The Impact of Order Effects on the Framing of Trust and Reciprocity Behaviors
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Results
2.1. Control Measures
2.2. Game Measures
2.3. Expectation Measures
3. Discussion
3.1. Framing Effects for First Trust and Reciprocity Decisions
3.2. Order Effects in Trust and Reciprocity Decisions
3.3. The Impact of the Order on Framing of Trust and Reciprocity Decisions
4. Materials and Methods
4.1. Participants
4.2. Experimental Tasks
4.2.1. Trust Games
4.2.2. Expectation Tasks
4.2.3. Self-Report Control Questionnaires
4.3. Procedure
4.4. Statistical Analyses
4.4.1. Control Measures
4.4.2. Game Decision Measures
4.4.3. Expectation Measures
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Fehr, E.; Schurtenberger, I. Normative Foundations of Human Cooperation. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2018, 2, 458–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fehr, E.; Fischbacher, U. Social Norms and Human Cooperation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2004, 8, 185–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alós-Ferrer, C.; Farolfi, F. Trust Games and Beyond. Front. Neurosci. 2019, 887. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Rousseau, D.M.; Sitkin, S.B.; Burt, R.S.; Camerer, C. Not so Different after All: A Cross-Discipline View of Trust. Acad. Manag. Rev. 1998, 23, 393–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fareri, D.S. Neurobehavioral Mechanisms Supporting Trust and Reciprocity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2019, 13, 271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fetchenhauer, D.; Lang, A.; Ehlebracht, D.; Schlösser, T.; Dunning, D. Does Betrayal Aversion Really Guide Trust Decisions towards Strangers? J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2020, 33, 556–566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Krueger, F.; Bellucci, G.; Xu, P.; Feng, C. The Critical Role of the Right Dorsal and Ventral Anterior Insula in Reciprocity: Evidence From the Trust and Ultimatum Games. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2020, 14, 176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bellucci, G.; Feng, C.; Camilleri, J.; Eickhoff, S.B.; Krueger, F. The Role of the Anterior Insula in Social Norm Compliance and Enforcement: Evidence from Coordinate-Based and Functional Connectivity Meta-Analyses. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018, 92, 378–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bellucci, G.; Chernyak, S.V.; Goodyear, K.; Eickhoff, S.B.; Krueger, F. Neural Signatures of Trust in Reciprocity: A Coordinate-based Meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2017, 38, 1233–1248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Snijders, C.; Keren, G. Do You Trust? Whom Do You Trust? When Do You Trust? Adv. Gr. Process. 2001, 18, 129–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Czerlinski, J.; Gigerenzer, G.; Goldstein, D. How Good Are Simple Heuristics? In Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1999; pp. 97–118. [Google Scholar]
- Evans, A.M.; Krueger, J.I. Bounded Prospection in Dilemmas of Trust and Reciprocity. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2016, 20, 17–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Evans, A.M.; Athenstaedt, U.; Krueger, J.I. The Development of Trust and Altruism during Childhood. J. Econ. Psychol. 2013, 36, 82–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Evans, A.M.; Krueger, J.I. Elements of Trust: Risk and Perspective-Taking. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 47, 171–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Evans, A.M.; Krueger, J.I. Outcomes and Expectations in Dilemmas of Trust. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2014, 9, 90–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bohnet, I.; St Meier, S. Deciding to Distrust (Working Paper); Kennedy School of Government: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Kahneman, D.; Tversky, A. Prospect Theory: Analysis of Decision under Risk. Econometrica 1979, 4, 99–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hertwig, R.; Herzog, S.M. Fast and Frugal Heuristics: Tools of Social Rationality. Soc. Cogn. 2009, 27, 661–698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cui, F.; YANG, J.; GU, R.; LIU, J. Functional Connectivities of the Right Temporoparietal Junction and Moral Network Predict Social Framing Effect: Evidence from Resting-State FMRI. Acta Psychol. Sin. 2021, 53, 55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sellitto, M.; Neufang, S.; Schweda, A.; Weber, B.; Kalenscher, T. Arbitration between Insula and Temporoparietal Junction Subserves Framing-Induced Boosts in Generosity during Social Discounting. Neuroimage 2021, 238, 118211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liu, J.; Gu, R.; Liao, C.; Lu, J.; Fang, Y.; Xu, P.; Luo, Y.; Cui, F. The Neural Mechanism of the Social Framing Effect: Evidence from FMRI and TDCS Studies. J. Neurosci. 2020, 40, 3646–3656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berg, J.; Dickhaut, J.; McCabe, K. Trust, Reciprocity, and Social History. Games Econ. Behav. 1995, 10, 122–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Zhang, Y.; Zhang, Y.; Wu, Y.; Krueger, F. Default Matters in Trust and Reciprocity. Games 2023, 14, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fang, H.; Liao, C.; Fu, Z.; Tian, S.; Luo, Y.; Xu, P.; Krueger, F. Connectome-Based Individualized Prediction of Reciprocity Propensity and Sensitivity to Framing: A Resting-State Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study. Cereb. Cortex 2022, 4, bhac269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Reimann, M.; Schilke, O.; Cook, K.S. Trust Is Heritable, Whereas Distrust Is Not. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2017, 114, 7007–7012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- McCabe, K.A.; Rigdon, M.L.; Smith, V.L. Positive Reciprocity and Intentions in Trust Games. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2003, 52, 267–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rabin, M. Incorporating Fairness into Game Theory and Economics. Am. Econ. Rev. 1993, 83, 1281–1302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Baron, J.; Ritov, I. Reference Points and Omission Bias. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1994, 59, 475–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Day, B.; Bateman, I.J.; Carson, R.T.; Dupont, D.; Louviere, J.J.; Morimoto, S.; Scarpa, R.; Wang, P. Ordering Effects and Choice Set Awareness in Repeat-Response Stated Preference Studies. J. Environ. Econ. Manag. 2012, 63, 73–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perreault, W.D. Controlling order-effect bias. Public Opin. Q. 1975, 39, 544–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luchins, A.S.; Luchins, E.H. Anchorage and Ordering Effects of Information on Personality Impression. J. Soc. Psychol. 1965, 66, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moore, D.W. Measuring New Types of Question-Order Effects: Additive and Subtractive. Public Opin. Q. 2002, 66, 80–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davison, E.; Xiao, Y.; Yan, H. Response Order Biases in Economic Surveys. Public Health Policy eJournal 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moore, D.A. Order Effects in Preference Judgments: Evidence for Context Dependence in the Generation of Preferences. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1999, 78, 146–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Furnham, A.; Boo, H.C. A Literature Review of the Anchoring Effect. J. Socio-Econ. 2011, 40, 35–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Herriges, J.A.; Shogren, J.F. Starting Point Bias in Dichotomous Choice Valuation with Follow-Up Questioning. J. Environ. Econ. Manag. 1996, 30, 112–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tversky, A.; Kahneman, D. Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Science 1974, 185, 1124–1131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ariely, D.; Loewenstein, G.; Prelec, D. “Coherent Arbitrariness”: Stable Demand Curves Without Stable Preferences. Q. J. Econ. 2003, 118, 73–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Epley, N.; Gilovich, T. Putting Adjustment Back in the Anchoring and Adjustment Heuristic: Differential Processing of Self-Generated and Experimenter-Provided Anchors. Psychol. Sci. 2001, 12, 391–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oyebode, B.; Nicholls, N. Social Norms as Anchor Points for Trust. Soc. Sci. J. 2020, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eskinazi, N.; Malul, M.; Rosenboim, M.; Shavit, T. An Experimental Study of the Effect of the Anchor of the Option’s Underlying Asset on Investors’ Pricing Decisions. J. Behav. Financ. 2022, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yu, J.; Zhu, L.; Leslie, A.M. Children’s Sharing Behavior in Mini-Dictator Games: The Role of In-Group Favoritism and Theory of Mind. Child Dev. 2016, 87, 1747–1757. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wittig, M.; Jensen, K.; Tomasello, M. Five-Year-Olds Understand Fair as Equal in a Mini-Ultimatum Game. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2013, 116, 324–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gummerum, M.; Hanoch, Y.; Keller, M.; Parsons, K.; Hummel, A. Preschoolers’ Allocations in the Dictator Game: The Role of Moral Emotions. J. Econ. Psychol. 2010, 31, 25–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rodriguez-Lara, I. Equity and Bargaining Power in Ultimatum Games. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2016, 130, 144–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Zhang, L.; Ortmann, A. On the Interpretation of Giving, Taking, and Destruction in Dictator Games and Joy-of-Destruction Games. UNSW Bus. Sch. Res. Pap. Ser. 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Xiao, Q.; Hu, C.; Wang, T. Mindfulness Practice Makes Moral People More Moral. Mindfulness (N. Y.) 2020, 11, 2639–2650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Adler, N.E.; Epel, E.S.; Castellazzo, G.; Ickovics, J.R. Relationship of Subjective and Objective Social Status with Psychological and Physiological Functioning: Preliminary Data in Healthy, White Women. Health Psychol. 2000, 586–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maples, J.L.; Guan, L.; Carter, N.T.; Miller, J.D. A Test of the International Personality Item Pool Representation of the Revised NEO Personality Inventory and Development of a 120-Item IPIP-Based Measure of the Five-Factor Model. Psychol. Assess. 2014, 26, 1070–1084. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davis, M. A Multidimensional Approach to Individual Differences in Empathy. JSAS Cat. Sel. Doc. Psychol. 1980, 10. [Google Scholar]
- Perugini, M.; Gallucci, M.; Presaghi, F.; Ercolani, A.P. The Personal Norm of Reciprocity. Eur. J. Pers. 2003, 17, 251–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pagdin, R.; Salkovskis, P.M.; Nathwani, F.; Wilkinson-Tough, M.; Warnock-Parkes, E. ‘I Was Treated like Dirt’: Evaluating Links between Betrayal and Mental Contamination in Clinical Samples. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 2020, 49, 21–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Johnson, N.D.; Mislin, A.A. Trust Games: A Meta-Analysis. J. Econ. Psychol. 2011, 32, 865–889. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Trustor Groups | Trustee Groups | Statistics | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
G-T (n = 40) | T-G (n = 43) | G-T (n = 41) | T-G (n = 47) | F-Value | p-Value | ||
Demographics | |||||||
Age | 19.09 ± 1.02 | 19.67 ± 1.85 | 19.57 ± 1.57 | 19.67 ± 1.77 | 1.14 | 0.33 | 0.01 |
Gender | 1.49 ± 0.50 | 1.48 ± 0.50 | 1.49 ± 0.50 | 1.50 ± 0.50 | 0.01 | 0.99 | 0.00 |
SES | 5.47 ± 1.07 | 5.22 ± 1.50 | 4.98 ± 1.37 | 5.33 ± 1.38 | 0.90 | 0.43 | 0.01 |
NEO Personality Inventory | |||||||
Trust Trait | 28.05 ± 4.60 | 27.48 ± 4.77 | 27.36 ± 5.20 | 27.67 ± 4.95 | 0.21 | 0.89 | 0.00 |
Interpersonal Reactivity Index | |||||||
Perspective Taking | 17.63 ± 3.92 | 17.59 ± 3.29 | 17.55 ± 3.55 | 19.19 ± 3.51 | 2.35 | 0.07 | 0.03 |
Fantasy | 18.37 ± 4.30 | 17.00 ± 4.15 | 15.91 ± 4.36 | 16.29 ± 3.97 | 1.13 | 0.34 | 0.01 |
Empathic Concern | 19.23 ± 3.56 | 19.26 ± 3.85 | 17.70 ± 4.52 | 18.65 ± 3.85 | 1.54 | 0.20 | 0.02 |
Personal Distress | 14.77 ± 3.86 | 15.09 ± 4.20 | 14.51 ± 3.99 | 14.27 ± 3.42 | 0.48 | 0.69 | 0.00 |
Dispositional Moral Sensitivity Questionnaire | |||||||
Empathic Guilt | 23.60 ± 5.59 | 23.43 ± 6.38 | 21.98 ± 6.96 | 23.60 ± 5.79 | 0.78 | 0.50 | 0.01 |
Tendency to Punishment | 12.67 ± 6.05 | 12.39 ± 5.77 | 11.70 ± 4.51 | 13.54 ± 5.19 | 1.15 | 0.33 | 0.01 |
Intrusiveness of Empathy | 16.47 ± 4.98 | 16.28 ± 6.22 | 14.94 ± 6.73 | 15.81 ± 5.25 | 0.74 | 0.52 | 0.01 |
Frequency of Perception | 9.30 ± 4.53 | 9.91 ± 4.96 | 8.57 ± 3.56 | 10.19 ± 3.63 | 1.61 | 0.18 | 0.02 |
Sympathetic Imagination | 9.09 ± 2.82 | 8.35 ± 4.21 | 7.34 ± 3.57 | 8.02 ± 3.54 | 2.01 | 0.11 | 0.03 |
Beliefs in Reciprocity | 39.81 ± 8.37 | 41.54 ± 8.00 | 39.45 ± 7.61 | 39.98 ± 5.74 | 1.10 | 0.35 | 0.01 |
Personal Norm of Reciprocity | |||||||
Positive Reciprocity | 52.26 ± 7.01 | 50.98 ± 7.29 | 51.45 ± 6.63 | 51.92 ± 5.66 | 0.33 | 0.80 | 0.00 |
Negative Reciprocity | 34.86 ± 9.83 | 35.02 ± 10.30 | 33.28 ± 9.22 | 35.58 ± 7.18 | 0.80 | 0.49 | 0.01 |
Perception of Betrayal Scale | |||||||
Preoccupation with Betrayal Events | 32.95 ± 12.58 | 31.30 ± 11.6 | 34.89 ± 13.50 | 37.46 ± 12.32 | 1.72 | 0.16 | 0.02 |
Betrayal Causing Life Change | 21.93 ± 7.66 | 21.59 ± 8.29 | 20.34 ± 7.63 | 23.35 ± 8.52 | 0.94 | 0.41 | 0.01 |
Lack of Trust Due to Betrayal | 23.37 ± 7.31 | 24.22 ± 8.01 | 21.74 ± 8.14 | 24.40 ± 8.27 | 1.05 | 0.37 | 0.01 |
Betrayal Leading to Traumatic Changes | 14.23 ± 5.10 | 12.96 ± 5.21 | 14.51 ± 6.23 | 14.83 ± 5.25 | 0.95 | 0.41 | 0.01 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Bayat, D.; Mohamadpour, H.; Fang, H.; Xu, P.; Krueger, F. The Impact of Order Effects on the Framing of Trust and Reciprocity Behaviors. Games 2023, 14, 21. https://doi.org/10.3390/g14020021
Bayat D, Mohamadpour H, Fang H, Xu P, Krueger F. The Impact of Order Effects on the Framing of Trust and Reciprocity Behaviors. Games. 2023; 14(2):21. https://doi.org/10.3390/g14020021
Chicago/Turabian StyleBayat, Davood, Hadi Mohamadpour, Huihua Fang, Pengfei Xu, and Frank Krueger. 2023. "The Impact of Order Effects on the Framing of Trust and Reciprocity Behaviors" Games 14, no. 2: 21. https://doi.org/10.3390/g14020021
APA StyleBayat, D., Mohamadpour, H., Fang, H., Xu, P., & Krueger, F. (2023). The Impact of Order Effects on the Framing of Trust and Reciprocity Behaviors. Games, 14(2), 21. https://doi.org/10.3390/g14020021