1. Introduction
The narrative that that young people are increasingly uninterested in politics is widespread {xe “politics: young people’s lack of interest”} [
1,
2,
3], although there are some alternative analyses (such as Ekström [
4], Ross and Dooly [
5] and Kiisel et al. [
6]). Hahn suggested that many studies of political socialization construct young people as passive recipients of political messages from the social environment [
7] (p. 20): her study of citizenship education programmes in the UK, Denmark, Germany, The Netherlands and the USA concluded that political culture and educational ethos interact to limit the extent to which educational programmes contribute to the construction of political identity. Resistance to political and social education is sometimes associated with a denial that young people can understand sophisticated political concepts (Maitles [
8]). Carrington and Troyna [
9], Claire and Holden [
10] and Cowan and Maitles [
11] all report on teacher {xe “teachers: avoiding controversial issues” \t “
See also school ”} and institutional resistance to introducing anything seen as controversial in educational settings. This paper is a study of the social construction of political identity, rather than of ‘political socialization’: the methodology described assumes that young people have agency in their active construction of the political, rather than being the passive recipients of some ‘socialization’ process. It also suggests that many of these young people construct themselves as a generationally distinct cohort from their elders: more inclined to a cosmopolitan world-view, particularly with an emphasis on what they see as distinctively European values of rights and equalities, and less inclined to identify with their ‘nation’.
The literature on young people’s ‘democratic deficit {xe “democratic deficit”}’ has developed particularly over the last two decades (for example, Oelkers [
12], Christodoulou et al. [
13], Hendriks [
14]; Frank et al. [
15]). There have been concerns in the European Union (EU), where the European Parliamentary elections participation has often been lower than the national election vote (Avbelj [
16]; Mitchell [
17]; European Parliament [
18]. Calenda and Meijer [
19] suggest that younger people are less interested in politics, not because of their age, but as a cohort effect: ‘older generations now were more active as youngsters than young people are today,’ and this ‘can be attributed to a changing attitude towards politics … related to a more individualistic, and even hedonistic, attitude’ [
19] (p. 879). Forbrig points out that ‘many lament a dramatic decline in the political involvement of younger generations, and decreasing levels of youth participation in elections, political parties and traditional social organizations are seen to provide ample evidence of this’ [
20] (p. 7).
These claims are largely predicated on particular styles of political participation, and it can be argued that such ‘traditional’ political activity {xe “young people: political-non-traditional activism”} as voting is not the only possible way of participation, and indeed is rooted in an outdated conception of what constitutes a civic culture. Much commentary is based on a concept of civic culture in which most citizens were expected to quietly endorse the political system, occasionally selecting between parties with broadly similar policies. The classic exposition by Almond and Verba [
21] described a passive acceptance of existing political {xe “politics: traditional forms of engagement”} systems and structures, with a very few more actively involved in political roles. Manning [
22] points out that the ‘discourse of youth apathy typically draws upon quantitative methodologies and orthodox hegemonic notions of politics {xe “politics: traditional forms of engagement”} … [which] privileges institutionalized politics and holds the activities of political parties and electoral politics at its core’ [
22] (p. 2). Academic analyses of young people’s political awareness at the time of Almond and Verba tended to focus on how well civic institutions and symbols were recognized in questionnaires: the presidency, the flag, national days, and buildings such as parliament (Greenstein et al. [
23], Jackson [
24]). Such narratives of young people’s apathy hold that these narrow and regulatory models of political activity
are politics, and that lack of participation in this traditional electoral model is indicative of lack of knowledge and interest in the political. Henn et al. have called this ‘conventional political science’ [
25] (p. 170): they argue that including wider forms of political participation in studies of young people’s participation would show much greater evidence of activity [
26]. This analysis suggests that many of the cohort studied show a distinctly different awareness of their political engagement—their sense of attachment to the political entities of the nation and state is less robust—and that this reflects their perception of changes in demography (increased diversity), changes in the geo-political (globalization, a post-cold war environment, and the extension of the European Union), and changes in news media consumption.
Norris [
27] also argues that older forms of political and social engagement are being replaced: ‘political participation is evolving in terms of the ‘who’ (the agencies and the collective organizations), ‘what’ (the repertoires of actions commonly used for political expression) and ‘where’ (the targets that participants seek to influence)’ [
27] (p. 4). Traditional electoral participation and political party membership is being supplanted by informal political and social participation through demonstrations, political activism around single issues, petitions, greater participation at the micro level, and young people in particular are involved in these activities [
28].
This paper adds to these challenges to the assumptions of apathy and low participation, through analysis of some of the data gathered in a large qualitative one-person study made by the author of how young people were constructing their social and political identities in a number of European countries. This larger study, reported more fully elsewhere [
29,
30] examined identity construction in terms of identification with a country and with Europe, and the methodology is outlined in the following section. Young people (aged between 11 and 20) took part in small group conversations (n =
c6) that were very largely non-directive, and which showed that many of them expressed interest and concerns about political issues, and sometimes frustration at a lack of fora in which these could be discussed. This age range was selected partly because it was the first generation of teenagers to be born after the fall of the Berlin Wall (the eldest, in the earliest conversations, were born in 1990), and partly for opportunistic reasons of access through educational establishments. There were also sometimes accounts of attempts at political actions around these issues of concern. Issues that were often raised included perceptions of injustice around human rights, migration and refugee policies, and concerns at the rise of nationalist parties. Particularly, there was frequently a sense of a generational difference from the views of (sometimes) their parents’ and (often) their grandparents’ views around human rights and the growth of the European Union.
2. Materials and Methods
Social constructions are made in social contexts, and are inevitably not fixed essentialist beliefs, but contingent on the social lens used in interactions with others [
31,
32,
33]. The ways in which individuals and groups identify with an entity such as a country or a state, or with something as potentially nebulous as ‘Europe’, will be expressed in very different ways in a lengthy conversation than it will be in response to a questionnaire that presents pre-defined categories. Nuances and ambiguities that are context-dependent will lead to considerably less precise and clear-cut data which, I would argue, are more representative of the way that most people approach political issues and controversies. The overall research {xe “discussion group: as a methodology”} strategy outlined here was to use group conversations to elicit from the group a series of potential lenses, and to invite members to discuss their identities in these contexts. These elicited several different constructions from the same group: but the interest was not simply in what these different identities were; although they were of some significance, they were necessarily transient, and were not precisely reproducible. The whole range of contingencies surrounding any social encounter cannot be controlled or fully noted. Of more enduring significance is to understand the range of variables and resources used to make differing constructions, and how the individual and group make apparently dissonant or even conflicting constructions compatible.
An added complication in working with young people in education is that they very often anticipate questions to be closed. Teachers (and many other adults) tend to use questions to test or assess young people’s knowledge (Alexander [
34], Hogden and Webb [
35]), rather than to elicit ideas. Therefore, they expect a question to have a ‘correct’ answer that they are supposed to supply, and often feel obliged to find a ‘right’ response.
Group conversations have been developed in German social science research over the past 20 years (Bohnsack [
36], Loos and Schäffer [
37]): The
gruppendiskussionsverfahren [group discussion method] has been described as ‘an open interview, intended to let respondents develop a topic in their own language, in their symbolic system and their relevant framework’, so that analysis ‘can avoid projecting into single utterances meanings that are not appropriate … [we] learn more if this statement is put into a narrative context by the respondent … in his/her own language’ (Bohnsack [
36] (p. 21), translated by Scheunpflug et al. [
38]). This is less structured and more open than traditional focus group techniques. Scheunpflug et al. describe it as a method ‘in which respondents can set the structures and contents of the conversation by themselves,’ thus exploring ‘knowledge stocks that are not located on the surface of conscious and clear explicable attitudes and values, but which are beneath the surface’ [
38] (p. 10). Wagener refers to this as ‘conjunctive knowledge … implicit, action-guiding knowledge … based and acquired in fundamental experiences … that groups of individuals share with each other’ [
39] (p. 92). My method was thus to provide narrative-generating stimuli to initiate discussion: I would begin by exploring immanent issues—the topics, accounts and language that the group members use in their narratives—and only later move to ask exmanent questions—my own agenda of themes, thus giving them the opportunity to develop structures that seem relevant to them.
The conversations {xe “discussion groups: strategies”} were varied in focus and emphasis, and my questions changed in response, and in their wording, in order to maintain the mode as conversational rather than interrogatory. While I had areas that I wanted to explore, I did not refer to this in the discussions, or stick to a particular sequence. Therefore responses cannot be numerically analyzed, nor be easily presented in a tabular form: I can describe apparently significant trends, but not assert, for example, that “67 percent said that political values were a significant area of interest”—and even had I put an identical question to every one of them, the statistic would still be meaningless. But the conversations were not structureless: I had my ‘instruments of construction’ (Bourdieu et al. [
40] (p. 248). I asked them initially to each describe their identity, and from this usually elicited what country/countries they felt they identified with; and would then ask what they saw as the positive and less positive aspects of those countries’ societies. Did they think the views they expressed would be different from those of people in other parts of the country, or those of a different age? Europe was often mentioned in this: I would then ask if they ‘at some times, in some ways, also felt that they were European?’ Towards the end of each conversation, I often asked whether they though possible Russian membership of the European Union was desirable or not; and ask if, and with whom, they discussed the issues we had been talking about (Family? Friends? Teachers in school?).
I would assure each group that there are no right answers, and that any response would be accepted and valued, that disagreement was possible. My objective was to establish an empowering rapport, so that discussion was, to a substantial extent, directed and paced by group members.
My strategies to initiate discussions {xe “discussion groups: strategies”} that produced this kind of dialogue were as follows:
not to introduce leading terms, such as nation or state, but to use words such as ‘country’;
to only use terms such as nation, state, Balkan, or Nordic when they themselves had introduced them;
questions to be transparently open (if someone said they were French, I might respond ‘What makes you French?’);
to accept all responses as valid (nodding, saying how interesting the response was);
to maintain direct eye contact with each speaker (showing I was following them);
to often construct questions as responses to what they had said (so it appeared that the group was determining the agenda;
to asking as few questions as possible (leaving space for disagreement, supplementary comments);
not directly asking an individual to respond (not everyone replied to each question: this was a discussion, not a sequential interview);
to ask for elaborations, explanations and examples; and
to loop the conversation back to earlier comments, when appropriate.
These strategies {xe “discussion groups: overview of responses”} were not always wholly successful, but nearly every group sustained a conversation for more than 30 min (the average was 45 min), and many could have lasted 90 min or more. Most young people (about 95 percent) made more than a minimal contribution: two thirds could be described as fully participative for the entire session.
Where possible we sat in a circle around a table. I introduced myself as a professor from London who was visiting about 30 European countries to talk with groups of young people, such as themselves, about their ideas. I was very grateful to them for giving their time. I introduced my colleague, from a university in the town or city, who would interpret when needed. At this point we established which language we would use: in about two thirds of the cases we used predominantly English (very often wholly so), and in less than a sixth we used only the local language, with everything translated. My colleague was able, where necessary, to translate words, sentences or phrases in either direction.
All conversations were recorded and transcribed in full. Pseudonyms have been given to all participants which reflect the country from which their real first name was drawn and their gender.
This was a one-person study, which I began when I semi-retired in 2010. I had previously been researching citizenship education in Europe, partly through coordinating an Erasmus Academic Network in this area to 2008, which {xe “Children’s Identity and Citizenship in Europe”} linked academics across Europe who shared an interest in issues of young people’s identities and sense of citizenship. Members of this network became important supporters and enablers in carrying out this project, but I also used the Network for European Citizenship Education {xe “Netword for European Citizenship Education”} (NECE, organized by the German Bundeszentrale für politische Bildung) and the British Council {xe “British Council”}. A Jean Monnet professorship supported my travel expenses for the period 2010–2012, and thereafter I was self-funded.
My first phase of fieldwork {xe “fieldwork: range of countries”} (2010–2012) covered the countries that had joined the European Union after 2004 (Bulgaria, Czech Republic, Cyprus (including the northern part of the island), Estonia, Hungary, Latvia, Lithuania, Poland, Romania, Slovakia, Slovenia) and the candidate countries at that time (Iceland, Croatia, Macedonia and Turkey) [
29]. A second phase {xe “fieldwork: dates”} (2014–2016) added many of the pre-2004 European Union members (Austria, Belgium, Denmark, Finland, France, Germany, Italy, Luxembourg, The Netherlands, Portugal, Spain and Sweden) and two non-EU members (Norway and Switzerland). I did not include Greece, because I thought that the particular relationship with the European Union over the entire period would be too distracting; nor the United Kingdom and the Republic of Ireland (to be treated together because of the border issue), intending to do this after the 2016 referendum. In these 29 states, I organized 324 group discussions, each lasting an average length of 45 min.
I visited at least two places in every country {xe “fieldwork: range of locations”} (with the exception of Luxembourg). In the larger countries I visited more, and at least four locations in every country with a population greater that 10 million: I covered 70 percent of the 75 mainland Level 2 NUTS regions (‘Nomenclature of National Territorial Units for Statistics’) of these countries. Much empirical social science research has been criticized for drawing subjects from a very narrow base: one estimate is that 80 percent of the non-USA studies are drawn from psychology undergraduates in the capital city (Arnett [
41]), and that many such studies are extrapolated to be representative of the country’s inhabitants in general (Rozin [
42]). Gosling et al. [
43] found that, in a sample of social science research articles, the subjects were 85 percent undergraduate students and 71 percent female. Rochat points out that:
In academia, a priori claims of universality sell better than diversity, which complicates rather than simplifies matters. Universality claims get more attention because they are cleaner and sharper, encompassing control and predictive power … [with] greater impact and appeal. This tends to relegate diversity to noise rather than as a primary object of study.
My study was
intentionally noisy, reflecting the diversity of the populations of these countries: hence my emphasis on different locations, and an avoidance of an overemphasis on the capital city. Within the constraints that the population being sampled was of young residents of these European countries (largely industrialized, democratic and comparatively affluent), my recruitment process was designed to avoid the pitfalls analyzed by Henrich et al. [
45]. The sample was not representative, in the sense that the clusters were drawn from schools (the most efficient way of accessing groups of young people of approximately the same age), but was intended to cover the possibilities of regional variation and of the size of settlement (Pichler [
46]). The 104 locations visited correspond to the rank-size distribution of European settlements; ranging from three settlements of less than 1000 to 10 of over 1 million (Doxiadis [
47]). In each location, I asked a different colleague to identify one school or college in a middle-class or professional area, and another in a more working-class area. In each school I was usually able to recruit two groups of six students, one in the 12–15 age range, one 15–18. I specified that I wanted to include young residents of each country (not just citizens) and an appropriate representation of significant minority groups.
{xe “fieldwork: recruitment of subjects”} Most groups were of between five and eight participants; over 95 percent were between 12 and 18 years old; and 56 percent were female (including one person transitioning to female). There were minority-origin young people in many discussion groups: {xe “Sinti” \t “See Roma”} by country of origin, 77 percent had parents and grandparents from the country they were living in: of the remainder, 7.4 percent had at least one parent/grandparent from another European Union country, 8.4 percent from a European country not in the European Union, and 7.4 percent from outside Europe. By father’s occupation, 52 percent were ‘middle class’, 43 percent ‘working class’, and 5 percent unknown. This was, and never could have been, a strictly statistically representative sample of young people, but was a range of potential views across each country: from different regions, social backgrounds and cultural groups.
Ethical approval was given by the Faculty Research Ethics Committee of my former institution (London Metropolitan University), and were (for the first phase) based on the British Educational Research Association’s Revised Ethical Guidelines for Educational Research [
48] or (for the second phase) on the subsequent set of guidelines [
49]. Consent was obtained {xe “fieldwork: ethical considerations”} from school principals, and written consent from the young people’s parents (all of those under 16, and older in some countries) and the young people themselves. Letters to parents, in the national language, explained that I was ‘making a small study of young people’s ideas … about how they feel as part of their community, region and country’, and gave details of my local colleague for further information, and specifying that they could withdraw from the study at any stage.
This study was not intended to collect quantitative data, nor to be the basis of definitive statements about ‘what young people thought’. The numbers of young people recruited, and the numbers of discussion groups, are different in different countries, and are not proportionate to the populations of the countries involved. Smaller countries had disproportionally more groups. In order to be able to say anything meaningful about an individual country, there is a necessary slew in the sampling. I used different colleagues within each country to recruit schools for each location; I used different networks to identify colleagues who might help. Clearly, my networks may include, to an extent, like-minded individuals, but not to the extent that there would be sufficient bias to make the sample of locations and schools very significantly unrepresentative. I had to depend on schools and colleges to be gatekeepers in the recruiting of individual young people, asking them to include a representation of the school’s population in terms of minorities.
My analysis {xe “analytic approach: identification of themes”} of the data was partly grounded in the data itself in an iterative, inductive and comparative process (Glaser and Straus [
50], Charmaz and Belgrave [
51]), but also drew extensively on earlier literature, including some country-specific literature, which were combined into a meta analysis (Rabiee [
52] (p. 657). These major themes were of values and issues, and the distinction between cultural and civic components of identity (drawing on Anderson [
31]; Brubaker [
53]; Bruter [
54]; Joppke [
55]); diversities and migration (Decimo and Gribaldo [
56], Kertzer [
57]); and generational difference (Lutz et al. [
58]; Miller-Idris [
59]; Fulbrook [
60]). I also examined continuities and contingent resources, regional variation within Europe and nations and hierarchies of location. These are more fully described in a more substantial work [
30].
Qualitative research carried out by a lone individual raises issues of subjectivity {xe “subjectivity: of the researcher”} and interpretation: I argue that the interpretation offered is consistent, simply because it has been carried out by a singular objectivity. But a study of identities will need to be understood in the context of the researcher’s own identity (Chadderton [
61]). Clearly, any research process—any social interaction—is asymmetrical {xe “discussion groups: assymetrical nature”}, and my presence, my appearance, my identity-as-presumed by the people I talk with will all affect the nature of the responses I obtained, and the manner in which they were given, This may be particularly true in respect of my age, my citizenship and my ethnicity. I was clearly much older than my subjects, and was aware that when they talked of their grandparent’s generation they would see me as in some way being part of that. My position as a UK citizen was another possible factor: all the fieldwork was completed before the 2016 referendum campaign on the UK’s membership of the EU began, but I was nevertheless a foreigner, which was perhaps an advantage as I would need to have things explained to me. My ethnicity, as a white European, may also have been a factor: Eddo-Lodge [
62] convincingly suggests that people of a visibly different ethnic group may have reservations in discussing {xe “discussion groups: potential issues around ‘race’”} racism {xe “racism: potential issues in discussion groups”} and ethnicity with members of a society’s dominant ethnic group. This last issue may also have affected the group dynamics of mixed ethnicity discussions.
4. Discussion
The data that has been presented above shows the diversities and similarities among these young people, but also, as should be clear, demonstrates the contingencies of their narratives: what they elect to talk about, the examples they chose to illustrate their points, their interaction and development—all are a reflection of the exact time and place at which the discussion took place. This data is not replicable, in any sense.
Nevertheless, I suggest it has value. In part, it does show the way in which they interact with current political events, and the broad lines of argument that they develop: this has been indicated, in part, by the thematic organization of the previous section. But it also demonstrates some more fundamental points about young European’s political constructions. Most of them are clearly able to operate with a variety of political identities, which they can easily and fluidly move between, without seeing any particular sense of difficulty of incongruity: they can be at one moment civic individuals debating the politics of their state, at another moment talking about their concerns as global citizens, and in another context actively considering their town or city concerns.
Their discourse also shows a particularly clear sense of identity as a generation different to their forebears. Every generation of young people does this to an extent, but in this discussion I suggest that there is a very real sense that we are witnessing a more intense and profound shift. The notion of the cohort—meaning those born within a particular period of time, rather than a particular year—is a useful analytical tool with which to examine the different experiences and societal constructs of different age groups. Lutz et al. summarize the differences between a cohort effect and a period effect:
A period effect is something affecting all ages and cohorts simultaneously, such as wars, epidemics or specific political events, while cohort effects only affect groups of people born in the same year and typically relates to factors that are associated with childhood experiences or socialization.
Lutz and his colleagues were here analyzing Eurobarometer data relating to how respondents said that they themselves were attached to Europe or their country, or to some mix of the two, and suggested that there was evidence of a generational shift towards a greater sense of being European evident in the younger cohorts and that, significantly, this persisted as the cohort grew older. This does appear to be confirmed by the qualitative data presented here (as well as by further Eurobarometer analysis conducted after Lutz et al.’s work in 2006 [
57], such as Ross [
30] (p. 20).
In Mary Fulbrook’s [
59] study of German political identities in the twentieth century,
Dissonant Lives she argues that there are not only significant differences in the ways that identities are constructed between generations, but that these are the consequence of political fractures and dissonance in German society. The age at which people experience key historical moments, such as the transitions within Germany in 1933, 1945 and 1989, can be a critical explanatory factor behind an individual or group’s ‘availability for mobilisation’ for political expression. She uses this ‘construction of a collective identity on the basis of generationally defined common experiences’ [
59] (p. 11) to explain the rise of National Socialism and the post-war politics of the Germanys. Age, she suggests, is ‘crucial at times of transition, with respect to the ways in which people can become involved in new regimes and societies’ [
59] (p. 488). She did not suggest that key formative experiences ‘necessarily produce similar
outcomes, but [that] common
challenges at a particular life stage, and …
unresolved issues’ produce generational difference (emphasis in original [
59] (p. 9)). Fulbrook draws here on Mannheim’s [
69] conception of ‘key experiences’ (
schlüsselerlebnisse) that contribute to the formation of a ‘social generation’ (
sozialen Generation). I am suggesting that similar cohort effects seem to be present in these young European’s narratives of nationalism, diversity and racism and connectivity, and these arise from significant transitions in the population, in the rise of social media, and in different generational experiences of conflict and war. This appears to be a pan-European trend, although different regions of the continent may particularly emphases specific aspects.
I have suggested [
29] that there is a cohort phenomenon in the accounts of young people in the post-2004 European Union states. These eastern-central European states went through a political fracture in the early 1990s, and the cohort born in the decade after 1989 were free of Soviet hegemony, and grew up in very different social and political conditions from their parents and grandparents. Their constructions of identities are different: period and cohort effects interact. In these states, the cohort of parents can recall the end of the Cold War and reconstruct identities in the post-communist state; and the cohort of grandparents could recall the communist state and sometimes World War II.
There have been other changes, particularly demographic changes, especially in the west European states. {xe “demographics of young people: changes in”} The dating of this is less clear cut than the fall of the Berlin Wall: it began in the 1950s in some of the former major colonial powers, rather later elsewhere, and is only beginning in the former communist states, but it has accelerated, very sharply, since the early 1990s. Not only are at least 23.3 percent of the sample of full or partial foreign descent, but 10.2 percent of them are of mixed descent, and these young people are thus in educational institutions where they are in daily contact with a demographic very different from that experienced by their grandparents, and substantially different in most cases from that that experienced by their parents {xe “diversity of population: changes in”}. They are aware of this, and many say that it means they have very different attitudes towards those of other backgrounds {xe “generation: changes: from demographic diversity”}{xe “generation: changes: from social media”}.
This perception by many young people of a generational shift, between themselves and either the generation of their parents or that of their grandparents, is also partly driven by the increase in the diversity of origin of their age cohort, particularly in the western European countries. In terms of the life experiences of the different generations, it is significant to look at the likely diversity of the school populations of the three different generations: group members, their parents and their grandparents.
The period spent in school is of particular significance, as this is when most people are most likely to experience most intensely the widest social diversity [
70,
71,
72]. Data on the diversity of European schools is not straightforward, not least historically: ethnicity data cannot be collected in France, Germany, Spain, Belgium, Denmark or Italy [
73,
74], and categories of diversity and origin are constantly shifting [
55,
56,
75]. But from the early 1990s there was a significantly diversified and larger pattern of migration both into Europe and within it, and after 2008 the number of non-EU migrants of both first generation (birth) and second generation (origin) again rose sharply, from 6.6 million in 2008 to 9.4 million in 2014 [
76,
77]. Agafiţei and Ivan estimated that by 2014, just over a fifth of all EU households included at least one person of migrant origin [
78] (p. 1). Although there has since the early 1950s been much intra-state migration (Italy from south to north, Germany from east to west), and migration between European states (Portugal to north-western Europe, Italy to Belgium, Turkey to Germany), the most significant extra-European migrations before the mid 1970s were of former colonial subjects to the UK and to France [
75,
76].
This suggests that the diversity of the population in the schools attended by these young people was very different from that of their grandparents, and quite different from their parents. Grandparents, at school from the late 1940s to the early 1960s, would in nearly all cases have attended schools that were largely monocultural, except in a very few areas of migrant settlement in the UK, France and Germany. Parents at school largely in the 1970s and 1980s would also very largely have been in schools that were monocultural, although in more urban areas in western European states there would have been some first and second generation non-European students. It was in the 1990s that diversity began to substantially increase, and over a wider area in these states, as second and third generations moved from initial areas of denser settlement to other areas, and from the early 2000s, as the number of children of mixed origin began to increase, to become the fastest growing group of the school population in the UK, France, Germany, Belgium and The Netherlands, followed by Sweden and Denmark [
72,
74,
78,
79]. Perrin et al. report that ‘among the Belgians born with at least one foreigner-born parent, there are only 23 percent with two foreigner-born parents’ [
80] (p. 205).
The young people in the discussion groups commonly offered this as an explanation: they had grown up, and in particular been to school with, a diverse cultural group. This was usually a generalized comment, as when Godofredo (♂15) in Lisboa said: ‘In school we get together with people from other races, we learn they are people too, the same—white, black. This didn’t happen with out grandparents—though my family is not racist’; and Anton (♂15) in Wien said: ‘Because I go with them [migrants] to school, I know them’. But sometimes it was more specific and personal: in Haslev, Troels (♂18) had arrived in his upper school two years earlier:
I grew up in the countryside, where we didn’t have any immigrants—I’ve never been really prejudiced against immigrants, I’d just never experienced it in the place I grew up—and then I came to the gymnasium, and suddenly there are lots of them. And at the start—I had some prejudices against some of them—because of the way they looked, they matched the way I had seen them look on the news, like the ‘immigrant criminal’ tends to be shown. I started thinking about them as what I’d seen through the media, but then, experiencing being with them as normal human beings, just ordinary people like me and you, that gave a lot to me.
The most extreme othering, where some migrant groups seemed to be considered literally ‘beyond the pale’, occurred in locations where schools were largely segregated or where there was residential segregation. This seems to illustrate Allport’s contact approach theory [
81], which hypothesizes that inter-group prejudice will be reduced under optimal conditions. ‘In particular, Allport held that reduced prejudice will result when four features of the contact situation are present: equal status between the groups in the situation; common goals; intergroup cooperation; and the support of authorities, law, or custom’ [
82] (p. 752).
A second significant cultural change over this period has been the development of the internet, the world-wide web, and the associated communication technologies of the social media. Many young people said that they thought they were using these technologies in ways that were both qualitatively and quantitatively different from many of their elders. Technological changes in the 1990s and 2000s have meant that these young people have had access to the media, in a social and interactive form, than did not exist for earlier generations. The first browsers for the world wide web became available in 1990. The size and use of the internet has risen exponentially. In January 1992—when the oldest people in this survey were born—there were just over 700,000 internet hosts worldwide. When the youngest were born, in 2004, there were 285 million. When this study began, in January 2010, there were more than 730,000 million such sites [
83] {xe “social media: generational use of”}. Over the period of data collection, the number of households with internet access in the EU–28 countries rose from 70 percent to 85 percent, but of households with dependent children, 94 percent had access by 2014. By 2014, 90 percent of all 16- to 20-year-olds were reporting at least daily use of the internet (by 2016 only 57 percent of 55- to 74-year-olds were reporting at least weekly use) [
84,
85]. These young people constitute the first generation to have such access, and—as they described in chapter four—they recognized the significance of this: they made a greater use of, and had a greater interest in, social media technology than older people; they were more able and willing to use it; they were consequently in greater contact with others in different countries; and had a different global perspective. As Marcelino (♂14, Portugal) said, ‘We were kind of born with a cell-phone in our hands.’
Young people saw the way they used social media as another significant way in which they differed from older people. The differences they perceived seemed to centre around four interlocked themes: the extent to which older people used the social media; their difficulties in using these technologies; their hesitation about such new media sources; and their (perhaps consequential) lesser understanding of a globalised society.
Firstly, it was sometimes asserted that some older people did not possess technological devices, and were not interested in them. ‘My parents’, said Marine (♀13) in a small south Italian town, ‘live differently: they don’t use the technology, the mobiles, the tablets and so on.’ Narcisa (♀16) in Spain said: ‘Older people don’t want more information—they are happier with what they know and with what television says, and they don’t want to contrast information. The internet is bigger, there is a lot of information, there are more sources.’
Secondly, those older people who did use the technology were unable or unwilling to fully exploit its capabilities. Rinus (♂16) in The Netherlands claimed: ‘A lot of older people have a hard time adapting to all the new technologies that are being brought on the market. The younger generations have been brought up with those things.’ In Portugal, Agostinho (♂15) said that ‘older people tend to stick to their own ways—we multi-task, do various things with this one device.’ Another young Portuguese, Davi (♂14), claimed that ‘old people usually don’t know how to use the internet—so they think it’s bad, because they don’t know how to use it.’
Thirdly, younger people were more open-minded, to the world and to new technologies. The argument was to a degree circular, openness to the world stimulating openness to new media, and vice versa. In Norway, Lovinda (♀17) thought that ‘younger people are more open for it—today we have all this social media, and we talk to people from all over the world … so we are more open for people coming into our country.’ In The Netherlands, Mieke (♀15) said that digital media meant that ‘I see a lot of different sides to every story. Older people just know their side, and that’s right, so they stick with that.’ In Italy, Gaudenzio (♂16) said ‘in the past people … voted without being properly informed. Now the internet gives you a wider idea of the political system—in the past there was just the news on TV.’
Young people felt that their global perspective was a consequence of their use of social media, and it also meant that they used these media to become even more aware. In Norway, Gøran (♂18) linked social media use with global awareness and anti-racism: ‘We have grown up with the social media, and it’s a lot easier for us to get to know people from the other side of the world, to see what’s happening in other places than Norway. A multicultural society [means] we learn [about] other cultures, and that’s something that our grandparents didn’t experience—my grandfather is very, very racist—he has a lot of strong opinions, because he doesn’t know any better.’
News media consumption is now, as O’Loughlin and Gillespie argue, ‘intrinsic to living and doing citizenship. News media consumption enables young people to acquire discursive competences in discussing news events and, as a consequence, develop an understanding of what politics is’ [
86] (p. 199). For this generation, this includes electronic and personalized media sources and, for many, the superdiversity of European society.