Next Article in Journal
Special Issue: Integrated Pest Management in Arable and Open Field Horticultural Crops
Previous Article in Journal
Two Distinct Genotypes of Spissistilus festinus (Say, 1830) (Hemiptera, Membracidae) in the United States Revealed by Phylogenetic and Morphological Analyses
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Allometry and Fighting Behaviour of a Dimorphic Stag Beetle Cyclommatus mniszechi (Coleoptera: Lucanidae)

Department of Life Science, National Taiwan Normal University, No. 88, Sec. 4, Tingzhou Rd., Taipei 11677, Taiwan
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors have contributed equally to this work.
Insects 2020, 11(2), 81; https://doi.org/10.3390/insects11020081
Submission received: 2 December 2019 / Revised: 16 January 2020 / Accepted: 20 January 2020 / Published: 23 January 2020

Abstract

:
Male stag beetles (Coleoptera: Lucanidae) use their mandibles as weapons to compete for resources and reproduction. Mandible size in stag beetles can be associated with different behaviours and the outcome of male contests. We investigated the allometric relationship between mandible and body size in males of the stag beetle Cyclommatus mniszechi to uncover distinct morphs. The results divided male C. mniszechi into majors and minors with the switch point of mandible length at 14.01 mm. The allometric slope of mandibles was positive for both morphs but was steeper for the minors. We also characterised the fighting behaviour of the different morphs in size-matched contests using sequential analyses. Males matched each other’s behaviour in contests with many physical contacts, no injury and a progression from low towards high aggression and rare de-escalation. Major and minor males employed the same behavioural elements in contests, but major males were more likely to escalate directly into more aggressive phases and minor males tended to stay within phases. This finding suggests that major males may compete more aggressively than minor males in contests.

1. Introduction

Males of many insects use weapons to compete for resources essential for their survival and reproduction [1]. Sexually selected traits, such as ornaments and weapons, often evolve to reach extremes of size and elaboration [1,2,3,4]. One group of insects with an amazing diversity of weapons is stag beetles (Lucanidae). Male stag beetles use their exaggerated mandibles as weapons to fight for access to sap sites, territory and females [1,5,6,7]. Mandible and body size are reliable indicators of male stag beetles’ resource holding potential (RHP) and positively correlate with their ability to win in contests [6,7,8]. Stag beetles exhibit a high diversity in the sizes and shapes of mandibles both within and across species [1,9,10,11] which might cause the behaviour they display in fighting contests to be highly variable [1].
The scaling or allometric relationship between the sizes of different body parts and overall body size provides important information about an organism’s biology such as polyphenism [12], alternative reproductive tactics [13] and the nature of selection on different body parts [14,15]. The allometric scaling of sexually selected traits can be positively allometric (slope > 1), isometric (slope = 1) or negatively allometric (slope < 1) and often reflects resource-allocation trade-offs, the balance between natural and sexual selection and the genetic constraints on these traits [14,16]. Mandible and overall body size in many stag beetles show a linear positive allometry, in which larger males have relatively larger mandibles than smaller males [10,17,18,19,20]. Positive allometries suggest that larger males gain greater fitness advantages by investing disproportionally more in the development of weapons than other body parts [4]. Non-linear (curved, sigmoid or discontinuous) allometries between mandible and body size have also been reported for stag beetles (4,10,19). The declining slope of the curved allometry in the largest males is probably due to the depletion of resources available for the development of the weapon [15,19,21]. Sigmoid and discontinuous allometries are characterised with the evolution of size-dependent, alternative reproductive tactics [3]. The non-linear allometries between trait size and body size can be characterised following the approach and regression models recommended by Knell 2009 [22].
Understanding the nature of an animal’s fighting behaviour is essential to understand the evolution and diversification of its weapon forms [1]. How male stag beetles interact with and respond to each other in contests, however, remains poorly understood. For the majority of stag beetle species, it is unclear how males use their mandibular weapons in combat and whether males with different weapon sizes and shapes display different contest behaviours. There are approximately 1200 species of stag beetles in the world [23]. So far, fighting behaviour has only been reported for five species, namely, Cyclommatus mniszechi [8], C. metallifer [6], Lucanus maculifemoratus and Prosopocoilus inclinatus [24] and Aegus chelifer [7]. These studies indicate that the behavioural elements and sequences are highly variable both among and within species. The commonly used method for studying the contest interactions of stag beetles includes (1) placing two males some distance apart on a horizontal tree log [6,8] and (2) allowing the study animals a short time period (≤15–60 min) to acclimatise [6,7,8,24]. This method does not appear to be suitable for investigating the contest behaviour of C. mniszechi because 47% of the contest pairs failed to interact [8]. Different experimental methods, more closely resembling the study animal’s natural habitat, are therefore needed for studying the contest behaviours of different species of stag beetles.
This study investigated the allometry and fighting behaviour in the stag beetle C. mniszechi. An earlier study classified male C. mniszechi into three morphs based on the shape and size of their mandibles (Figure 1, alpha, beta and gamma) [8]. The relationship between mandible and overall body size of these beetles, however, remains unclear. We first examined the relationship between mandible and body size to identify which type of allometry best describes the relationship and determine whether the males could be grouped into different morphs based on the relationship. The allometric slopes of male morphs provide important information on the level of resource allocated to production of weapons. They are also useful to examine differences in resource allocation and fighting behaviours and the relationship between them in the different morphs. We then characterised the behaviour of males of different morphs in size-matched contests using sequential analyses. Size-matched contests allowed us to remove the effect of differences in males’ mandible size (a proxy for RHP) on contest outcomes and behaviours in order to characterise more precisely the behavioural difference among male morphs. This experimental approach has not been employed by previous studies of stag beetles’ fighting behaviours [6,7,8,24]. To study the contest behaviours of the males, our trials were novel in their experimental setups which allowed the males to encounter each other at the feeding site to compete for food, resembling the competition occurring in their natural habitat.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Organism

The stag beetle C. mniszechi is distributed in southeast China, Vietnam and northern Taiwan and inhabits lowland forests below approximately 750 m [26]. During the breeding season, from May to August in Taiwan, males perform mate-guarding behaviours and engage in territorial fights at the sap sites on the branches or trunks of broadleaf trees including Fraxinus griffithii (Oleaceae), Broussonetia papyrifera (Moraceae), Citrus spp. (Rutaceae) and Koelreuteria elegans (Sapindaceae). Male C. mniszechi are highly variable in body size, and an earlier study [8] classified them into three morphs (alpha, beta and gamma) based on the size and the shape of their mandibles. Alpha males have the largest mandibles with tusk-like projections (denticles) at their distal half (Figure 1, black arrows). Beta males have smaller mandibles with denticles at their proximal halves and close to the base of the mandibles. Gamma males have the smallest saw-like mandibles with no apparent denticles.

2.2. Insect Rearing

Adult C. mniszechi were collected from lowland forests of northern Taiwan between 2016 and 2018 with light traps and by chopping decayed wood and manually searching at sap sites on tree trunks. The 72 males collected from the field included 27 (37.5%) alpha, 34 (47.2%) beta and 11 (15.3%) gamma morphs. Males collected in 2018 were used in fighting contests the same year, and the males collected in 2016 and 2017 were used in breeding and morphological measurement. Field-collected females and males (after use in contests) were bred in the laboratory. Some reared larvae were obtained from local insect breeders in 2016 and 2017, and they were used for breeding and morphological measurement. Adult stag beetles were kept individually with an adequate supply of insect jelly (PPS-801, Champ E Pets Corporation, Taipei, Taiwan) in plastic containers (15 cm × 10 cm × 11 cm) at 25 °C under a 12 h:12 h (light:dark) cycle. The 2016 larvae were reared in 250 mL cylindrical plastic containers (7.5 cm in diameter; 4.5 cm in height) filled with fermented sawdust (good-quality microparticle fermented oak sawdust, Max Piggyfat Insect Feeding Facilities, Taiwan) under a 12 h:12 h (light:dark) cycle at 25 °C. Synchronous metamorphosis of beetle pupae can be regulated by chemical signals, social cues and the fluctuation of temperature [27,28]. The temperature (15.8–25.3 °C) used for larval rearing from September of 2017 to May of 2018 was therefore adjusted every two weeks (minimising the detrimental effect of sudden changes in temperature on the beetle’s health) to match the 2012–2016 mean monthly temperature at the Central Weather Bureau stations closest to the collection sites (Cyuchih (70 m) and Shanjia (48 m)) to synchronise the emergence time of adult males in 2018 for subsequent use in fighting contests.

2.3. Morphological Measurements

The mandible length (ML), head width (HW) and elytra length (EL) of the beetles were measured to the nearest of 0.01 mm using a digital calliper (99MAD027M1, Mitutoyo, Kanagawa, Japan). Mandible length was the average linear distance between the distal tip of the mandible and the axis of the mandibular joint of the two mandibles [7,8,29]; HW was the linear distance between the tips of the protrusions anterior to the eyes; EL was the linear distance between the posterior ends of the scutellum and the elytra. These three measurements were obtained on the day on which the beetles were collected in the field or the 21st day after the eclosion of the beetles reared in the laboratory. The body weight (BW) of the beetles was measured to the nearest of 0.001 g using a digital scale (CT-50, HIRODA, Shenzhen, China) one day before the contests.

2.4. Allometry Analyses

The morphological measurements of a total of 232 males were used for allometry analysis (Table S1). The plot of natural log of body size (elytra length, EL; X) against the natural log of weapon size (mandible length, ML; Y) indicated that the relationship between them was continuous and non-linear (Figure 1). We thus followed Knell 2009 [22] to characterise possible non-linear allometries. We first tested for linearity using the quadratic model [30,31]:
lnY = β0 + β1 lnX + β2 lnX2 + ε
where β0 is the intercept, β1 and β2 are the regression coefficients and ε is the error. A coefficient β2 which significantly differs from zero would suggest a non-linear relationship. We then tested for the following statistical models [25,31] using Akaike (AIC) and Bayesian information criterion (BIC) for model selection:
Linear model:
lnY = β0 + β1 lnX + ε
Eberhard and Gutierrez continuous piecewise:
lnY = β0 + β1 lnX + β2 (lnX − lnX0) D + ε
Eberhard and Gutierrez discontinuous piecewise:
lnY = β0 + β1 lnX + β2 (lnX − lnX0) D + β3 D + ε
Kotiaho and Tomkins linear:
lnX = β0 + β1 lnY + ε
Kotiaho and Tomkins quadratic:
lnX = β0 + β1 lnY + β2 lnY2 + ε
Kotiaho and Tomkins continuous piecewise:
lnX = β0 + β1 lnY + β2 (lnY − lnY0) D + ε
Kotiaho and Tomkins discontinuous piecewise:
lnX = β0 + β1 lnY + β2 (lnY − lnY0) D + β3 D + ε
For the Eberhard and Gutierrez continuous and discontinuous piecewise models, where lnX0 is the switch point, D = 0 if lnX < lnX0 (minor males) or D = 1 if lnX ≥ lnX0 (major males). The best estimate of the value of lnX0 was determined by the maximum adjusted R-squared values of the regression following the method of Sugiura et al. 2007 [32]. If β2 was significantly different from zero, the slope of allometry changed at lnX0. If β3 differed significantly from zero, the allometry was discontinuous at lnX0. For the Kotiaho and Tomkins models, lnY0 of weapon size (mandible length) is the switch point.
The isometry test using the equation lnY = A lnX + lna, a linearised form of Y = aXA [17], was conducted to examine the allometric slope A between mandible size (ML)/head width (HW) and body size (EL) of different male morphs. The significance of the difference between A and 1 was evaluated by two-tailed t-test. An allometric slope of A > 1 (positive allometry) indicates that larger males have disproportionally larger mandibles than those of smaller males. When A = 1 (isometry), the mandible size among males is exactly proportional to their body sizes. A slope of A < 1 (negative allometry) shows that larger males have disproportionally smaller mandibles than those of smaller males. All allometric analyses were conducted in R (version 3.6.0, R Development Core Team, 2019) using the R scripts of Kojima and Lin 2017 [15].

2.5. Male–Male Contests

Size-matched contests were used to characterise the fighting behaviour of different males of C. mniszechi after eliminating the effect of unequal RHP. Because mandible size is used as a proxy for RHP in stag beetles [6,8], males reared in 2018 with similar mandible lengths were paired up for the contests. We only considered the fights staged between major and minor males because our analyses divided individuals of the beetles into these two groups (see results of allometry). We staged two types of contests (major versus major, minor versus minor) to investigate whether these two types of males use different fighting strategies. The difference in mandible length between the contestants was less than 5% (0.683 mm) of the median mandible length (13.655 mm) of all contestants (mean ± SD = 13.665 ± 3.126 mm, n = 114). The fighting arena was an acrylic container (32 cm × 18 cm × 30 cm) with 400 mL sawdust (1 cm height) at the bottom (Figure 2). The arena was divided into one fighting (16 cm × 18 cm) and two resting (8 cm × 18 cm) zones using two acrylic dividers (18 cm × 30 cm). Three to four dry oak leaves (Quercus glauca, Fagaceae) were placed on the surface of the sawdust layer of the resting zones for the beetles to hide under. A piece of half-cut wood (10 cm × 16 cm × 5 cm) was placed at the centre of the fighting zone to serve as a feeding station to facilitate competition between the two males. The arena was placed in a room at 25 °C with a 12 h:12 h (light:dark) cycle. None of the males used in the fighting contests had any prior fighting experience, and their bodies were all intact with no damaged or missing body parts.
All contests were conducted in the evening (from 18:00 to 19:00), because C. mniszechi is nocturnal [8]. At 18:00 the day before contest day, the two male contestants were placed in the two resting zones (one male per resting zone) of the fighting arena to acclimatise. During the daytime the male stag beetles usually hid under the dry leaves in the resting zones without obvious body movement. At 18:00 of the contest day, a 2 mL tube filled with insect jelly (PPS-801, Champ E Pets Corporation, Taipei, Taiwan) was inserted at the centre of the feeding station (Figure 2). Then, the two dividers were removed simultaneously to allow the two males to interact. The two males usually emerged from under the dry leaves, moved around the arena and walked towards the feeding station within 5 to 10 min of the dividers being removed. Agonistic interactions occurred when two males encountered each other, usually on the feeding station. This contest setup allowed the males to encounter each other during foraging/feeding and decide how to interact with each other. It resembled the situation that the males face in their natural habitat. The males were allowed to interact and fight until the fights were resolved with a clear winner and loser. If the two males did not exhibit sufficient aggression towards each other to produce a clear winner and loser, the trials were terminated after 1 h. After the termination of the trials, the two males were removed from the arena. The contests were recorded using night-vision video monitors (DS-VR7160H, Der Shuenn, Taipei, Taiwan) positioned 65 cm above the arena.

2.6. Sequential Analyses of Fighting Behaviour

We used BORIS v. 7.4 (Behavior Observation Research Interactive Software) [33] to transcribe the fighting behaviours of the males from the videos based on nine behavioural elements (Table 1). The contests were characterised as less (0: no “tussles”) or more intensive (1: with “tussles”). The fighting behaviours were coded individually in two adjacent columns, “preceding behaviour” followed by “subsequent behaviour”, for all behavioural transitions. The behavioural sequence data were summarised into adjacency matrices of contest behaviours using igraph network analysis package [34] in R following Green and Patek 2018 [35] (Tables S2 and S3). The transition rate for each behavioural transition was calculated from the ratio of the preceding behaviour to its subsequent behaviour. To examine whether a specific behavioural transition was more frequent than expected by chance, a permutation procedure was conducted by fixing the 1st columns (preceding behaviour), keeping the relative frequency of the behaviours and randomly sampling the 2nd columns (subsequent behaviour), thus randomising behavioural transitions between the behaviours [35,36]. The permutation process was executed 10,000 times to generate a null distribution of random transitions. A significant transition was found when the observed transition was more frequent than the respective 95% quantile of the null distribution of random transitions. Significant behavioural transitions of the contests were plotted as network graphs in igraph. The behavioural elements were organised into four phases (Table 1) based on the network graphs. We defined a phase as a behaviour or subset of behaviours, in which these behaviours had relatively similar level of aggressiveness and they were used with relatively equal frequency [35].

2.7. Statistical Analyses

The regression models were analysed in JMP (ver. 10.0.0, SAS institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). A logistic regression model was used to examine the effect of either male morphs or mandible length on the level of aggression (probability of tussle) for all contests. Mandible length was used as the single explanatory factor in regression analyses of the contests of different male morphs.

3. Results

3.1. Allometry

For the allometric relationship between the mandible and elytra length, the β2 coefficient in the quadratic model was significantly different from 0, suggesting that the relationship was significantly non-linear (Table 2). The Kotiaho and Tomkins continuous and discontinuous piecewise model had the lowest AIC and BIC score, respectively. The Kotiaho and Tomkins discontinuous piecewise model improved the model fit only slightly (ΔAIC = 0.17), and the β3 coefficient of the model was not significantly different from 0, so the simpler Kotiaho and Tomkins continuous piecewise model was considered to be the best fit model for the allometric relationship. The value of the switch point (lnY0) which showed the highest adjusted R2 in this model was 2.64 (14.01 mm). The β2 coefficient of this model was significantly different from 0, indicating that the linear slope between the mandible and elytra length is continuous but changes markedly either side of the switch point (ML = 14.01 mm) (Figure 1). Therefore, male C. mniszechi are dimorphic and can be divided allometrically into majors and minors above and below a mandible length of 14.01 mm, respectively. The major males contain mostly alpha morphs, and the minor males include beta and gamma morphs (Figure 1). The allometric slope of the mandibles of minor males (A = 2.89 ± 0.10) is steeper than that of major males (A = 1.43 ± 0.10). The mandibles and heads of both major and minor males showed significantly positive allometries (Table 3).

3.2. Behavioural Sequences of Fighting Contests

A total of 55 size-matched contests (24 major morphs and 31 minor morphs) were staged. Males of 14 (25.5%) contests escalated to more aggressive “tussle” behaviour. Three (5.5%) contests were resolved without aggressive interactions (i.e., “push”, “attack”, “tussle” or “clamp”). The average duration of the size-matched contests was 37.2 ± 58.8 s (mean ± SD). No observable injury was found on any male contestant.
The fighting contests of C. mniszechi stag beetle followed a variable course of behavioural sequences (Figure 3) (Video S1). The contest started when one male approached and touched its opponent’s body with its mandibles or forelegs (100%, transitional probability from “start” to “touch”, Figure 3). After “touch”, both major and minor males often entered a “defensive posture” by raising their heads and keeping their mandibles open towards their opponents (100%). “Defensive posture” then progressed to either “body raising” with rapid movement of the antenna (19%–30%) or “push” by raising mandibles and knocking the opponents (20%–37%). From “push”, the contests in both male morphs were either resolved by loser males entering “retreat” (23%–44%) or continuing into “attack” by biting the opponents with mandibles (34%–45%). From “body raising”, the behavioural sequences differed between major and minor males. In major males, “body raising” often directly led to “tussle” (50%) of phase 3 by interlocking mandibles and pushing each other, but it frequently resulted in “attack” of the same phase 2 in minor males (64%). Within phase 2, “push” was repeated many times in minor males (31%) but not in major males. “Attack” led to “tussle” (12%) and “retreat” (22%) in minor males, whereas “attack” was mainly repeated in major males (45%). In phase 3, minor males performed repetitive clamping onto the head (“clamp1”, 25%) and body of their opponents (“clamp2”, 25%) and frequent behavioural transitions between “tussle” and “clamp2” (12%–25%), while major males displayed repetitive “tussle” (50%) and “clamp2” (20%). From “tussle” of phase 3, the contests were often resolved by winner males clamping their rivals (“clamp1”, 29%–38%) and then flipping them into “retreat” (50%–100%).
Overall, the fighting behaviours of C. mniszechi progressed from phase 1, approach and touch; phase 2, defence and attack; phase 3, clamp and tussle and finally to phase 4, contest resolution (Figure 3). Behavioural matching between contestant males was observed for most behavioural elements. The contests of C. mniszechi males appeared to progress from less to more aggressive phases (e.g., from phase 1 to phase 2 or from phase 2 to phase 3) with a few behavioural transitions occurring within the same phases (Figure 3). Once contests escalated to aggressive physical contact (i.e., “push” and “attack” of phase 2; “clamp” and “tussle” of phase 3), behaviours frequently transitioned within phase or to phase 4 of contest resolution without first de-escalating to non-physical interactions in phase 2 (i.e., “body raising” or “defensive posture”). Minor males had more frequent behavioural transitions within phases than major males (i.e., between “body raising” and “attack”, and repetitive “push” of phase 2; between “tussle” and “clamp2”, and repetitive “clamp1” of phase 3, Figure 3). These results suggest that minor males tend to stay within these phases more than major males, whereas major males were more likely to escalate directly into more aggressive phases.

3.3. Male Morph and Contest Aggression

The regression analyses showed that the probability of tussle was not correlated with either mandible length (estimate ± SE = 0.185 ± 0.106, Chi-square = 3.02, p = 0.082) or male morphs (estimate ± SE = 0.344 ± 0.621, Chi-square = 0.31, p = 0.579) for all contests (n = 55). In the contests between minor males, the probability of tussle had a significant positive relation with mandible length (estimate ± SE = 0.709 ± 0.361, Chi-square = 3.86, p = 0.049, n = 31), whereas no significant relationship was found in the contests between major males (estimate ± SE = 0.341 ± 0.310, Chi-square = 1.21, p = 0.271, n = 24). These results indicate that neither mandible length nor male morphs have a significant effect on the probability of entering tussle. However, within minor morphs males with larger mandibles behaved more aggressively and were more likely to engage in tussles.

4. Discussion

Positive allometries were found for the mandibles and heads of C. mniszechi males. Allometric analyses demonstrate that C. mniszechi consists of dimorphic males defined quantitatively as majors and minors by mandible size at the switch point (Figure 1). The major males are mainly alpha morphs with the exception of a few beta morphs, whereas the minor males include both beta and gamma morphs, suggesting that the beta and gamma morphs have similar allometric slopes. The allometric slope of the mandibles of the major males is not as steep as that of the minor males, indicating that the major males invest proportionally less in the development of mandibles. A reduced allometric slope in the weapons of larger males is also found in other stag beetles [19], rhinoceros beetles [37,38] and flower beetles [15]. This pattern can be explained by resource exhaustion for pupal development of weapons [19,21], resource allocation between weapons and other body parts [39,40] or constraints of natural selection on costs in locomotion [41] and predation [42]. Alternatively, the difference in allometric slopes of mandibles between male morphs of C. mniszechi could be explained by the biomechanical functions of using mandibles as threat signals versus weapons [43]. The mandible opening behaviour of C. mniszechi could be used as a threat display to amplify the size difference between opponents. Whereas the functional advantages of a longer mandible may come from larger biting force or an enhanced ability to reach the rivals’ bodies to detach them. The reduced allometric slope in the mandibles of major males in C. mniszechi may represent a trade-off between these different functions [44].
We identified nine distinct behavioural elements in the fighting contests of C. mniszechi (Table 1), while an earlier study of the same species identified eleven distinct behaviours [8]. Eight of the eleven behaviours in Kuan 2011 [8] were observed in this study, the exceptions being “walk”, “approach” and “stand still”. The presence of these three behaviours may be an artefact of the trials being conducted directly on a horizontal tree log [8]; they were also observed in the fighting of C. metallifer stag beetles which used a similar setup [6]. We also observed a distinct “defensive posture” behaviour and separated “clamp” behaviour into “clamp 1” (head clamp) and “clamp 2” (thorax or abdomen clamp) to reflect the difference in the attacked body parts. “Encounter” in Kuan 2011 [8] was re-defined as ‘touch’ in our study to emphasise the physical contact between contestants. For C. mniszechi, “attack”, “push”, “clamp” and “tussle” are the four major fighting tactics used by males while competing with rivals. Among these actions, “attack” and “push” serve to keep the opponents away from the defended resources; “tussle” and “clamp” serve to flip the opponents or to dislodge them from the tree log. The same fighting tactics were also reported in congeneric C. metallifer stag beetles [6]. In addition, a peculiar shaking behaviour was observed in some winner males of C. metallifer immediately after the contests [6]. This shaking (pumping) behaviour was interpreted as intimidation by the winner male to display its dominance over the loser. Our observations in this study found that a few winner C. mniszechi males sometimes perform a similar shaking behaviour by lifting their wide-open mandibles up and down several times right after the contests. We suggest that this shaking behaviour in C. mniszechi may be an alert rather than intimidation behaviour because, immediately after the contests, the winner males were still very sensitive (motivated) and often responded to stimuli caused by the movement of loser males in the fighting area. The shaking behaviour might be involved in producing a substrate-borne vibrational signal which requires further study.
Our results suggest that the major and minor males of C. mniszechi employ the same behavioural elements in the fighting contests, but they differ in the extent to which they stay within phases in the contests and the likelihood of escalating into more aggressive tussle behaviour. Although the regression analyses found no significant effect of male morphs on the probability of tussle, the sequential analyses indicated that the major males were more likely to tussle and escalate directly into more aggressive phases, whereas the minor males tended to stay within the same phases. Our result contrasts with the observation in another stag beetle species, A. chelifer, in which the minor males tend to escalate more frequently than the major males [7]. Our findings suggest that the major males of C. mniszechi may have been selected to be more aggressive than minor males to compete in contests. The higher aggressiveness of major males in contests may be associated with using mandibles as weapons over threat signals to apply physical force and flip the rivals [43]. Within the minor morphs of C. mniszechi, the regression analysis indicated that males with larger mandibles were more likely to tussle which is consistent with the expectations from this weapon hypothesis. The elevated aggression in the major males can also be explained by their higher investment in the production of relatively larger weapons. The loss of fitness benefit through withdrawing from contests may have a higher cost for the major males, so they tend to be more aggressive than the minor males in contests. Larger males of stag beetles with longer weapons (more muscle mass) have disproportionally higher resting metabolic rates than smaller males with shorter weapons [45].

5. Conclusions

Stag beetles are charismatic insects known for displaying extremely diverse sizes and shapes in mandibles used as weapons in male contests for resources and reproduction. Size variation of stag beetles’ mandibles is important for determining their fighting behaviours and the outcomes of contests. Here, we found that two distinct male morphs of C. mniszechi employ the same behavioural elements but differ in their behavioural sequences in size-matched contests. Major males with larger mandibles are more likely to tussle and escalate directly into more aggressive phases, whereas minor males with smaller mandibles tend to stay within the same aggressive phases. Further research is needed to understand the underlying ecological and evolutionary mechanisms for producing this variation of contest behaviours.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/2075-4450/11/2/81/s1, (Version 2.0, 10.5281/zenodo.3603560): Table S1: The morphological measurements of males of Cyclommatus mniszechi used for allometry analyses. Table S2: The behavioural sequence data used for sequential analyses of size-matched contests in major males of Cyclommatus mniszechi. Table S3: The behavioural sequence data used for sequential analyses of size-matched contests in minor males of Cyclommatus mniszechi. Video S1: The behavioural sequence of males of Cyclommatus mniszechi in fighting contests under the laboratory setup. One of the opponents walked to the other and touched it (00:07), and then both of them displayed “defensive posture” (00:08) after “touch”. Once both individuals approached each other, they accelerated antennation and raised their mandibles and prothoracic parts. The contest then progressed into “body raising” (00:14). They then performed “attack” and “push” on each other several times and then escalated to “tussle” (00:20) and interlocked their mandibles until one of the contestants was clamped (“clamp1”) in the air by the other for a second and flipped (00:50). The winner dropped the loser and kept attacking and pushing the loser, while the loser retreated and moved backwards (00:51).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: C.-P.L., Y.H.; Methodology: Z.-Y.C., Y.H., C.-P.L.; Validation: Z.-Y.C., Y.H., C.-P.L.; Formal analysis: Z.-Y.C., Y.H.; Investigation: Z.-Y.C., Y.H., C.-P.L.; Resources: Z.-Y.C., Y.H., C.-P.L.; Data curation: Z.-Y.C.; Writing—original draft: Z.-Y.C.; Writing—review and editing: C.-P.L., Y.H., Z.-Y.C.; Visualization: Z.-Y.C.; Supervision: C.-P.L., Y.H.; Project administration: C.-P.L., Y.H.; Funding acquisition: C.-P.L., Y.H. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by research grants to C.-P.L. (MOST 106-2311-B-003-004-MY3 and 107-2311-B-003-002-MY3) and Y.H. (MOST 106-2621-B-003-001-MY3) from the Ministry of Science and Technology of Taiwan.

Acknowledgments

New Taipei City and the Forestry Bureau, Council of Agriculture, Executive Yuan of Taiwan issued research permits (No. 10606306500, 1062105547 and 1061152477). We thank three anonymous reviewers for comments on earlier versions of this manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Emlen, D.J. The evolution of animal weapons. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 2008, 39, 387–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Darwin, C. The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex; John Murray: London, UK, 1871. [Google Scholar]
  3. Andersson, M.B. Sexual Selection; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  4. Emlen, D.J.; Nijhout, H.F. The development and evolution of exaggerated morphologies in insects. Ann. Rev. Entomol. 2000, 45, 661–708. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Inoue, A.; Hasegawa, E. Effect of morph types, body size and prior residence on food-site holding by males of the male-dimorphic stag beetle Prosopocoilus inclinatus (Coleoptera: Lucanidae). J. Ethol. 2012, 31, 55–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Goyens, J.; Dirckx, J.; Aerts, P. Stag beetle battle behavior and its associated anatomical adaptations. J. Insect Behav. 2015, 28, 227–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Songvorawit, N.; Butcher, B.A.; Chaisuekul, C. Resource holding potential and the outcome of aggressive interactions between paired male Aegus chelifer chelifer (Coleoptera: Lucanidae) stag beetles. J. Insect Behav. 2018, 31, 347–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Kuan, C.Y. To win “big”: Determining Factors and Sequential Analyses in Male-Male Combats of a Stag Beetle, Cyclommatus mniszechi (Coleoptera: Lucanidae). Master’s Thesis, Tunghai University, Taichung City, Taiwan, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  9. Mizunuma, T.; Nagai, S. The Lucanid Beetles of the World; Mushisha: Tokyo, Japan, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  10. Kawano, K. Genera and allometry in the stag beetle family Lucanidae, Coleoptera. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 2000, 93, 198–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Shiokawa, T.; Iwahashi, O. Mandible dimorphism in males of a stag beetle, Prosopocoilus dissimilis okinawanus (Coleoptera: Lucanidae). Appl. Entomol. Zool. 2000, 35, 487–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Rowland, J.M.; Emlen, D.J. Two thresholds, three male forms result in facultative male trimorphism in beetles. Science 2009, 323, 773–776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Tomkins, J.L.; Kotiaho, J.S.; LeBas, N.R. Matters of scale: Positive allometry and the evolution of male dimorphisms. Am. Nat. 2005, 165, 389–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Bonduriansky, R. Sexual selection and allometry: A critical reappraisal of the evidence and ideas. Evolution 2007, 61, 838–849. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Kojima, W.; Lin, C.-P. It takes two to tango: Functional roles, sexual selection and allometry of multiple male weapons in the flower beetle Dicronocephalus wallichii bourgoini. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 2017, 121, 514–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Bonduriansky, R.; Day, T. The evolution of static allometry in sexually selected traits. Evolution 2003, 57, 2450–2458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Huxley, J.S. The relative size of antlers in deer. Proc. Zool. Soc. London 1931, 3, 819–864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Clark, J. Aspects of variation in the stag beetle Lucanus cervus (L.) (Coleoptera: Lucanidae). Syst. Entomol. 1977, 2, 9–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Knell, R.J.; Pomfret, J.C.; Tomkins, J.L. The limits of elaboration: Curved allometries reveal the constraints on mandible size in stag beetles. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 2004, 271, 523–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Romiti, F.; Tini, M.; Redolfi De Zan, L.; Chiari, S.; Zauli, A.; Carpaneto, G.M. Exaggerated allometric structures in relation to demographic and ecological parameters in Lucanus cervus (Coleoptera: Lucanidae). J. Morphol. 2015, 276, 1193–1204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Nijhout, H.; Wheeler, D.E. Growth models of complex allometries in holometabolous insects. Am. Nat. 1996, 148, 40–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Knell, R.J. On the analysis of non-linear allometries. Ecol. Entomol. 2009, 34, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Smith, A.B.T. A Review of the family-group names for the superfamily Scarabaeoidea (Coleoptera) with corrections to nomenclature and a current classification. Coleopt. Bull. 2006, 60, 144–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Okamoto, K.; Hongo, Y. Interspecific contests between males of two Japanese stag beetle species, Lucanus maculifemoratus and Prosopocoilus inclinatus: What overcomes a body size disadvantage? Behaviour 2013, 150, 39–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Kotiaho, J.S.; Tomkins, J.L. The discrimination of alternative male morphologies. Behav. Ecol. 2001, 12, 553–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Li, H.Y. Taiwanese Stag Beetles; Kissnature Publisher: Taipei, Taiwan, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  27. Kojima, W. Mechanism of synchronous metamorphosis: Larvae of a rhinoceros beetle alter the timing of pupation depending on maturity of their neighbours. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2014, 69, 415–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Kojima, W.; Ishikawa, Y.; Takanashi, T. Chemically mediated group formation in soil-dwelling larvae and pupae of the beetle Trypoxylus dichotomus. Naturwissenschaften 2014, 101, 687–695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Goyens, J.; Dirckx, J.; Aerts, P. Jaw morphology and fighting forces in stag beetles. J. Exp. Biol. 2016, 219, 2955–2961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  30. Songvorawit, N.; Butcher, B.A.; Chaisuekul, C. Different allometric intercepts in major Aegus chelifer chelifer stag beetle males from urban and forest habitats. J. Asia Pac. Entomol. 2017, 20, 835–839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Eberhard, W.G.; Gutiérrez, E.E. Male dimorphisms in beetles and earwigs and the question of developmental constraints. Evolution 1991, 45, 18–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Sugiura, S.; Yamaura, Y.; Makihara, H. Sexual and male horn dimorphism in Copris ochus (Coleoptera: Scarabaeidae). Zool. Sci. 2007, 24, 1082–1085. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Friard, O.; Gamba, M.; Fitzjohn, R. BORIS: A free, versatile open-source event-logging software for video/audio coding and live observations. Methods Ecol. Evol. 2016, 7, 1325–1330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Igraph-The Network Analysis Package. Available online: https://igraph.org (accessed on 1 April 2019).
  35. Green, P.A.; Patek, S.N. Mutual assessment during ritualized fighting in mantis shrimp (Stomatopoda). Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 2018, 285, 1871. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Bakeman, R.; Robinson, B.F.; Quera, V. Testing sequential association: Estimating exact p values using sampled permutations. Psychol. Methods 1996, 1, 4–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Hongo, Y. Evolution of male dimorphic allometry in a population of the Japanese horned beetle Trypoxylus dichotomus septentrionalis. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2007, 62, 245–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. McCullough, E.L.; Ledger, K.J.; O’Brien, D.M.; Emlen, D.J. Variation in the allometry of exaggerated rhinoceros beetle horns. Anim. Behav. 2015, 109, 133–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Emlen, D.J. Costs and the diversification of exaggerated animal structures. Science 2001, 291, 1534–1536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  40. Simmons, L.W.; Emlen, D.J. Evolutionary trade-off between weapons and testes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2006, 103, 16346–16351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Goyens, J.; Dirckx, J.; Aerts, P.; Davidowitz, G. Costly sexual dimorphism in Cyclommatus metallifer stag beetles. Funct. Ecol. 2015, 29, 35–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Kojima, W.; Sugiura, S.; Makihara, H.; Ishikawa, Y.; Takanashi, T. Rhinoceros beetles suffer male-biased predation by mammalian and avian predators. Zool. Sci. 2014, 31, 109–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Eberhard, W.G.; Rodríguez, R.L.; Huber, B.A.; Speck, B.; Miller, H.; Buzatto, B.A.; Machado, G. Sexual selection and static allometry: The importance of function. Q. Rev. Biol. 2018, 93, 207–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Dennenmoser, S.; Christy, J.H. The design of a beautiful weapon: Compensation for opposing sexual selection on a trait with two functions. Evolution 2013, 67, 1181–1188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. O’Brien, D.M.; Boisseau, R.P.; Duell, M.; McCullough, E.; Powell, E.C.; Somjee, U.; Solie, S.; Hickey, A.J.; Holwell, G.I.; Painting, C.J.; et al. Muscle mass drives cost in sexually selected arthropod weapons. Proc. R. Soc. B 2019, 286, 20191063. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. The allometric relationships between ln-transformed mandible length (ML) and elytra length (EL) of Cyclommatus mniszechi based on 34 collected and 198 reared males (n = 232). Solid grey lines are the linear regression lines of the major (circle, slope A = 1.43 ± 0.10) and minor (triangle, slope A = 2.89 ± 0.10). The 95% confidence intervals are shown in light grey. The dashed grey line is the switch point of ln mandible length at 2.64 (14.01 mm) based on Kotiaho and Tomkins [25] continuous piecewise model. In this study, males are divided allometrically into majors and minors above and below a mandible length of 14.01 mm, respectively. The black arrows point at the denticles on the mandibles which were used to classify males into alpha, beta and gamma morphs [8]. Alpha males have the largest mandibles with tusk-like projections (denticles) at their distal half. Beta males have smaller mandibles with denticles at their proximal halves and close to the base of the mandibles. Gamma males have the smallest saw-like mandibles with no apparent denticles. The major males contain mostly alpha morphs, and the minor males include beta and gamma morphs. The circles and triangles with black edges indicate the data points of the five beetles to their right.
Figure 1. The allometric relationships between ln-transformed mandible length (ML) and elytra length (EL) of Cyclommatus mniszechi based on 34 collected and 198 reared males (n = 232). Solid grey lines are the linear regression lines of the major (circle, slope A = 1.43 ± 0.10) and minor (triangle, slope A = 2.89 ± 0.10). The 95% confidence intervals are shown in light grey. The dashed grey line is the switch point of ln mandible length at 2.64 (14.01 mm) based on Kotiaho and Tomkins [25] continuous piecewise model. In this study, males are divided allometrically into majors and minors above and below a mandible length of 14.01 mm, respectively. The black arrows point at the denticles on the mandibles which were used to classify males into alpha, beta and gamma morphs [8]. Alpha males have the largest mandibles with tusk-like projections (denticles) at their distal half. Beta males have smaller mandibles with denticles at their proximal halves and close to the base of the mandibles. Gamma males have the smallest saw-like mandibles with no apparent denticles. The major males contain mostly alpha morphs, and the minor males include beta and gamma morphs. The circles and triangles with black edges indicate the data points of the five beetles to their right.
Insects 11 00081 g001
Figure 2. The fighting arena of Cyclommatus mniszechi used in this study: (a) overhead view, and (b) front view.
Figure 2. The fighting arena of Cyclommatus mniszechi used in this study: (a) overhead view, and (b) front view.
Insects 11 00081 g002
Figure 3. Sequential analyses of the contest behaviours of the major (n = 24) and minor (n = 31) males in size-matched contests of Cyclommatus mniszechi. Circles represent behavioural elements; circle size and colour are scaled to the percentage of total contest behaviours. Arrows represent significant behavioural transitions; non-significant behavioural transitions are not shown; arrow width is scaled to the transitional probability of the behaviours. Phases are coloured for visualisation.
Figure 3. Sequential analyses of the contest behaviours of the major (n = 24) and minor (n = 31) males in size-matched contests of Cyclommatus mniszechi. Circles represent behavioural elements; circle size and colour are scaled to the percentage of total contest behaviours. Arrows represent significant behavioural transitions; non-significant behavioural transitions are not shown; arrow width is scaled to the transitional probability of the behaviours. Phases are coloured for visualisation.
Insects 11 00081 g003
Table 1. Behavioural elements in the contest interactions among males of Cyclommatus mniszechi.
Table 1. Behavioural elements in the contest interactions among males of Cyclommatus mniszechi.
Behavioural ElementsDescriptionPhase
TouchOne of the contestants touches its opponent’s body with mandibles, antenna or legs.1
Defensive postureOne of the contestants keeps its mandibles open and raises its head.2
Body raisingTwo contestants face each other and raise their bodies, thoraxes and mandibles with rapid movement of antennas and forelegs and move towards each other.2
AttackOne of the contestants uses its mandibles to bite its opponent.2
PushOne of the contestants raises its mandibles and knocks its opponent.2
TussleTwo contestants interlock each other’s mandibles and push each other.3
Clamp 1One of the contestants uses its mandibles to clamp onto the head of its opponent.3
Clamp 2One of the contestants uses its mandibles to clamp onto the body (thorax or abdomen) of its opponent.3
RetreatOne of the contestants moves backwards and away from the other male.4
Table 2. Model selection of the allometry between mandible length (ML) and elytra length (EL) in Cyclommatus mniszechi.
Table 2. Model selection of the allometry between mandible length (ML) and elytra length (EL) in Cyclommatus mniszechi.
ParameterEstimateSEpAICΔAICBICΔBIC
Linear model
β0−4.3330.131<0.001−471.06503.32−460.73499.69
β12.5910.049<0.001
Quadratic model
β0−20.2502.297<0.001−513461.38−499.56460.86
β114.6981.745<0.001
β2−2.2980.331<0.001
Eberhard and Gutierrez continuous piecewise
β0−5.3720.165<0.001−535.14439.24−521.35439.07
β12.9970.063<0.001
β2−1.6270.187<0.001
Eberhard and Gutierrez discontinuous piecewise
β0−5.3670.188<0.001−533.14441.24−515.91444.51
β12.9940.073<0.001
β2−1.633−1.633<0.001
β30.0010.0190.95
Kotiaho and Tomkins linear
β01.7470.017<0.001−931.4042.98−921.0639.36
β10.3560.007<0.001
Kotiaho and Tomkins quadratic
β02.3590.114<0.001−957.6916.69−943.9116.51
β1−0.1560.0940.0987
β20.1050.019<0.001
Kotiaho and Tomkins continuous piecewise
β01.8600.023<0.001−974.210.17−960.420
β10.3050.010<0.001
β20.2020.029<0.001
Kotiaho and Tomkins discontinuous piecewise
β01.8770.025<0.001−974.380−957.153.27
β10.2970.011<0.001
β20.1790.033<0.001
β30.0110.0070.144
Table 3. Parameters estimated for the isometry tests in Cyclommatus mniszechi (ML = mandible length, HW = head width, A = allometric slope, lna = intercept).
Table 3. Parameters estimated for the isometry tests in Cyclommatus mniszechi (ML = mandible length, HW = head width, A = allometric slope, lna = intercept).
MorphTraitsCoefficientsEstimate ± SE95% Lower Limit95% Upper Limitp
MajorML   A   1.43 ± 0.101.241.62<0.001 *
lna−1.13 ± 0.27−1.66−0.60
HW   A   1.30 ± 0.071.171.440.001 *
lna−0.94 ± 0.18−1.30−0.57
MinorML   A   2.89 ± 0.102.693.10<0.001 *
lna−5.12 ± 0.27−5.65−4.58
HW   A   1.97 ± 0.061.862.080.001 *
lna−2.77 ± 0.14−3.06−2.49
* p-value < 0.05.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Chen, Z.-Y.; Hsu, Y.; Lin, C.-P. Allometry and Fighting Behaviour of a Dimorphic Stag Beetle Cyclommatus mniszechi (Coleoptera: Lucanidae). Insects 2020, 11, 81. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects11020081

AMA Style

Chen Z-Y, Hsu Y, Lin C-P. Allometry and Fighting Behaviour of a Dimorphic Stag Beetle Cyclommatus mniszechi (Coleoptera: Lucanidae). Insects. 2020; 11(2):81. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects11020081

Chicago/Turabian Style

Chen, Zhen-Yi, Yuying Hsu, and Chung-Ping Lin. 2020. "Allometry and Fighting Behaviour of a Dimorphic Stag Beetle Cyclommatus mniszechi (Coleoptera: Lucanidae)" Insects 11, no. 2: 81. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects11020081

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop