Democratic Frustration: Concept, Dimensions and Behavioural Consequences
Abstract
:1. Heterogeneity in Democratic Crises
2. Mapping Democratic Frustration Vis-à-Vis Other Models of Democratic Crises
2.1. Existing Models
2.2. The Psychological Concept of Frustration
3. Democratic Frustration = Expectation * Perceived Delivery Deficit
3.1. Democratic Frustration and Other Expectations-Delivery Gap Combinations
3.2. Dimensions of Democratic Frustration
- Ideological, which pertains to the existence of a congruent offer to respond to a citizens’ substantive preferences,
- Institutional, when it comes to the existence of adequate processes capable of effectively and transparently achieving democratic linkage, and
- Political, which relates to agency, political personnel, and the integrity of their behaviour, ethos, motivations, and democratic service.
3.3. Consequences of Democratic Frustration
4. Methodology, Data, and Operationalization Strategy
5. Analysis
5.1. Scoping Democratic Frustration
5.2. Stability and Evolution of Democratic Frustration over Time
5.3. Behavioural Consequences of Democratic Frustration
6. Conclusions: Understanding Desire to Understand Disappointment
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Appendix A.1. Methodological Details of Surveys
Appendix A.2. Survey Questions:
- [0 = not important at all… 10 = extremely important]
- Represented by people whose ideas are close to mine
- That politicians are genuinely interested in listening to citizens
- That politicians are genuinely more interested in what is best for citizens rather than what is best for themselves
- That I have a genuine choice between a range of political alternatives
- That politicians are transparent and honest
- That politicians consider the long-term interest of citizens
- That I am involved in the democratic process
- That if citizens are unhappy with the way they are governed, they can get rid of leaders who have not performed well
- That I feel respected by politicians
- [0 = not good at all… 10: extremely good]
- Represented by people whose ideas are close to mine
- That politicians are genuinely interested in listening to citizens
- That politicians are genuinely more interested in what is best for citizens rather than what is best for themselves
- That I have a genuine choice between a range of political alternatives
- That politicians are transparent and honest
- That politicians consider the long-term interest of citizens
- That I am involved in the democratic process
- That if citizens are unhappy with the way they are governed, they can get rid of leaders who have not performed well
- That I feel respected by politicians
Model | Collaboration | Abstention | Peaceful Demonstration | Radical Party | Violent Demonstration | Revolution | Leave Country |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | – | −0.13 ** | −0.18 ** | – | −0.20 ** | −0.23 ** | −0.27 ** |
Gender | – | – | −0.06 ** | −0.10 ** | −0.09 ** | −0.08 ** | – |
Social Cat | 0.05 * | −0.11 ** | – | – | – | −0.05 * | 0.05 * |
Hostility | 0.12 ** | – | 0.18 ** | 0.16 ** | 0.19 ** | 0.20 ** | 0.15 ** |
Sympathy | – | 0.22 ** | – | 0.07 ** | 0.17 ** | 0.14 ** | 0.13 ** |
Frustration(ideo) | – | 0.14 ** | – | – | – | – | – |
Frustration (inst) | – | – | 0.14 ** | 0.16 ** | 0.22 ** | 0.25 ** | 0.15 ** |
Frustration (pol) | – | – | – | −0.10 * | −0.26 ** | −0.20 ** | −0.10 * |
R2 | 0.03 | 0.09 | 0.08 | 0.07 | 0.16 | 0.16 | 0.14 |
Appendix A.3. Factor Loadings
- Three items entered.
- Eigenvalue:
- Component 1: 2.28 (75.92% of cumulative variance)
- [Eigenvalue of component 2 is 0.37]
- Component matrix loadings (see questionnaire sample for variables description):
- Institutional 1: 0.86
- Institutional 2: 0.87
- Institutional 3: 0.87
- Four items entered.
- Eigenvalue:
- Component 1: 3.38 (84.45% of cumulative variance)
- [Eigenvalue of component 2 is 0.21]
- Component matrix loadings (see questionnaire sample for variables description):
- Political 1: 0.92
- Political 2: 0.91
- Political 3: 0.92
- Political 4: 0.92
References
- Sorensen, R. Democratic frustration of vast majorities. Political Stud. 1982, 30, 272–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brooks, J.E. Democratic frustration in the Anglo-American polities: A quantification of inconsistency between mass public opinion and public policy. West. Political Q. 1985, 38, 250–261. [Google Scholar]
- Kim, J. On Frustration: Toward a Theory of a Democratic Politics of Perseverance. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Bertsou, E. Analysing Attitudes of Political Distrust in Europe. Ph.D. Thesis, London School of Economics and Political Science, London, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Newton, K. Trust, social capital, civil society, and democracy. Int. Political Sci. Rev. 2001, 22, 201–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mishler, W.; Rose, R. Trust, distrust and skepticism: Population evaluations of civil and political institutions in post-communist societies. J. Politics 1997, 59, 418–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Seligman, A.B. The Problem of Trust; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Kaase, M.; Newton, K.; Scarbrough, E. A look at the beliefs in governments study. PS Political Sci. Politics 1996, 29, 226–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Norris, P. (Ed.) Critical Citizens: Global Support for Democratic Government; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Norris, P. Democratic Deficit: Critical Citizens Revisited; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Torcal, M.; Montero, J.R. (Eds.) Political Disaffection in Contemporary Democracies: Social Capital, Institutions and Politics; Routledge: London, UK, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- LeDuc, L.; Niemi, R.; Norris, P. (Eds.) Comparing Democracies: Elections and Voting in Global Perspective; Sage: London, UK, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Franklin, M.N. Voter Turnout and the Dynamics of Electoral Competition in Established Democracies Since 1945; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Scarrow, S. Parties and Their Members; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Katz, P.; Mair, P. How Parties Organise? Sage: London, UK, 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Pharr, S.; Putnam, R. (Eds.) Disaffected Democracies? In What’s Troubling the Trilateral Countries? Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Bruter, M.; Harrison, S. Mapping Extreme Right Ideology: An Empirical Geography of the European Extreme Right; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Kimberlee, R. ‘Why don’t British young people vote at general elections? J. Youth Stud. 2002, 5, 85–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dalton, R.; Welzel, C. (Eds.) The Civic Culture Transformed from Allegiant to Assertive Citizens; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Bruter, M.; Harrison, S. Inside the Mind of a Voter: A New Approach to Electoral Psychology in Six Democracies; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 2020; in press. [Google Scholar]
- Underwood, B.J. Experimental Psychology: An introduction; Appelton-Century-Crofts: New York, NY, USA, 1949. [Google Scholar]
- Lacan, J. Le seminaire. In Book 4 La relation d’objet (1956–1957); Paris: Seuil, France, 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Smuts, A. The desire-frustration theory of suspense. J. Aesthet. Art Crit. 2008, 66, 281–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blair, R.J.R. Psychopathy, frustration, and reactive aggression: The role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Br. J. Psychol. 2010, 101, 383–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosenzweig, S. An Outline of Frustration Theory. In Personality and the Behavior Disorders; Hunt, J.M., Ed.; Ronald Press: New York, NY, USA, 1944. [Google Scholar]
- Berkowitz, L. Frustration-aggression hypothesis: Examination and reformulation. Psychol. Bull. 1989, 106, 59–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Crosby, F. A model of egoistical relative deprivation. Psychol. Rev. 1976, 83, 85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fox, S.; Spector, P.E. A model of work frustration–aggression. J. Organ. Behav. 1999, 20, 915–931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berkowitz, L. Aggression: A Social Psychological Analysis; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 1962. [Google Scholar]
- Lockwood, J.L.; Roll, S. Effects of fantasy behavior, level of fantasy predisposition, age, and sex on direction of aggression in young children. J. Genet. Psychol. 1980, 136, 255–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Perlman, S.B.; Luna, B.; Hein, T.C.; Huppert, T.J. fNIRS evidence of prefrontal regulation of frustration in early childhood. NeuroImage 2014, 85, 326–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Crossman, A.M.; Sullivan, M.W.; Hitchcock, D.M.; Lewis, M. When frustration is repeated: Behavioral and emotion responses during extinction over time. Emotion 2009, 9, 92–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Maslow, A.H. Deprivation, threat, and frustration. Psychol. Rev. 1941, 48, 364–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kulik, J.A.; Brown, R. Frustration, attribution of blame, and aggression. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1979, 15, 183–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yuan, J.; Ding, N.; Liu, Y.; Yang, J. Unconscious emotion regulation: Nonconscious reappraisal decreases emotion-related physiological reactivity during frustration. Cogn. Emot. 2015, 29, 1042–1053. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carr, A.J.; Gibson, B.; Robinson, P.G. Is quality of life determined by expectations or experience? BMJ 2001, 322, 1240–1243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rogers, J.; Ward, C. Expectation-experience discrepancies and psychological adjustment during cross-cultural re-entry. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 1993, 17, 185–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sartori, G. Democratic Theory; Praeger: Santa Barbara, CA, USA, 1965. [Google Scholar]
- Dahl, R.A. A Preface to Democratic Theory; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Katz, R.S. Democracy and Elections; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Dennis, J. Support for the Institution of Elections by the Mass Public. Am. Political Sci. Rev. 1970, 64, 819–835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miller, W.E.; Stokes, D.E. Constituency influence in Congress. Am. Political Sci. Rev. 1963, 57, 45–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eulau, H.; Karps, P. The puzzle of representation: Specifying components of responsiveness. Legis. Stud. Q. 1977, 2, 233–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Przeworski, A.; Stokes, S. Democracy, Accountability, and Representation; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Gibson, J.L.; Caldeira, G.A. The legitimacy of transnational legal institutions: Compliance, support, and the European Court of Justice. Am. J. Political Sci. 1995, 39, 459–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berry, C.R.; Howell, W.G. Accountability and local elections: Rethinking retrospective voting. J. Politics 2007, 69, 844–858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosema, M. The Sincere Vote: A Psychological Study of Voting. PhD. Thesis, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherland, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Sargent, S.S. Reaction to frustration—A critique and hypothesis. Psychol. Rev. 1948, 55, 108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bandura, A. Aggression: A Social Learning Analysis; Prentice-Hall: Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA, 1973. [Google Scholar]
- Martin, R.C.; Dahlen, E.R. Irrational beliefs and the experience and expression of anger. J. Ration. Emot. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 2004, 22, 3–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dollard, J.; Miller, N.E.; Doob, L.E.; Mowrer, H.; Sears, R.R. Frustration and Aggression; Yale University Press: New Haven, CT, USA, 1939. [Google Scholar]
1 | Note that the data available in this article does not permit the test of individual level stability. Only the aggregate level stability will be tested. |
Institutional Dimension | Perceived Delivery Deficit | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Expectations | Low | Medium | High | |
High | Satisfaction | Criticality | Frustration | |
Low | [Will recalibrate] * | Apathy/indifference | Cynicism |
Reaction Level | Withdrawal | Anger | Aggression |
---|---|---|---|
Soft | Abstention | Peaceful demonstration | Violent demonstration |
Hard | Leaving the country | Extremist vote | Revolution |
© 2020 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Harrison, S. Democratic Frustration: Concept, Dimensions and Behavioural Consequences. Societies 2020, 10, 19. https://doi.org/10.3390/soc10010019
Harrison S. Democratic Frustration: Concept, Dimensions and Behavioural Consequences. Societies. 2020; 10(1):19. https://doi.org/10.3390/soc10010019
Chicago/Turabian StyleHarrison, Sarah. 2020. "Democratic Frustration: Concept, Dimensions and Behavioural Consequences" Societies 10, no. 1: 19. https://doi.org/10.3390/soc10010019
APA StyleHarrison, S. (2020). Democratic Frustration: Concept, Dimensions and Behavioural Consequences. Societies, 10(1), 19. https://doi.org/10.3390/soc10010019