda Dolan-Pascoe, Jean Twenge, Brooke Wells, and Alissa Maitino. 2009. Gender differences in domain-specific self-esteem: A meta-analysis. Review of General Psychology 13: 34–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Geraldes, Raquel, Isabel Soares, and Carla Martins. 2013. Vinculação no contexto familiar: Relações entre cônjuges, entre pais e filhos adolescentes e entre irmãos. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica 26: 799–808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Green, Arthur. 1984. Child abuse by siblings. Child Abuse & Neglect 8: 311–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Hazan, Cindy, and Debra Zeifman. 1994. Sex and the psychological tether. In Advances in Personal Relationships. Edited by Kim Bartholomew and Daniel Perlman. London: Jessica Kingsley, vol. 5, pp. 151–80. [Google Scholar]
  23. Hoffman, Kristi, and John Edwards. 2004. An integrated theoretical model of sibling violence and abuse. Journal of Family Violence 19: 185–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Khan, Roxanne, and Paul Rogers. 2015. The normalization of sibling violence: Does gender and personal experience of violence influence perceptions of physical assault against siblings? Journal of Interpersonal Violence 30: 437–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Kiang, Lisa, and Wyndol Furman. 2007. Representations of attachment to parents in adolescent sibling pairs: Concordant or discordant? New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development 2007: 73–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Kindlon, Dan, and Michael Thompson. 2000. Criando Caim. Proteger a vida Emocional dos Rapazes. Porto: Ambar. [Google Scholar]
  27. Kiselica, Mark, and Mandy Morrill-Richards. 2007. Sibling maltreatment: The forgotten abuse. Journal of Counseling & Development 85: 148–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Krienert, Jessie, and Jeffrey Walsh. 2011. My brother’s keeper: A contemporary examination of reported sibling violence using national level data, 2000–2005. Journal of Family Violence 26: 331–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Laopratai, Mananya, Ketsupar Jirakran, and Weerasak Chonchaiya. 2023. Factors affecting sibling bullying and its association with self-esteem and depression in middle school students. European Journal of Pediatrics 182: 3501–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Legato, Marianne. 2009. Porque Morrem os Homens Primeiro? Lisboa: Caleidoscópio. [Google Scholar]
  31. Linares, Oriana. 2006. An understudied form of intra-family violence: Sibling-to-sibling aggression among foster children. Aggression and Violent Behavior 11: 95–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Marôco, João. 2007. Análise Estatística com Utilização do SPSS. Lisboa: Sílabo. [Google Scholar]
  33. Mayseless, Ofra. 2004. Home leaving to militar service: Attachment concerns, transfer of attachment functions from parents to peers, and adjustment. Journal of Adolescent Research 1: 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Meeus, Wim, Annerieke Oosterwegel, and Wilma Volleberg. 2002. Parental and peer attachment and identity development in adolescence. Journal of Adolescence 25: 93–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Menesini, Ersilia, Marina Camodeca, and Annalaura Nocentini. 2010. Bullying among siblings: The role of personality and relational variables. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 28: 921–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Monks, Claire, Peter Smith, Paul Naylor, Christine Barter, Jane Ireland, and Iain Coyne. 2009. Bullying in different contexts: Commonalities, differences and the role of theory. Aggression and Violent Behavior 14: 146–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Mota, Catarina Pinheiro. 2021. Crianças, Jovens e Cuidadores no Acolhimento Residencial: A vivência de quem Cuida e de quem quer ser Cuidado [Children, Young People and Caregivers in Residential Care: The Experience of Those Who Care and Those Who Want to Be Cared for]. Lisboa: Edições Sílabo. ISBN 9789895611614. [Google Scholar]
  38. Mota, Catarina Pinheiro, and Magda Rocha. 2012. Adolescência e jovem adultícia: Crescimento pessoal, separação-individuação e o jogo das relações [Adolescence and young adulthood: Personal growth, separation-individuation and the game of relationships]. Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa 28: 357–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Mota, Catarina Pinheiro, and Paula Mena Matos. 2015. Does sibling relationship matter to self-concept and resilience in institutionalized adolescents? Child & Youth Service Review 56: 97–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Mota, Catarina Pinheiro, Liliana Serra, Inês Carvalho Relva, and Otília Monteiro Fernandes. 2017. Do sibling relationship protects adolescents in residential care and traditional families from developing of psychopathologies? Journal of Family Studies 23: 260–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Nickerson, Amanda, and Richard Nagle. 2005. Parent and peer relations in middle childhood and early adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence 25: 223–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Pallant, Julie. 2020. SPSS Survival Manual: A Step by Step Guide to Data Analysis Using SPSS for Windows, 7th ed. Crows Nest: Allen & Unwin. [Google Scholar]
  43. Quatman, Teri, and Cary Watson. 2001. Gender differences in adolescent self-esteem: An exploration of domains. The Journal of Genetic Psychology 162: 93–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Relva, Inês Carvalho, Madalena Alarcão, Otília Monteiro Fernandes, Joana Carvalho, and Angèle Fauchier. 2019. Sibling conflict and parental discipline: The mediating role of family communication in Portuguese adolescents. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal 36: 295–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Relva, Inês Carvalho, Otília Monteiro Fernandes, and Madalena Alarcão. 2012. Violência entre irmãos: Uma realidade desconhecida. Revista Interamericana de Psicologia 46: 205–14. [Google Scholar]
  46. Relva, Inês Carvalho, Otília Monteiro Fernandes, and Raquel Costa. 2013. Psychometric properties of revised conflict tactics scales: Portuguese sibling version (CTS2-SP). Journal of Family Violence 28: 577–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Relva, Inês Carvalho, Otília Monteiro Fernandes, Madalena Alarcão, and Amadeu Quelhas Martins. 2014. Estudo exploratório sobre a violência entre irmãos em Portugal. Psicologia Reflexão e Escrita 27: 398–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Rocha, Magda. 2008. O Desenvolvimento de Relações de Vinculação na Adolescência: Associações Entre Contextos Relacionais com os Pais, Pares e par Amoroso. Ph.D. dissertation, Faculdade de Psicologia e de Ciências da Educação, Porto, Portugal. [Google Scholar]
  49. Rosenberg, Morris. 1965. Society and the Adolescent Self-image. Princeton: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  50. Rosenberg, Morris, Carmi Schooler, Carrie Schoenbach, and Florence Rosenberg. 1995. Global self-esteem and specific self-esteem: Different concepts, different outcomes. American Sociological Review 60: 141–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Rubia, Francisco. 2007. El sexo del Cerebro. Sevilla: Ediciones Temas de Hoy. [Google Scholar]
  52. Ruiz, David Moreno, Estefanía Estévez López, Sergio Murgui Pérez, and Gonzalo Musitu Ochoa. 2009. Reputación social y violencia relacional en adolescentes: El rol de la soledad, la self-esteem y la satisfacción vital. Psicothema 21: 537–42. [Google Scholar]
  53. Seginer, Rachel. 1998. Adolescents’ perceptions of relationships with older sibling in the context of other close relationships. Journal of Research on Adolescence 8: 287–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Seixas, Sónia Raquel. 2009. Diferenças de género nos comportamentos de bullying: Contributos da neurobiologia. Interacções 13: 63–97. [Google Scholar]
  55. Serra, Adriano Supardo Vaz. 1988. O auto-conceito. Análise Psicológica 2: 101–10. [Google Scholar]
  56. Simões, Sónia, Joaquim Jorge Ferreira, Sandra Braga, and Henrique Vicente. 2015. Bullying, vinculação e estilos educativos parentais em adolescentes do 3º ciclo do ensino básico. Revista Portuguesa de Investigação Comportamental e Social 1: 30–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Simonelli, Catherine, Thomas Mullis, Ann Elliott, and Thomas Pierce. 2002. Abuse by siblings and subsequent experiences of violence within the dating relationship. Journal of Interpersonal Violence 17: 103–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Straus, Murray, Sherry Hamby, Sue Boney-McCoy, and David Bruce Sugarman. 1996. The Revised Conflict Tactics Scales (CTS2): Development and preliminary psychometric data. Journal of Family Issues 17: 283–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Tippett, Neil, and Dieter Wolker. 2015. Aggression between siblings: Associations with the home environment and peer bullying. Aggressive Behavior 41: 14–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Toseeb, Umar, and Dieter Wolke. 2022. Sibling bullying: A prospective longitudinal study of associations with positive and negative mental health during adolescence. Journal of Youth Adolescence 51: 940–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Waite, Evelyn, Lilly Shanahan, Susan Calkins, Susan Keane, and Marion O’Brien. 2011. Life events, sibling warmth, and youths’ adjustment. Journal of Marriage and the Family 73: 902–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Walters, Jeanette, Julee Farley, Kirby Deater-Deckard, and Jungmeen Kim-Spoon. 2020. Predictors of sibling victimization and associations with self-perception and relationship attachment in adolescence. The Journal of Early Adolescence 40: 305–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Wiehe, Vernon. 1997. Sibling Abuse: Hidden Physical, Emotional and Sexual Trauma, 2nd ed. Lexington: Sage Publications. [Google Scholar]
  64. Wolke, Dieter, Neil Tippett, and Slava Dantchev. 2015. Bullying in the family: Sibling bullying. The Lancet Psychiatry 2: 917–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Yeh, Hsiu-Chen, and Jacques Lempers. 2004. Perceived sibling relationships and adolescent development. Journal of Youth and Adolescence 33: 133–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Conceptual Model.
Figure 1. Conceptual Model.
Socsci 12 00595 g001
Table 1. Correlation Between Variables, Mean and Standard Deviation (n = 286).
Table 1. Correlation Between Variables, Mean and Standard Deviation (n = 286).
Variables1234567891011
1. SELF-ESTEEM-
SIBLING VIOLENCE—PERPETRATION
2. Negotiation
Sig. (2 tailed)
0.21 **
0.000
-
3. Psychological aggression
Sig. (2 tailed)
−0.19 **
0.001
0.25 **
0.000
-
4. Physical assault
Sig. (2 tailed)
−0.18 **
0.002
0.13 **
0.034
0.74 **
0.000
-
5. Sexual coercion
Sig. (2 tailed)
−0.15 *
0.014
−0.07
0.238
0.06
0.281
0.33 **
0.000
-
6. Injury
Sig (2 tailed)
−0.18 **
0.002
−0.04
0.482
0.31 **
0.000
0.62 **
0.000
0.51 **
0.000
-
SIBLING VIOLENCE—VICTIMIZATION
7. Negotiation
Sig (2 tailed)
0.24 **
0.000
0.94 **
0.000
0.16 **
0.005
0.06
0.283
−0.07
0.247
−0.05
0.377
-
8. Psychological aggression
Sig (2 tailed)
−0.19 **
0.002
0.25 **
0.000
0.95 **
0.000
0.73 **
0.000
0.06
0.332
0.32 **
0.000
0.18 **
0.003
-
9. Physical assault
Sig (2 tailed)
−0.18 **
0.003
0.13 *
0.023
0.73 **
0.000
0.92 **
0.000
0.30 **
0.000
0.59 **
0.000
0.07
0.276
0.73 **
0.000
-
10. Sexual coercion
Sig (2 tailed)
−0.02
0.755
0.03
0.629
0.16 **
0.007
0.43 **
0.000
0.60 **
0.000
0.59 **
0.000
0.03
0.585
0.14 *
0.018
0.39 **
0.000
-
11. Injury
Sig (2 tailed)
−0.21 **
0.000
−0.01
0.863
0.33 **
0.000
0.62 **
0.000
0.51 **
0.000
0.87 **
0.000
−0.04
0.534
0.31 **
0.000
0.64 **
0.000
0.53 **
0.000
-
M3.013.041.090.380.080.172.911.020.400.070.18
SD0.591.681.200.650.300.401.661.190.690.270.43
Note: * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
Table 2. Mean values and Standard Deviation of Violence Between Siblings as a Function of Sex.
Table 2. Mean values and Standard Deviation of Violence Between Siblings as a Function of Sex.
Variables 1—Female2—MaleDirection of Significant Differences
(n = 171)(n = 115)
IC 95%M ± SDM ± SD
Sibling Violence—Perpetration
Negotiation[−0.81; −0.02]3.20 ± 1.592.79 ± 1.771 > 2
Psychological aggression[−0.57; −0.01]1.21 ± 1.250.92 ± 1.091 > 2
Sexual coercion[0.02; 0.18]0.03 ± 0.200.14 ± 0.391 < 2
Sibling Violence—Victimization
Psychological aggression[−0.58; −0.03]1.14 ± 1.260.84 ± 1.061 > 2
Injury[0.00; 0.22]0.13 ± 0.350.24 ± 0.521 < 2
Table 3. Differential Analysis of Sibling Violence as a Function of Position in the Sibling Dyad.
Table 3. Differential Analysis of Sibling Violence as a Function of Position in the Sibling Dyad.
Position in the Sibling DyadM ± SDIC 95%Direction of Differences
Significant
Sibling violence—Victimization
Physical assault1—Youngest sibling0.40 ± 0.70[0.29, 0.50]1 < 2
2 > 3
2—Middle sibling0.72 ± 0.98[0.50, 0.95]
3—Eldest sibling0.28 ± 0.45[0.14, 0.42]
Table 4. Hierarchical Multiple Regression for the Dimensions of the Subscale Perpetration of Violence Between Siblings.
Table 4. Hierarchical Multiple Regression for the Dimensions of the Subscale Perpetration of Violence Between Siblings.
Sibling Violence—Perpetration
Negotiation
R2R2ChangeBSEβtp
Block 1—Sex (dummy)0.02 0.430.200.132.190.030
Block 2—Position in the sibling dyad0.030.02
Middle sibling Ref.
Youngest sibling −0.680.30−0.20−2.250.025
Eldest sibling −0.550.32−0.15−1.700.09
Block 3—Self-esteem 0.620.160.223.790.000
Psychological aggressionR2R2ChangeBSEβtp
Block 1—Sex (dummy)0.01 0.260.140.111.830.068
Block 2—Position in the sibling dyad (dummy)0.030.02
Middle sibling Ref.
Youngest sibling −0.370.22−0.15−1.700.090
Eldest sibling −0.520.23−0.20−2.220.027
Block 3—Self-esteem0.060.05−0.360.12−0.18−3.060.002
Physical assaultR2R2ChangeBSEβtp
Block 1—Sex (dummy)0.00 0.000.080.000.040.971
Block 2—Position in the sibling dyad (dummy)0.010.00
Middle sibling Ref.
Youngest sibling −0.110.12−0.08−0.910.363
Eldest sibling −0.190.13−0.14−1.480.141
Block 3—Self-esteem0.040.03−0.200.06−0.18−3.070.002
Sexual coercionR2R2ChangeBSEβtp
Block 1—Sex (dummy)0.03 −0.100.04−0.17−2.910.004
Block 2—Position in the sibling dyad (dummy)0.030.02
Middle sibling Ref.
Youngest sibling −0.020.05−0.03−0.300.762
Eldest sibling −0.040.06−0.07−0.740.459
Block 3—Self-esteem0.060.04−0.080.03−0.15−2.640.009
InjuryR2R2ChangeBSEβtp
Block 1—Sex (dummy)0.01 −0.070.05−0.09−1.470.143
Block 2—Position in the sibling dyad (dummy)0.010.00
Middle sibling Ref.
Youngest sibling 0.070.070.090.990.326
Eldest sibling 0.0070.080.0090.100.924
Block 3—Self-esteem0.050.03−0.120.04−0.18−3.100.002
Note: B, SE and β for a significance level of p < 0.05. Block 1—Sex; Block 2—Position in the sibling dyad; Block 3—Self-esteem.
Table 5. Hierarchical Multiple Regression for the Dimensions of the Subscale Victimization of Violence Between Siblings.
Table 5. Hierarchical Multiple Regression for the Dimensions of the Subscale Victimization of Violence Between Siblings.
Sibling Violence—Victimization
Negotiation
R2R2ChangeBSEβtp
Block 1—Sex (dummy)0.01 0.340.200.101.720.086
Block 2—Position in the sibling dyad (dummy)0.010.01
Middle sibling Ref.
Youngest sibling −0.510.30−0.15−1.710.089
Eldest sibling −0.470.32−0.13−1.450.148
Block 3—Self-esteem0.060.060.680.160.244.200.000
Psychological aggressionR2R2ChangeBSEβtp
Block 1—Sex (dummy)0.02 0.280.140.111.960.051
Block 2—Position in the sibling dyad (dummy)0.030.02
Middle sibling Ref.
Youngest sibling −0.320.22−0.13−1.470.143
Eldest sibling −0.470.23−0.18−2.000.046
Block 3—Self-esteem0.060.05−0.350.12−0.18−3.010.003
Physical assaultR2R2ChangeBSEβtp
Block 1—Sex (dummy)0.00 −0.060.08−0.05−0.790.431
Block 2—Position in the sibling dyad (dummy)0.040.03
Middle sibling Ref.
Youngest sibling −0.330.16−0.24−2.620.009
Eldest sibling −0.430.13−0.29−30.250.001
Block 3—Self-esteem0.070.05−0.200.07−0.17−2.970.003
Sexual coercionR2R2ChangeBSEβtp
Block 1—Sex (dummy)0.000.00−0.020.03−0.04−0.710.77
Block 2—Position in the sibling dyad (dummy)0.00−0.01
Middle sibling Ref.
Youngest sibling 0.040.050.070.740.462
Eldest sibling 0.030.050.040.460.648
Block 3—Self-esteem0.00−0.01−0.010.03−0.02−0.350.729
InjuryR2R2ChangeBSEβtP
Block 1—Sex (dummy)0.02 −0.120.05−0.14−2.380.018
Block 2—Position in the sibling dyad (dummy)0.020.01
Middle sibling Ref.
Youngest sibling 0.050.080.060.690.493
Eldest sibling −0.010.08−0.01−0.140.890
Block 3—Self-esteem0.070.05−0.150.04−0.21−3.660.000
Note: B, SE and β for a significance level of p < 0.05. Block 1—Sex; Block 2—Position in the phratry; Block 3—Self-esteem.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Mota, C.P.; Sousa, J.R.; Relva, I.C. Sibling Violence and Position in Sibling Dyad in a Sample of Adolescents: How Does It Relate to Self-Esteem? Soc. Sci. 2023, 12, 595. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci12110595

AMA Style

Mota CP, Sousa JR, Relva IC. Sibling Violence and Position in Sibling Dyad in a Sample of Adolescents: How Does It Relate to Self-Esteem? Social Sciences. 2023; 12(11):595. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci12110595

Chicago/Turabian Style

Mota, Catarina Pinheiro, Joana Rita Sousa, and Inês Carvalho Relva. 2023. "Sibling Violence and Position in Sibling Dyad in a Sample of Adolescents: How Does It Relate to Self-Esteem?" Social Sciences 12, no. 11: 595. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci12110595

APA Style

Mota, C. P., Sousa, J. R., & Relva, I. C. (2023). Sibling Violence and Position in Sibling Dyad in a Sample of Adolescents: How Does It Relate to Self-Esteem? Social Sciences, 12(11), 595. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci12110595

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop