Next Article in Journal
Sports in Natural Forests: A Systematic Review of Environmental Impact and Compatibility for Readability
Previous Article in Journal
Physical Fitness Profiles of Young Female Team Sport Athletes from Portuguese Rural Settings: A Cross-Sectional Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Social Cognitive Theory and Physical Activity: Examining Gender-Based Prediction Patterns and Theoretical Validity

Department of Education, Saarland University, 66123 Saarbrücken, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sports 2025, 13(8), 249; https://doi.org/10.3390/sports13080249
Submission received: 16 June 2025 / Revised: 11 July 2025 / Accepted: 24 July 2025 / Published: 29 July 2025

Abstract

This study explored gender-specific nuances in the applicability of Social Cognitive Theory (SCT) to predict physical activity behavior. This study aimed to determine whether similar or different prediction patterns emerge for men and women, particularly emphasizing the tenability of the SCT model’s theoretical assumptions across gender. Six hundred fifty-four participants (58.1% women, 41.1% men) completed two validated questionnaires at separate time points (t1 = social cognitive and demographic variables; t2 = physical activity behavior). We employed a multigroup Structural Equation Model (SEM) to examine the validity of the theoretical assumptions and the influence of gender. The results suggest that SCT’s theoretical assumptions hold true for men and women, indicated by a highly satisfactory fit of the SEM despite the variance explained being small (R2women = 11.9%, R2men = 7.3%). However, the importance of the specific theoretical paths and the underlying mechanisms of action might differ between genders, and the interplay of the social and cognitive variables to predict physical activity may vary significantly for men and women. The use of SCT can be recommended for explaining and predicting physical activity behavior, although gender-specific differences in the underlying theoretical relationships should be taken into consideration when designing interventions or when being used to explain physical activity behavior.

1. Introduction

Albert Bandura’s social cognitive theory (SCT) is among the most prominent psychological theories on human behavior [1]. It is notable for its comprehensive scope, encompassing almost the entire field of psychological factors. The theory posits that personal, behavioral, and environmental factors interact to influence an individual’s behaviors, facilitating a holistic examination of the socio-cognitive factors associated with human action. SCT postulates that a dynamic interplay between these factors shapes individual behaviors. The theory places a particular emphasis on self-efficacy (the confidence in the ability to successfully perform physical activity, even when facing obstacles) and outcome expectations (expectations concerning the potential positive and negative consequences of being physically active). The theory also encompasses goal setting (the establishment of explicit, attainable physical activity objectives) and sociostructural factors (environmental elements such as facility availability, social support, and time constraints) [2]. According to the theory, self-efficacy is the most influential predictor of physical activity behavior. A positive linkage has been demonstrated between high self-efficacy and expectations of positive outcomes, the establishment of more concrete goals, and an increased level of participation. Individuals who possess high self-efficacy demonstrate a propensity to persist in physical activity despite the presence of adversity. Figure 1 illustrates the theoretical model.
A substantial body of literature has tested and confirmed several aspects of SCT in relation to behavior in both specific populations and selected domains, as well as in the context of physical activity behavior [3,5,6,7,8,9].
However, SCT has rarely been subjected to comprehensive testing and modeling following Bandura’s original intentions. Studies have often only examined selected elements; this was critiqued by Young et al. [9] in a review of studies on SCT pertaining to physical activity, with the authors concluding that “the majority of SCT research has focused solely upon self-efficacy or examined self-efficacy in combination with only one or two other variables” (p. 985). Thus, it would appear to be important to examine the theory as Bandura originally intended, including all four constructs with their assumed direct, indirect, and total effects. Moreover, evidence supporting the power of SCT to predict and explain physical activity has primarily been demonstrated for specific samples, such as those with diseases like multiple sclerosis (e.g., [6,10]), type 2 diabetes (e.g., [8]), and severe mental illness (e.g., [11]). The theory has also been applied to understand the experiences of individuals with cancer (e.g., [5,12]) and those dealing with obesity (e.g., [13]), as well as Crohn’s disease [14]. The existing literature on SCT in regard to physical activity behavior concerning healthy adult populations has been relatively sparse [3,9,13].
Although a more nuanced comprehension of the behavioral antecedents in these particular samples is undoubtedly a point of interest, it is imperative to acknowledge that the phenomenon of physical inactivity is not exclusive to those samples. As evidenced by Guthold’s [15] and Strain’s [16] comprehensive reviews, physical inactivity is a pervasive global issue. The studies revealed that approximately 30% of adults globally fail to attain the physical activity level recommended by the World Health Organization (WHO) [16] for adults to be at least 150 min moderately physically active or 75 min vigorously physically active per week. The objective of these recommendations is to promote substantial health benefits [17].
It can be reasonably deduced that investigating SCT with a focus on a holistic model test in a large population of healthy individuals may be of significant interest. Promising results have emerged from other recent studies with objectives aligned with those of the present study, leading to the assumption that the theoretical assumptions of the model in question satisfactorily reflect reality.
However, prior research findings concerning physical activity and SCT elements have also indicated the existence of notable gender disparities, and the importance of a gender-sensitive approach is currently being discussed [18]. A substantial body of literature has documented the existence of gender differences in self-efficacy related to physical activity, with men having stronger self-efficacy beliefs than women [19,20,21] and self-efficacy being differently related to behavior for men than for women [21]. Anderson et al. [22] documented differences in the outcome expectations of men and women. Additionally, studies have indicated that the correlations between outcome expectations and behavior are gender specific. For instance, Trost et al. [23] demonstrated that outcome expectations were a significant predictor of physical activity behavior for men but not for women. In regard to sociostructural factors, a definitive conclusion concerning gender differences remains elusive, although some research has indicated that barriers may impact women more than men [24]. The contradictory findings observed in this domain may be contingent upon the conceptualization employed [25,26]. Similarly, the existing evidence on gender-specific objectives is not yet sufficiently clear to permit drawing definitive conclusions. Conversely, clear gender differences have been demonstrated for physical activity, with studies consistently demonstrating variations in men’s and women’s participation, intensity, level, and preferred types of exercise [27,28]. Not only do men report and demonstrate higher physical activity levels than women, but they also report engaging in a higher intensity of physical activity. Such differences remain largely consistent throughout the lifespan [16,23,29,30,31,32].
Therefore, alongside the general validity of the theoretical assumptions, their universality should also be tested, namely that the model’s theoretical assumptions are tenable across gender and that the theory can be used equally effectively for men and women to explain and predict physical activity behavior.
Drawing on recent research results [3], we hypothesize that SCT—when modeled as Bandura proposed—fits the data and describes the theoretically stated relations between self-efficacy, outcome expectations, sociostructural factors, goal setting, and physical activity in its entirety (H1). We thereby aim to replicate the hypothesis of Egele et al. [3]. Furthermore, given the gender differences reported for multiple SCT elements [21,23,24], we investigate the model fit individually for men and women using a multigroup Structural Equation Model (SEM) (H2). We assume that the theoretically stated relations between self-efficacy, outcome expectations, sociostructural factors, goal setting, and physical activity are valid for men and women separately and explore potential differences in the prediction patterns.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Several methods were used to recruit participants, including the placement of notices on the campus of Saarland University and other cultural centers in Germany and online advertisements. Prior to analysis, we excluded data from any subjects who did not complete both questionnaires (n = 36). In addition, the data were checked for response time and response pattern usage, which led to the exclusion of two more participants. The final sample comprised 654 German participants (58.9% women, 41.1% men) aged between 18 and 68 (M = 31.90, SD = 13.18). The mean age of the 385 women was 30.40 years (SD = 12.66), while the mean age of the 269 men was 34.04 years (SD = 13.66). Participants reported an average of 2685.44 metabolic equivalent of task (MET) minutes of total physical activity per week (SD = 2630.32). This amount was comparable to the physical activity reported in similar studies [3,30], yet clearly exceeded the WHO’s recommended physical activity levels, which are equivalent to approximately 600 MET minutes [17].

2.2. Instruments

The scales for measuring the SCT components were taken from a validated German scale [33], allowing a specific evaluation of the components of SCT within the context of physical activity. The scales comprise 18 items in total and have demonstrated adequate psychometric properties [33]. Five items measured self-efficacy, and the subscale’s reliability was α = 0.912 (McDonald’s Omega = 0.904). Five items were used to assess outcome expectations, and this subscale’s reliability was α = 0.906 (McDonald’s Omega = 0.801). The subscale for measuring goals consisted of four items, and its reliability was α = 0.889 (McDonald’s Omega = 0.884). Finally, sociostructural factors were assessed by four items, with the subscale’s reliability at α = 0.751 (McDonald’s Omega = 0.604).
Physical activity was measured using seven items of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire—Short Form in German (IPAQ-SF, [34]), which assesses general physical activity over the previous seven days. The questionnaire uses an open response format to assess the number of days and the average time (hours and minutes) spent on physical activity and sitting. We selected the IPAQ-SF because of its parsimony, good psychometric qualities, and implementation in multiple existing studies [35,36,37]. According to the scoring protocol, the MET as a measure of physical activity was calculated as a weighted sum of vigorous and moderate physical activity and time spent walking. The resulting score was divided by 1000 to obtain scores of comparable ranges for SCT and physical activity.
Demographics, gender, and age were assessed using open questions.

2.3. Procedure

This study was conducted in compliance with the ethical standards of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Empirical Human and Economic Sciences of Saarland University (Approval 24–32) on 18 November 2024. All subjects provided written informed consent prior to data collection. All hypotheses were specified before data collection.
This study was conducted entirely online, with the questionnaires implemented using SoSci Survey (Version 3.7.06) [38]. In the first questionnaire, the participants answered the scales to assess the SCT components and provided demographic information. They provided information on their physical activity one week later.

2.4. Data Analyses

R (Version 4.4.0, R Core Team, 2020, Vienna, Austria), specifically the packages Lavaan [39] and psych [40], was used for the calculations. The model depicted in Figure 1 was set up as an SEM with latent variables, and physical activity was included as a manifest variable. We implemented the MLM estimator with the Satorra–Bentler-scaled χ2-test statistic and robust standard errors, as was also carried out previously in similar studies [3], since some variables exhibited a skewed distribution. We included the indirect and total effects through defined parameters.
To test Hypothesis 1, we considered the model’s fit in the whole sample. The first step examined each latent variable’s measurement model with a single-factor confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). The second step included a CFA with all four latent variables to evaluate the entire measurement model’s fit. The fit of the model was evaluated following Schermelleh-Engel et al.’s [41] criteria: the χ2-test statistic should be greater than p = 0.05, RMSEA should be smaller than 0.05 with a p-value greater than 0.10 for the test of close fit, CFI should be greater than 0.97, and SRMR should be smaller than 0.05. In the third step, the SEM was estimated and evaluated against the above criteria, hereafter called SEM1. Standard fit measures of the SEM do not allow an assessment of the theoretically stated relations only in the structural part, since they simultaneously assess both the fit of the measurement and the structural parts. As such, we followed Lance et al.’s [42] recommendations; Lance et al. [42] recommended looking at the C9 and C10 indexes to evaluate the structural model. As also described in our prior study [3], the C9 index is an index of goodness-of-fit, ranging from 0 to 1. A value closer to 1 indicates a better fit of the structural model. C9 reflects a model comparison of the theoretical model and a model with no relations between the latent variables. The C10 index is a badness-of-fit index, also ranging between 0 and 1. A value closer to 0 indicates a better fit of the structural model. C10 reflects a model comparison of the theoretical model and a model with no assumed paths between latent variables. We used formulas based on the models’ χ2-test statistics, as provided by Lance et al. [42], to calculate C9 and C10, hereafter called C91 and C101. We modeled a CFA containing the covariances of the latent variables with MET to estimate the structural model part only with the traditional fit indices.
We calculated and tested the direct, indirect, and total effects with a nominal significance level of α = 0.05.
To test Hypothesis 2, we first tested the measurement invariance between men and women. Following the recommendations of Guenole et al. [43] for comparing the regression coefficients, the measurement invariance up to the level of metric measurement invariance was established. After establishing measurement invariance across groups, we ran a multigroup SEM (hereafter referred to as SEM2) and employed a Wald test to test for overall group differences by simultaneously comparing all direct paths across the two groups.
Fit measures for the two groups were evaluated against Schermelleh-Engel et al.’s [41] criteria. To assess the validity of the theoretically stated relations for men and women separately, we evaluated C9 and C10 indices (C92M, C92W, C102M, and C102W). Two CFAs were modeled, containing the covariances of the latent variables with MET (hereafter referred to as partial structural model2M and partial structural model2W) to assess the structural model part only with traditional fit indices.
Again, the direct, indirect, and total effects for men and women separately were calculated and tested, with a nominal significance level of α = 0.05.
Data are available on request.

3. Results

Table 1 displays the single-factor measurement models and the CFA with all four latent variables, demonstrating a satisfactory model fit for each model.
For Hypothesis 1, the SEM fit was good, as Table 1 reports. The partial SEM, in which the structural part was separated from the measurement model, also indicated a good fit. The C9 and C10 indices were 0.988 and 0.012, respectively, which indicated a very good fit for the structural model part. The results support Hypothesis 1, confirming that the theoretical assumptions of SCT were valid in the overall sample. Table 1 depicts the direct, indirect, and total effects of the overall model.
For Hypothesis 2, we first established configural invariance to test measurement invariance. The fit indices of the model were close to Schermelleh-Engel et al.’s [41] criteria: χ2 (248) = 385.275, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.972, RMSEA = 0.045, 90% CI [0.036–0.054], pclose = 0.806, SRMR = 0.061. Therefore, we assumed configural invariance as given. Then, the factor loadings were constrained to be equal across groups to test metric invariance. The model fit was good: χ2 (262) = 398.494, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.972, RMSEA = 0.044, 90% CI [0.033–0.047], pclose = 0.875, SRMR = 0.064. Comparing the two models’ fit, the difference in χ2 was not significant: χ2 (14) = 13.14, p = 0.515, and the change in CFI was ΔCFI < 0.001. Based on Meade et al.’s [44] suggested threshold of ΔCFI = 0.002 and the non-significant difference in χ2, we concluded that metric invariance was supported by our data.
We then examined the model fit of the multigroup SEM; the model fit was good, as depicted in Table 1. For men and women separately, the fit of the structural part was very good. The C9M index was 0.997, the C9W index was 0.975, the C10M index was 0.003, and the C10W index was 0.025. Both partial SEMs also indicated a good fit for the structural models.
A comparison of all direct paths of SCT across the two groups indicated no significant differences, Wald (8) = 12.718, p = 0.122. Given the satisfactory fit of the structural model for men and women and since there were no significant differences in the Wald test, we concluded that SCT’s theoretically stated relations appear to hold for both men and women. However, as Table 2 depicts, some paths appear to be only significant for women: sociostructural factors on goals (b2), outcome expectations on goals (c2), and goals on physical activity (c3). Figure 2 and Figure 3 illustrate the direct paths for men and women, with significant paths marked in blue and red, respectively.

4. Discussion

This study aimed to replicate the validity of the theoretical assumptions of SCT and investigate whether these assumptions are tenable for both men and women.
In this section, we will first briefly discuss the findings of the replication of Egele et al. [3] and then discuss the applicability and tenability of the theoretical assumptions of SCT to men and women.
In contrast to the findings of Egele et al. [3], the present study found that all hypothesized direct and indirect paths were statistically significant. This outcome is aligned with the predictions of Bandura’s theoretical framework. Only two pathways were not significant in the present study: the indirect effect of self-efficacy via sociostructural factors on goals and the indirect path of self-efficacy via sociostructural factors and goals on behavior. The underlying reasons for this phenomenon are likely attributable to the pathways’ intricate nature. They will be further elucidated later, as the pathways will be examined independently for men and women.
Thus, the findings of the present study indicated that a greater number of paths were significant compared to those of Egele et al. [3]. This discrepancy may be attributed to the sample size and its associated statistical power. In the present study, the number of participants exceeded three times the number of subjects in Egele et al.’s [3] study. Nonetheless, both studies support the fundamental assumption that SCT’s theoretical assumptions appear valid.
Furthermore, the findings of the present study confirm that the theoretical assumptions of SCT appear valid and applicable to both men and women. However, it appears that some paths may be more pertinent for women than men. Self-efficacy and outcome expectations appear to be of importance for both men and women. However, it appears that for men, the model’s complexity may be reduced to a direct path of self-efficacy on behavior, which is seemingly of great importance, while other paths seem less relevant. For instance, the influence of sociostructural factors on goals seems to be a notable phenomenon for women but not for men. Similarly, the path from outcome expectations to goals was significant for women, but not for men, as was the path from goals to behavior. Thus, for men, it appears that only self-efficacy plays a role in physical activity behavior, while for women, the complex path network seems to be more appropriate. This raises the question of what the underlying causes of these observed differences may be and invites a closer look.
It seems that men may possess greater self-efficacy than women; at least, this is suggested by the literature demonstrating that men have stronger self-efficacy beliefs than women, both generally and in relation to physical activity [19,20,21]. This is further supported by recent findings on adolescent populations, which have revealed strong gender-specific emotional and behavioral profiles that influence physical activity levels [45]. It is similarly conceivable that the assessment of SCT constructs itself exerts a somewhat limiting effect in this regard. For instance, it may be that women are more likely to have physical outcome expectations concerning the outcomes in question, which include health, good looks, and fitness, while men are more likely to have affective outcome expectations, such as enjoyment of sport and exercise [46]. However, contradictory research results have also been reported [47], indicating the necessity of clarification in future research. The scale employed in this study predominantly inquired about physical, rather than affective, outcome expectations [33], potentially contributing to the observed discrepancy in the outcome expectations guiding action between men and women. To obtain a more nuanced understanding of gender differences, it may be necessary to employ more differentiated data collection instruments, as was carried out by Gellert et al. [48]. This could also help ascertain whether self-efficacy expectations are indeed the primary motivating factor for men’s actions. The argument that a more differentiated assessment may be beneficial is further supported by the apparent validity of the theoretical model’s assumptions regarding the complex interplay of social and cognitive factors for men. While the present study could not provide definitive support that these complex pathways guide physical activity in men, the more differentiated recording of SCT components appears to open a meaningful new research avenue. Given the rarity of comprehensive testing of the theory and the lack of separate examinations of men and women [9], it would be beneficial to enhance their efficacy by pursuing this line of research further, particularly in the context of Social Cognitive Theory-based interventions. If it is indeed determined that self-efficacy is the primary determinant of behavior for men, whereas it is a multi-faceted interplay of social and cognitive influences for women, then adaptations to the intervention may be valuable.
Furthermore, the examination of the variance in physical activity that can be ascribed to social and cognitive variables merits closer scrutiny. Comparing the current study with others on SCT in the context of physical activity reveals a low variance level explained in the present study. Overall, 10% of the variance in physical activity was explained by SCT; social cognitive variables explained 7% of the variance in physical activity for men and 12% for women. The discrepancy between the intended and actual variance explained can be partially attributed to the intention–behavior gap. The discrepancy between individuals’ self-set goals and actual behaviors has been extensively documented in prior research, as exemplified by Feil et al. [49] in the context of physical activity. This was also evident in the current study, wherein the explained variance in goals was significantly higher overall (31%) as well as for men (27%) and women (37%) than in behavior. In meta-analyses (e.g., [49]), the correlation between goals and behavior has been observed to be at the random level, indicating that the issue of limited variance explained in the criterion may not be a limitation of the present study but, rather, a phenomenon warranting consideration within a broader context.
However, with this little share of variance explained, the theory does not meet Baranowski et al.’s [50] recommendations of R2 ≥ 0.30 to be considered a useful framework for intervention designs. The question thus arises as to why less variance could be explained in the current study than in prior ones.
This may also be attributed to our conceptualization of the construct of “physical activity.” Ours was a relatively broad conceptualization, as is common practice in many population-based surveys (e.g., [15,16]), to encompass as many individuals and diverse exercise behaviors as possible. SCT may explain a greater proportion of the variance if the behavior in question were more clearly defined. For instance, if physical activity was operationalized as a weekly frequency of ≥30 min exercise sessions and SCT scales were adapted to this criterion, there might be a higher correspondence and, subsequently, a stronger relationship. Consequently, we recommend that this study be replicated with an alternative physical activity criterion to further elucidate the underlying mechanisms.
In light of this study’s findings, it remains debatable whether SCT is a suitable basis for interventions, since the model’s theoretical assumptions are valid for both men and women and, thus, are both tenable and applicable to a diverse demographic. Conversely, the low variance explained may require a reconsideration of the unreserved recommendation of the theory.

Limitations

One potential limitation of this study is the recording of the constructs, including social cognitive variables and physical activity behavior. This study replicated the procedure established by Egele et al. [3] and employed the same study design and questionnaires. However, there are several points worthy of further discussion.
In terms of the recording of the social cognitive variables, it is noteworthy that we utilized a validated questionnaire to assess SCT. This approach is more rigorous than that employed in many studies using non-validated questionnaires, a practice that has drawn critique from Young et al. [9], among others. However, the questionnaires are economic yet relatively brief, precluding the formulation of more precise statements about construct elements, including, for example, different types of outcome expectations. It might be conceivable that specific facets demonstrate gender-based differences; however, the questionnaire employed cannot ascertain this. The incorporation of additional facets of the SCT constructs is also conceivable concerning sociostructural factors, such as social support from family and friends, living conditions, and governmental support. In regard to objectives, assessing goal strength and specificity may be of additional value.
Furthermore, in regard to the recording of physical activity behavior, the assessment employed is open to criticism. Given the lack of consensus regarding the optimal method for recording physical activity behavior [51], our approach was guided by larger population comparison studies (e.g., [15,16]). Young et al.’s [9] meta-analysis also demonstrated that the assessment of physical activity behavior, subjectively or objectively, does not significantly impact the explained variance. However, it is essential to note that the recording of physical activity behavior through retrospective self-reports can be susceptible to potential biases and may not reach the desired validity, as previously evidenced by Egele et al. [52] and Straßburg et al. [53]. More specifically, social desirability [52] and recall bias [54] may result in bias in self-reported health behaviors. Thus, future studies might benefit from the inclusion of more objective assessment methods, such as accelerometers.
A further limitation of this study is the temporal spacing of the two data collection points. While two data collection points are already a step forward from the simultaneous assessment of the elements of SCT and physical activity—as previously conducted in a number of studies [9]—the evaluation of the elements of SCT and physical activity one week apart may still have restricted causal inference. Future studies may benefit from implementing a more longitudinal approach to enable a more thorough examination of causal inference.

5. Conclusions

The objective of this study was to assess the applicability of SCT for men and women and determine whether a similar or different gender-specific prediction pattern emerges. The results indicate that the theoretical assumptions appear to be valid for both the overall sample and for men and women separately. Consequently, the use of SCT can be recommended in the context of physical activity; however, potential gender-specific differences in the underlying theoretical relations should be considered, and the low variance explained may require a reconsideration of the unreserved recommendation of the theory. The present study is, thereby, positioned as basic research with the purpose of investigating causal relationships between SCT elements in relation to general physical activity. Pursuant to this study’s findings, which are partly exploratory in nature, subsequent research endeavors may concentrate on implementing the results within the domain of public health interventions to address the issue of physical inactivity. According to the findings of this study—though partially exploratory—it may be appropriate for men to primarily concentrate on sources of self-efficacy in order to increase self-efficacy, while a multi-perspective approach may be more appropriate for women. This approach would involve targeting sources of self-efficacy, modifying outcome expectations, and considering sociostructural factors and goals.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, V.S.E.; methodology, V.S.E.; software, V.S.E.; validation, V.S.E. and R.S.; formal analysis, V.S.E.; investigation, V.S.E.; resources, R.S.; data curation, V.S.E.; writing—original draft preparation, V.S.E.; writing—review and editing, V.S.E. and R.S.; visualization, V.S.E.; supervision, R.S.; project administration, R.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Empirical Human and Economic Sciences of Saarland University (Approval 24–32) on 18 November 2024.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in this study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are available from the authors upon request. The data are not publicly available due to privacy and ethical restrictions.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Abbreviations

The following abbreviations are used in this manuscript:
SCTSocial Cognitive Theory
SEMStructural Equation Model

References

  1. Bandura, A. Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory; Prentice-Hall, Inc.: Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA, 1986; ISBN 978-0-13-815614-5. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bandura, A. Health Promotion by Social Cognitive Means. Health Educ. Behav. 2004, 31, 143–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Egele, V.S.; Klopp, E.; Stark, R. How Valid Is Bandura’s Social Cognitive Theory to Explain Physical Activity Behavior? Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2025, 15, 20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Bandura, A. Health Promotion from the Perspective of Social Cognitive Theory. In Understanding and Changing Health Behaviour: From Health Beliefs to Self-Regulation; Norman, P., Abraham, C., Conner, M., Eds.; Harwood Academic Publishers: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2000; p. 339. ISBN 978-90-5823-073-7. [Google Scholar]
  5. Auster-Gussman, L.A.; Gavin, K.L.; Siddique, J.; Welch, W.A.; Solk, P.; Whitaker, M.; Cullather, E.; Fanning, J.; Maria, C.S.; Gradishar, W.; et al. Social Cognitive Variables and Physical Activity during Chemotherapy for Breast Cancer: An Intensive Longitudinal Examination. Psycho-Oncology 2022, 31, 425–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Baird, J.F.; Silveira, S.L.; Motl, R.W. Social Cognitive Theory and Physical Activity in Older Adults with Multiple Sclerosis. Int. J. MS Care 2021, 23, 21–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Beauchamp, M.R.; Crawford, K.L.; Jackson, B. Social Cognitive Theory and Physical Activity: Mechanisms of Behavior Change, Critique, and Legacy. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2019, 42, 110–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Sebastian, A.T.; Rajkumar, E.; Tejaswini, P.; Lakshmi, R.; Romate, J. Applying Social Cognitive Theory to Predict Physical Activity and Dietary Behavior among Patients with Type-2 Diabetes. Health Psychol. Res. 2021, 9, 24510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Young, M.D.; Plotnikoff, R.C.; Collins, C.E.; Callister, R.; Morgan, P.J. Social Cognitive Theory and Physical Activity: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Obes. Rev. 2014, 15, 983–995. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Silveira, S.L.; Motl, R.W. Do Social Cognitive Theory Constructs Explain Response Heterogeneity with a Physical Activity Behavioral Intervention in Multiple Sclerosis? Contemp. Clin. Trials Commun. 2019, 15, 100366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Zechner, M.R.; Gill, K.J. Predictors of Physical Activity in Persons with Mental Illness: Testing a Social Cognitive Model. Psychiatr. Rehabil. J. 2016, 39, 321–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Stacey, F.G.; James, E.L.; Chapman, K.; Courneya, K.S.; Lubans, D.R. A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Social Cognitive Theory-Based Physical Activity and/or Nutrition Behavior Change Interventions for Cancer Survivors. J. Cancer Surviv. 2015, 9, 305–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Young, M.D.; Plotnikoff, R.C.; Collins, C.E.; Callister, R.; Morgan, P.J. A Test of Social Cognitive Theory to Explain Men’s Physical Activity During a Gender-Tailored Weight Loss Program. Am. J. Men’s Health 2016, 10, NP176–NP187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Neal, W.N.; Pekmezi, D.; Motl, R.W. Physical Activity Levels and Social Cognitive Theory Correlates among Adults with Crohn’s Disease: Preliminary Results from a Cross-Sectional Analysis. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2024, 21, 462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Guthold, R.; Stevens, G.A.; Riley, L.M.; Bull, F.C. Worldwide Trends in Insufficient Physical Activity from 2001 to 2016: A Pooled Analysis of 358 Population-Based Surveys with 1·9 Million Participants. The Lancet Global Health 2018, 6, e1077–e1086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Strain, T.; Flaxman, S.; Guthold, R.; Semenova, E.; Cowan, M.; Riley, L.M.; Bull, F.C.; Stevens, G.A.; Abdul Raheem, R.; Agoudavi, K.; et al. National, Regional, and Global Trends in Insufficient Physical Activity among Adults from 2000 to 2022: A Pooled Analysis of 507 Population-Based Surveys with 5·7 Million Participants. Lancet Glob. Health 2024, 12, e1232–e1243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Bull, F.C.; Al-Ansari, S.S.; Biddle, S.; Borodulin, K.; Buman, M.P.; Cardon, G.; Carty, C.; Chaput, J.-P.; Chastin, S.; Chou, R.; et al. World Health Organization 2020 Guidelines on Physical Activity and Sedentary Behaviour. Br. J. Sports Med. 2020, 54, 1451–1462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Campesi, I.; Franconi, F.; Serra, P.A. The Appropriateness of Medical Devices Is Strongly Influenced by Sex and Gender. Life 2024, 14, 234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Choi, J.; Lee, M.; Lee, J.; Kang, D.; Choi, J.-Y. Correlates Associated with Participation in Physical Activity among Adults: A Systematic Review of Reviews and Update. BMC Public Health 2017, 17, 356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Robbins, L.B.; Pender, N.J.; Ronis, D.L.; Kazanis, A.S.; Pis, M.B. Physical Activity, Self-efficacy, and Perceived Exertion among Adolescents. Res. Nurs. Health 2004, 27, 435–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Spence, J.C.; Blanchard, C.M.; Clark, M.; Plotnikoff, R.C.; Storey, K.E.; McCargar, L. The Role of Self-Efficacy in Explaining Gender Differences in Physical Activity Among Adolescents: A Multilevel Analysis. J. Phys. Act. Health 2010, 7, 176–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Anderson, E.S.; Wojcik, J.R.; Winett, R.A.; Williams, D.M. Social-Cognitive Determinants of Physical Activity: The Influence of Social Support, Self-Efficacy, Outcome Expectations, and Self-Regulation among Participants in a Church-Based Health Promotion Study. Health Psychol. 2006, 25, 510–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Trost, S.G.; Pate, R.R.; Sallis, J.F.; Freedson, P.S.; Taylor, W.C.; Dowda, M.; Sirard, J. Age and Gender Differences in Objectively Measured Physical Activity in Youth. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2002, 34, 350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Portela-Pino, I.; López-Castedo, A.; Martínez-Patiño, M.J.; Valverde-Esteve, T.; Domínguez-Alonso, J. Gender Differences in Motivation and Barriers for The Practice of Physical Exercise in Adolescence. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Ah Hong, S.; Peltzer, K.; Wimonpeerapattana, W. Impact of Self-Efficacy and Parenting Practice on Physical Activity among School Children. Nagoya J. Med. Sci. 2017, 79, 339–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Martin, J.J.; McCaughtry, N. Predicting Physical Activity in Inner-City Hispanic American Children. Hisp. Health Care Int. 2008, 6, 150–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Ji, H.; Gulati, M.; Huang, T.Y.; Kwan, A.C.; Ouyang, D.; Ebinger, J.E.; Casaletto, K.; Moreau, K.L.; Skali, H.; Cheng, S. Sex Differences in Association of Physical Activity with All-Cause and Cardiovascular Mortality. J. Am. Coll. Cardiol. 2024, 83, 783–793. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Seabra, A.F.T.; Maia, J.; Mendonça, D.; Thomis, M.; Caspersen, C.; Fulton, J. Age and Sex Differences in Physical Activity of Portuguese Adolescents. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2008, 40, 65–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Amagasa, S.; Inoue, S.; Shibata, A.; Ishii, K.; Kurosawa, S.; Owen, N.; Oka, K. Differences in Accelerometer-Measured Physical Activity and Sedentary Behavior Between Middle-Aged Men and Women in Japan: A Compositional Data Analysis. J. Phys. Act. Health 2022, 19, 500–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Rodriguez-Ayllon, M.; Cadenas-Sánchez, C.; Estévez-López, F.; Muñoz, N.E.; Mora-Gonzalez, J.; Migueles, J.H.; Molina-García, P.; Henriksson, H.; Mena-Molina, A.; Martínez-Vizcaíno, V.; et al. Role of Physical Activity and Sedentary Behavior in the Mental Health of Preschoolers, Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Sports Med. 2019, 49, 1383–1410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. van Uffelen, J.G.Z.; Khan, A.; Burton, N.W. Gender Differences in Physical Activity Motivators and Context Preferences: A Population-Based Study in People in Their Sixties. BMC Public Health 2017, 17, 624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Whipple, M.O.; Pinto, A.J.; Abushamat, L.A.; Bergouignan, A.; Chapman, K.; Huebschmann, A.G.; Masters, K.S.; Nadeau, K.J.; Scalzo, R.L.; Schauer, I.E.; et al. Sex Differences in Physical Activity Among Individuals With Type 2 Diabetes Across the Life Span: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Diabetes Care 2022, 45, 2163–2177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Egele, V.S.; Stark, R. Operationalization of the Social Cognitive Theory to Explain and Predict Physical Activity in Germany: A Scale Development. Front. Sports Act. Living 2024, 6, 1508602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Booth, M. Assessment of Physical Activity: An International Perspective. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 2000, 71, 114–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Craig, C.L.; Marshall, A.L.; Sjöström, M.; Bauman, A.E.; Booth, M.L.; Ainsworth, B.E.; Pratt, M.; Ekelund, U.; Yngve, A.; Sallis, J.F.; et al. International Physical Activity Questionnaire: 12-Country Reliability and Validity. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2003, 35, 1381–1395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Hagströmer, M.; Oja, P.; Sjöström, M. The International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ): A Study of Concurrent and Construct Validity. Public Health Nutr. 2006, 9, 755–762. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Bauman, A.; Ainsworth, B.E.; Bull, F.; Craig, C.L.; Hagströmer, M.; Sallis, J.F.; Pratt, M.; Sjöström, M. Progress and Pitfalls in the Use of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ) for Adult Physical Activity Surveillance. J. Phys. Act. Health 2009, 6, S5–S8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Leiner, D.J. SoSci Survey, (Version 3.7.06) [Computer Software]. Available online: https://www.soscisurvey.de (accessed on 21 November 2024).
  39. Rosseel, Y. Lavaan: An R Package for Structural Equation Modeling. J. Stat. Softw. 2012, 48, 1–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Revelle, W. Psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research.(2.3.3) [Computer Software] 2023. Available online: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/psych/index.html (accessed on 12 January 2025).
  41. Schermelleh-Engel, K.; Moosbrugger, H.; Müller, H. Evaluating the Fit of Structural Equation Models: Tests of Significance and Descriptive Goodness-of-Fit Measures. Methods Psychol. Res. 2003, 8, 23–74. [Google Scholar]
  42. Lance, C.E.; Beck, S.S.; Fan, Y.; Carter, N.T. A Taxonomy of Path-Related Goodness-of-Fit Indices and Recommended Criterion Values. Psychol. Methods 2016, 21, 388–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Guenole, N.; Brown, A. The Consequences of Ignoring Measurement Invariance for Path Coefficients in Structural Equation Models. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 980. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Meade, A.W.; Johnson, E.C.; Braddy, P.W. Power and Sensitivity of Alternative Fit Indices in Tests of Measurement Invariance. J. Appl. Psychol. 2008, 93, 568–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Andrade, A.; D’Oliveira, A.; dos Santos, K.M.; Falese, L.; Mancone, S.; Diotaiuti, P.; Stabile, L.; Buonanno, G. Mood States of Active and Insufficiently Active Adolescents Related to Sleep Quality, Gender, Academic Performance and Guidelines for the Post-COVID-19 Scenario. Front. Psychol. 2024, 15, 1494456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Wiley, E.; Moncion, K.; Rodrigues, L.; Fang, H.; Noguchi, K.S.; Roig, M.; Richardson, J.; MacDermid, J.C.; Tang, A. Exploring Differences between Gender Expressions in Exercise Self-Efficacy and Outcome Expectations for Exercise in Individuals with Stroke. PLoS ONE 2024, 19, e0299288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Larsen, S.; Mozdoorzoy, T.; Kristiansen, E.; Nygaard Falch, H.; Aune, T.K.; van den Tillaar, R. A Comparison of Motives by Gender and Age Categories for Training at Norwegian Fitness Centres. Sports 2021, 9, 113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Gellert, P.; Ziegelmann, J.P.; Schwarzer, R. Affective and Health-Related Outcome Expectancies for Physical Activity in Older Adults. Psychol. Health 2012, 27, 816–828. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Feil, K.; Fritsch, J.; Rhodes, R.E. The Intention-Behaviour Gap in Physical Activity: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of the Action Control Framework. Br. J. Sports Med. 2023, 57, 1265–1271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Baranowski, T.; Anderson, C.; Carmack, C. Mediating Variable Framework in Physical Activity Interventions. How Are We Doing? How Might We Do Better? Am. J. Prev. Med. 1998, 15, 266–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Lee, P.H.; Macfarlane, D.J.; Lam, T.; Stewart, S.M. Validity of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire Short Form (IPAQ-SF): A Systematic Review. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2011, 8, 115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Egele, V.S.; Kiefer, L.H.; Stark, R. Faking Self-Reports of Health Behavior: A Comparison between a Within- and a Between-Subjects Design. Health Psychol. Behav. Med. 2021, 9, 895–916. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Straßburg, A.; Eisinger-Watzl, M.; Krems, C.; Roth, A.; Hoffmann, I. Comparison of Food Consumption and Nutrient Intake Assessed with Three Dietary Assessment Methods: Results of the German National Nutrition Survey II. Eur. J. Nutr. 2019, 58, 193–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Egele, V.S.; Klopp, E.; Stark, R. Evaluating Self-Reported Retrospective Average Daily Fruit, Vegetable, and Egg Intake: Trustworthy—Sometimes! Appl. Psychol. Health Well-Being 2023, 15, 1130–1149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Social Cognitive Theory. This figure was previously published by Egele et al. [3], as proposed by Bandura [4]. Note: The nomenclature of these paths (a1, a2, a3, b1, b2, c1, c2, c3) is entirely subjective, arising from the authors’ personal predilection for the conceptualization of the SEM.
Figure 1. Social Cognitive Theory. This figure was previously published by Egele et al. [3], as proposed by Bandura [4]. Note: The nomenclature of these paths (a1, a2, a3, b1, b2, c1, c2, c3) is entirely subjective, arising from the authors’ personal predilection for the conceptualization of the SEM.
Sports 13 00249 g001
Figure 2. Direct paths in SCT for men; significant paths are marked in blue.
Figure 2. Direct paths in SCT for men; significant paths are marked in blue.
Sports 13 00249 g002
Figure 3. Direct paths in SCT for women; significant paths are marked in red.
Figure 3. Direct paths in SCT for women; significant paths are marked in red.
Sports 13 00249 g003
Table 1. Fit measure of the measurement and Structural Equation Models.
Table 1. Fit measure of the measurement and Structural Equation Models.
χ2dfpCFISRMRRMSEA [CI]pclose
Self-efficacy6.73430.0810.9980.0090.052 [0.000–0.106]0.546
Goals6.05120.0490.9970.0130.056 [0.005–0.126]0.265
Outcome expectations7.26630.0640.9930.0170.054 [0.000–0.106]0.365
Sociostructural factors1.10410.2961.0000.0080.013 [0.000–0.111]0.577
Complete CFA230.202124<0.0010.9780.0530.040 [0.032–0.048]0.979
Hypothesis 1
SEM1258.545140<0.0010.9760.0520.039 [0.032–0.047]0.991
Null modell1 *735.883------
All paths SEM1 *369.726------
Partial structural model1<0.0012<0.0011.000<0.0010.000 [0.000–0.000]>0.999
Hypothesis 2
SEM2overall444.044294<0.0010.9700.0620.043 [0.035–0.051]0.022
SEM2M210.152------
SEM2W233.892------
Null modell2M *395.332------
Null modell2W *450.108------
All paths SEM2M *209.602------
All paths SEM2W *228.396------
Partial structural model2M<0.0012<0.0011.000<0.0010.000 [0.000–0.000]>0.999
Partial structural model2W<0.0012<0.0011.000<0.0010.000 [0.000–0.000]>0.999
Note: * C9 and C10 indices were calculated by the null and all paths SEMs. This table only reports χ2-test statistics, as the models were not judged in regard to their model fit.
Table 2. Direct, indirect, and total effects for the standardized regression coefficients both overall and for men and women separately.
Table 2. Direct, indirect, and total effects for the standardized regression coefficients both overall and for men and women separately.
Paths EstimateSEzp
Total effect
Total effectOverall0.3360.0734.595<0.001
Men0.3380.1522.2270.026
Women0.3270.0754.383<0.001
Direct effects
a1
Self-efficacy → Outcome expectations
Overall0.3110.0388.223<0.001
Men0.3930.0586.724<0.001
Women0.2520.0376.745<0.001
a2
Self-efficacy → Sociostructural factors
Overall−0.2190.047−4.657<0.001
Men−0.1600.067−2.3810.017
Women−0.2640.059−4.441<0.001
a3
Self-efficacy → Goals
Overall0.3960.0596.695<0.001
Men0.2940.1012.9200.004
Women0.4580.0696.646<0.001
b1
Outcome expectations → Goals
Overall0.5040.1114.543<0.001
Men0.5780.1703.3920.001
Women0.4960.1393.573<0.001
b2
Sociostructural factors → Goals
Overall0.1020.0432.3500.019
Men0.0740.0740.9960.319
Women0.1050.0531.9780.048
c1
Self-efficacy → Physical activity
Overall0.3070.0773.975<0.001
Men0.3480.1492.3400.019
Women0.2510.0892.8320.005
c2
Outcome expectations → Physical activity
Overall−0.3550.137−2.5960.009
Men−0.2730.227−1.2040.229
Women−0.4110.170−2.4170.016
c3
Goals → Physical activity
Overall0.1830.0553.3510.001
Men0.1030.0651.5910.112
Women0.2570.0823.1540.002
Indirect effects
a1 × c2
Self-efficacy → Outcome expectations → Physical activity
Overall−0.0680.017−4.053<0.001
Men−0.0630.028−2.2320.026
Women−0.0660.018−3.673<0.001
a1 × b1
Self-efficacy → Outcome expectations → Goals
Overall0.1570.0374.215<0.001
Men0.2270.0733.1260.002
Women0.1250.0383.2930.001
a1 × b1 × c3
Self-efficacy → Outcome expectations → Goals → Physical activity
Overall−0.0290.0122.4220.015
Men0.0230.0191.2660.206
Women0.0320.0152.2090.027
a3 × c3
Self-efficacy → Goals → Physical activity
Overall0.0720.0223.2840.001
Men0.0300.0191.5930.111
Women0.1180.0393.0270.002
a2 × b2
Self-efficacy → Sociostructural factors → Goals
Overall−0.0220.011−2.1070.034
Men−0.0120.013−0.9430.346
Women−0.0280.015−1.7920.073
a2 × b2 × c3
Self-efficacy → Sociostructural factors → Goals → Physical activity
Overall−0.0040.002−1.9360.053
Men−0.0010.001−0.8410.400
Women−0.0070.004−1.7270.084
b1 × c3
Outcome expectations → Goals → Physical activity
Overall−0.0920.0382.4570.014
Men0.0600.0471.2750.202
Women0.1280.0552.3090.021
b2 × c3
Sociostructural factors → Goals → Physical activity
Overall−0.0190.0090.0370.037
Men0.0080.0090.8590.390
Women0.0270.0141.8750.061
a1 × c2 + a1 × b1 + c3 + a2 × b2 × c3 + a3 × c3
Complete indirect path of self-efficacy
Overall0.0290.0340.8390.401
Men−0.0100.045−0.2240.822
Women0.0760.0501.5430.123
Note: Figure 1 displays the designation of the path coefficients.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Egele, V.S.; Stark, R. Social Cognitive Theory and Physical Activity: Examining Gender-Based Prediction Patterns and Theoretical Validity. Sports 2025, 13, 249. https://doi.org/10.3390/sports13080249

AMA Style

Egele VS, Stark R. Social Cognitive Theory and Physical Activity: Examining Gender-Based Prediction Patterns and Theoretical Validity. Sports. 2025; 13(8):249. https://doi.org/10.3390/sports13080249

Chicago/Turabian Style

Egele, Viktoria Sophie, and Robin Stark. 2025. "Social Cognitive Theory and Physical Activity: Examining Gender-Based Prediction Patterns and Theoretical Validity" Sports 13, no. 8: 249. https://doi.org/10.3390/sports13080249

APA Style

Egele, V. S., & Stark, R. (2025). Social Cognitive Theory and Physical Activity: Examining Gender-Based Prediction Patterns and Theoretical Validity. Sports, 13(8), 249. https://doi.org/10.3390/sports13080249

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop