Next Article in Journal
Heavy Metals in the Environment and Thyroid Cancer
Next Article in Special Issue
Etiologies of Melanoma Development and Prevention Measures: A Review of the Current Evidence
Previous Article in Journal
Laryngeal Neural Monitoring during Pediatric Thyroid Cancer Surgery—Is Transcartilage Recording a Preferable Method?
Previous Article in Special Issue
Age and Cohort Trends of Malignant Melanoma in the United States
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Behavioral and Psychological Outcomes Associated with Skin Cancer Genetic Testing in Albuquerque Primary Care

1
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Memorial Sloan Kettering Cancer Center, New York, NY 10065, USA
2
Cancer Communication Research, Huntsman Cancer Center, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT 84112, USA
3
Klein Buendel, Inc., Golden, CO 80401, USA
4
New Mexico VA Health System, Veterans Health Administration, Albuquerque, NM 87108, USA
5
Department of Family and Community Medicine, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87102, USA
6
University of New Mexico Comprehensive Cancer Center, Albuquerque, NM 87102, USA
7
Department of Anthropology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87102, USA
8
Department of Internal Medicine, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87102, USA
9
Department of Dermatology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87102, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Cancers 2021, 13(16), 4053; https://doi.org/10.3390/cancers13164053
Submission received: 10 June 2021 / Revised: 28 July 2021 / Accepted: 3 August 2021 / Published: 12 August 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Melanoma: Prevention and Molecular Epidemiology)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Genetic information is publicly available but research examining the best use of such information has not engaged diverse members of the public. We examined public reactions to melanoma genetic testing (using the melanocortin-1 receptor [MC1R] gene) in a study randomizing (like the flip of a coin) 600 diverse primary care patients to a MC1R test offer or usual care. We found that testing did not improve sun protection and skin cancer screening, nor did it lead worry to increase. However, groups less aware of their skin cancer risk, including those who thought their risk was “unlikely” at the start of the study, showed significant improvements in sun protection at three months. In conclusion, testing might be very helpful for certain people who have the most to learn about their risk, who may become motivated to protect themselves from the damaging effects of the sun as a result of skin cancer genetic testing.

Abstract

Public availability of genetic information is increasing; thus, efforts to improve diversity in basic and translational research in genomics is a top priority. Given the increasing U.S. incidence and mortality of melanoma, and the prevalence of common melanocortin-1 receptor (MC1R) gene melanoma risk variants in the general population, we examined genomic testing of MC1R for skin cancer risk in a randomized controlled trial in Albuquerque, New Mexico primary care. Participants were 48% Hispanic and were randomized 5:1 to a MC1R test invitation or usual care. We assessed 3 month sun protection, skin cancer screening, and skin cancer worry outcomes associated with testing, and key effect moderators (e.g., cancer risk perceptions, and skin cancer risk factors). Our findings indicate that the primary outcomes were unchanged by the MC1R test offer, test acceptance, and level of risk feedback. Moderator analyses showed that those with lower risk perception, and those with skin that readily tans, significantly increased their sun protection in response to higher than average risk feedback. Risk feedback did not prompt cancer worry, and average risk feedback did not erode existing sun protection. This study paves the way for the development of tailored strategies to address low skin cancer risk awareness in this understudied context of public health genomics.

1. Introduction

Genetic information regarding health risk, and cancer risk specifically, is available through multiple sources—both within and outside of the U.S. medical and public health systems, and there are relatively high levels of public awareness concerning this availability [1]. Genetic information about cancer risk is of interest to the general population, and this interest stems from a range of motives, including personal curiosity, empowerment regarding health, efforts to enhance cancer prevention and control, as well as to enhance family awareness and cancer risk reduction [1]. Direct-to-consumer (DTC) genetic testing has been available for several decades. By 2021, it is estimated that more genetic testing will be conducted outside than inside the traditional health system [2,3]. Genetic information provided by DTC channels is controversial in the medical community, constituting criticisms regarding test validity, utility and comprehensibility of results, as well as the potential to increase worry and poor outreach to diverse populations [4]. Nonetheless, 40% of the population is aware of DTC testing [5,6], and utilization has expanded accordingly [7]. As of 2018, the consumer genomics industry estimated that 12+ million individuals had submitted samples for DTC genetic testing. By early 2019, an estimated 26 million people contributed their genetic results to one of the large consumer genomics databases (e.g., AncestryDNA and 23andMe) [2]. NIH also is collecting biospecimens on one million members of the population, targeting recruitment to racial and ethnic minorities in order to further understanding of gene–environment interactions and the ideal return of results through the All of Us Research Project (https://allofus.nih.gov/) (accessed: 26 July 2021). Costs for sequencing have been decreasing rapidly [8]. Despite the scale achieved for DTC genetic testing, the health promotion opportunities, particularly in diverse populations [9], that are afforded by this momentum have not yet been realized. Although members of racial and ethnic minority groups stand to benefit from genomic technologies, to date, those who pursue personalized genetic information have been disproportionately non-Hispanic White and at higher socioeconomic, education, and literacy levels [10]. Additionally, DTC testing has largely been conducted outside the healthcare system; the potential for some integration into established healthcare could improve access for underserved populations, and present enhanced opportunities for links between genetic test result feedback and counseling regarding health promotion and maintenance.
Melanoma is the 5th most common cancer in the U.S., and its incidence has tripled in the last four decades _ENREF_1 [11,12,13,14,15]. Furthermore, over 4.3 million adults are treated for keratinocyte cancers (i.e., squamous cell carcinoma (SCC) and basal cell carcinoma (BCC)) annually in the U.S. [16], and incidence is increasing steadily [17,18]. Despite low associated mortality, keratinocyte cancers are common and expensive to treat [19]. _ENREF_23 Most melanoma is caused by ultraviolet radiation (UVR), largely from sun exposure [20,21,22,23]. Limiting UVR exposure is estimated to substantially reduce melanoma rates [20]. Keratinocyte cancers share UVR exposure as a major risk factor [24,25].
The melanocortin-1 receptor (MC1R) gene encodes a protein that signals the production of melanins. MC1R exhibits great genetic variability, and a large proportion of individuals of European ancestry, including Hispanics, carry at least one MC1R risk variant [26,27,28]. The inheritance of specific MC1R variants is a robust marker of increased risk of melanoma and keratinocyte skin cancers [26,29,30,31]. Associations between MC1R variants and skin cancer are stronger in individuals with “sun-resistant” phenotypic characteristics (e.g., darker hair, scant freckling) than in those with “sun-sensitive” characteristics (e.g., red hair, easy burning, freckling) [26,27]. The theoretical reduction of melanomas through the “removal” of one of the nine most prevalent MC1R variants (i.e., attributable risk) ranges from 1.2 to 8.9% and approaches 40% when summed across variants [26]; this proportion is 30% and 45% for basal and squamous cell skin cancers, respectively [31]. Variants tested for, and accession numbers, include the following: V60L (rs1805005), D84E (rs1805006), V92M (rs2228479), R142H (rs11547464), R151C (rs1805007), I155T (rs1110400), R160W (rs1805008), R163Q (rs885479) and D294H (rs1805009). As such, the MC1R genotype conveys information about inherited skin cancer risk for a broad spectrum of individuals, including Hispanics. Most (50–70%) of the general population of European ancestry and 50% of Hispanics have at least one risk variant at MC1R [26,28], even in individuals with darker complexions [32,33], making genetic testing for MC1R variants a potential novel route to motivate sun protection and skin cancer screening in the general population. In New Mexico, specifically, those who identify as Hispanic derive from mating between Native Americans (resident in the region for at least 13,000 years) and Europeans and Africans, who arrived over the past 400 years. Recent studies indicate that the majority of ancestry is European (mean = 71%), followed by Native American (27%) and African (2%) [34,35,36]. Given the documented frequency of MC1R variants in Europe (26–28), many New Mexican Hispanics today carry MC1R variants.
Genomic testing of MC1R for skin cancer risk may promote behavior change across ethnically and racially diverse populations [37], especially among Hispanics, who generally have low skin cancer risk awareness [38,39,40,41,42,43]. Meta-analytic work has shown that genetic information motivates behavior change and does not generate high levels of distress or worry [44], yet the use and impact of genetic information in diverse community settings [45] is extremely understudied [46]. This randomized controlled trial (“SOMBRA: Skin Health Online for Melanoma: Better Risk Assessment”) was conducted in a diverse primary care population in Albuquerque, New Mexico, which experiences year-round sun exposure [47]. We examined 3 month sun protection, skin cancer screening, and skin cancer worry outcomes associated with MC1R test offer and completion. Our research questions were as follows. First, what are the 3 month outcomes (i.e., overall sun protection, skin cancer screening, cancer worry) associated with the receipt of higher (compared to average) risk MC1R feedback? We also examined outcomes associated with receiving a MC1R test offer (compared to usual care) and accepting that offer (compared to declining the offer). Second, were there significant moderators of the relationship between the receipt of higher vs. average genetic risk findings and the 3 month outcomes (i.e., overall sun protection, skin cancer screening, cancer worry)? Potential moderators included participant health literacy, perceived importance of genetic information, cancer risk perceptions, demographics, and skin cancer risk factors, all of which may influence behavioral and psychosocial outcomes associated with skin cancer genetic testing [48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55].

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Bilingual Project Assistants (PAs) approached primary care patients in University of New Mexico (UNM) outpatient primary care clinics with SOMBRA study invitation flyers (English and Spanish) and National Cancer Institute skin cancer information for diverse skin types (available in English and Spanish versions: “Anyone can get skin cancer”) [56]. Patients were eligible if they were registered in any UNM clinic for at least six months, assigned a UNM primary care provider, were aged 18 years or older, and fluent in English or Spanish. All study procedures and materials were approved by the University of New Mexico (UNM) Institutional Review Board (ClinicalTrials.gov identifier: NCT03130569).

2.2. Procedure

Patients who were eligible completed a one-minute screening survey that assessed perceived importance in learning about genes and health risks, skin cancer risk perceptions, and demographics. Refusers were asked their reasons for study refusal. Those willing to participate completed the informed consent and baseline assessment and were then randomized 5:1 to an invitation to consider MC1R testing or usual care (control group), balanced across Hispanic vs. non-Hispanic ethnicities. Overall, 600 participants enrolled in the RCT and completed the baseline assessment; 499 were randomized to the MC1R testing offer; and 101 were randomized to usual care. Usual care participants did not receive an invitation to log on to the study website. All participants received National Cancer Institute skin cancer information brochures about skin cancer risk in diverse skin types (available in both English and Spanish) [47]. See Figure 1 for study design.
Those randomized to the intervention arm were invited to log onto the study website [47] to read the three educational modules regarding MC1R testing and then register their decision to participate in testing. Patients without internet access were offered the opportunity to view the website via paper format. Participants who chose testing were mailed a kit to obtain their DNA, and if they so desired, provided a saliva sample for genetic testing. Those participants who sent in their sample received a report with their results in the form of genetic risk feedback stating that they were at either average risk, based on the presence of no MC1R variants associated with melanoma risk, or higher risk, based on the presence of at least one MC1R variant associated with risk of developing melanoma [55]. The design of this risk feedback, using plain language and clear communication guidelines, was adapted from the Multiplex Initiative study [57]. Participants who opted for testing received a follow-up telephone call two weeks after receipt of their results, for an assessment of comprehension, satisfaction with risk information, and distress level; these findings are reported elsewhere [55]. Primary outcomes were assessed at three months. Those participants randomized to usual care were offered MC1R testing as a courtesy after completion of the final 3 month follow-up outcome assessment, and 57 of 101 usual care participants requested it.

2.3. Measures

2.3.1. Outcome Measures

Sun protection: The frequency of use of five sun protective behaviors while in the sun—sunscreen, shade-seeking, hats, protective clothing, and sunglasses—were reported at baseline and again at the 3 month follow-up. Participants rated each behavior on a 5-point frequency scale ranging from 1 = Never to 5 = Always [58]. We calculated overall sun protection (summary score including all 5 sun protection strategies ranged from 5 to 25) as a primary outcome variable.
Skin cancer screening: At the baseline assessment, we assessed whether participants had ever had their skin checked for skin cancer (from head to toe) by a healthcare professional. At the 3 month follow-up, they were asked whether they had received skin cancer screening since they had started the study.
Cancer worry: Worry items were drawn from Lerman and colleagues [59] on 4-point scales, including the frequency of worry about the possibility of getting skin cancer in the past two weeks (1 = rarely/never to 4 = all the time), and the level of concern about the possibility of developing skin cancer (1 = not at all to 4 = very concerned); a summary score ranged from 2 to 8.

2.3.2. Predictors

We examined three predictors. First, we explored the outcomes associated with the receipt of an MC1R test offer (vs. usual care). Second, among those who received the test offer, we examined the outcomes associated with accepting that offer (vs. declining that offer). Test offer acceptance was defined as logging on to the website and requesting a test kit. Declining the test offer included those who actively declined the test offer on the study website, as well as those who did not log on to the website at all. Third, of those who completed testing by accepting the test offer, we tested the outcomes associated with the receipt of higher (vs. average) risk test results. See Figure 1.

2.3.3. Moderators

We included participant health literacy, perceived importance of genetic information, cancer risk perceptions, demographics, and skin cancer risk factors in the models as potential effect moderators. Health literacy was assessed with three items: (1) level of confidence in filling out medical forms independently, (2) frequency of needed assistance reading hospital materials, and (3) frequency of problems learning about medical conditions because of difficulty reading hospital materials (1 = not at all/none of the time to 5 = extremely/all the time). These items have _ENREF_133 good sensitivity across diverse literacy levels [60,61]. Moderators were assessed through the use of interaction terms, using standardized effect sizes, rather than the use of p-values for hypotheses testing, to avoid the problem of multiple comparisons.
Perceived importance in learning about ones’ genes and associated health risks was assessed (1 = not at all important to 7 = very important) [53]. Perceived skin cancer risk was assessed using three widely used scales [62,63], including two absolute verbal likelihood scales (1 = no chance to 7 = certain to happen; and 1 = unlikely or 2 = likely to happen) with “don’t know” response options [64]. A comparative likelihood assessment (1 = well below average to 5 = well above average, compared to individuals of participants’ age and sex) was also included. Demographics included ethnicity (participants were asked if they were of Hispanic, Latino/a, or Spanish origin), race, sex, educational attainment, age, and income. Skin cancer risk factors included history of cancer, family history of skin cancer, skin type (burnability, tannability), and intermittent sun exposure (lifetime number of sunburns) [65,66].

2.4. Biostatistical Approach

Sun protection and cancer worry were treated as continuous 3 month outcomes, and skin cancer screening as a dichotomous outcome. Attrition between the baseline and 3 month follow-up was described; completers were compared to non-completers based on baseline characteristics. Differences by attrition were tested via a series of independent sample t-tests and Chi-square tests, for continuous and categorical variables, respectively. The general framework for analysis was generalized regression modeling, regressing 3 month outcomes on predictors (i.e., intervention arm, test acceptance, and risk results) and adjusting for baseline. For our first research question, the 3 month outcomes were compared across MC1R test offer (compared to usual care), test acceptance (compared to declined), and receipt of higher (compared to average risk results). Models were fitted both with and without adjustment for baseline characteristics that differed based on attrition status. For our second research question, another series of regression models were used to assess moderator effects with the receipt of higher, compared to average, risk test results on 3 month outcomes, separately. For these models, the baseline outcome, potential moderator (e.g., participant health literacy), test results, and interaction of moderator and test results were included. Eta-squared (η2) in a regression model is the ratio of the variance explained by a parameter of interest. The interaction term was the parameter of interest, with η2 reported as a measure of standardized effect sizes for continuous outcomes and standardized odds ratios for the binary skin exam outcome. This strategy of using standardized effect sizes and then probing non-trivial associations, rather than merely relying on p-values, reduced the risk of inflated family-wise error rate when investigating a large number of potential moderators [67]. For η2 in multiple regression, values of 0.02, 0.13, and 0.26 were considered small, medium, and large effect sizes, respectively. For interaction odds ratios (iOR) with standardized variables and rare outcomes, values of 1.68, 3.47, and 6.71 were considered small, medium, and large effects [68]. Significant findings were investigated with stratified analyses. All statistical analyses were conducted in SAS version 9.4.

3. Results

As reported previously [69], 1998 primary care patients were approached, 917 (46%) agreed to be screened for eligibility, and of the 726 patients who were eligible for the study, 621 (86%) consented to study participation; 21 did not complete the baseline assessment. Study acceptance was higher in non-Hispanic whites compared to Hispanics, and those with higher (>high school) compared to lower educational attainment (<high school) but did not differ on other demographic factors [69]. Further, those with higher perceived skin cancer risk and those with greater interest in learning about genes were more likely to participate. Participants who answered “don’t know” to absolute perceived skin cancer risk were less likely to participate in the study than patients who gave another scale response; these psychosocial factors remained significant predictors of study participation after adjustment for ethnicity and education [54].
Of the 600 study participants, 79% were female, 48% Hispanic, 71% white, and 77% had more than a high school education. Most (n = 494, 82%) completed a 3 month follow-up. Participants who were not retained at the 3 month follow-up were more likely to be Hispanic (63% vs. 45%, p < 0.01), have lower educational attainment (p < 0.01) or income (p < 0.01), have no family history of cancer (79% vs. 63%, p < 0.01), have responded “don’t know” to absolute perceived risk (18% vs. 9%, p < 0.01), and have rated their risk as “unlikely” at baseline (65% vs. 48%, p = 0.02). Overall, those who completed the study were predominantly female (79%), white (72%), had at least some college education (80%), and were 53 years of age (mean = 53.2, SD = 14.0). Baseline levels of sun protection, skin screening, and cancer worry were unrelated to the study completion status. Baseline characteristics both overall and by study completion status are shown in Table 1 and Table 2.
At baseline, only one quarter (24%) of participants had sun protection scores less than 15 across the five behaviors; the mean score was 17 (on a 5 to 25 scale), which translates to somewhere between “sometimes” and “often.” However, only one third (37%) of the participants had ever received skin cancer screening, and skin cancer worry was extremely low (mean = 2.5, SD = 1.1 on a 2 to 8 scale).
Most participants reported little to no change in all outcomes between the baseline and 3 month follow-up. For example, the median sun protection change score was zero (inter-quartile range −2 to 2; possible range −20 to +20), and only 5% of participants had a change score of more than five points in either direction. Similarly, only 10% of participants who had never had skin screening received one in the study time period, and cancer worry scores increased slightly, by 0.03 points on average. In models adjusted for baseline outcomes only, the predictors (test offer, test completion, and test feedback level) were not significantly associated with sun protection, skin cancer screening, or cancer worry at 3 months (Table 3). The baseline scores were strong predictors for most of these 3 month outcome measures. Models adjusted for participant characteristics that varied by attrition (i.e., ethnicity, education, income, family skin cancer history, and perceived risk) showed results consistent with the pattern outlined above.
Most moderators had no significant interaction effects with level of MC1R risk feedback (higher vs. average risk) on outcomes; however, some of these effects stood apart with small but significant effect sizes. See Table 4.
Tannability (η2 = 0.03; p = 0.01) and perceived risk (absolute, dichotomous; η2 = 0.02; p = 0.04) were significant moderators of the effect of risk feedback on sun protection. The same risk perception item had a non-trivial effect size (η2 = 0.05; p = 0.20) for moderation of risk feedback level on cancer worry. These moderations effects are depicted in Figure 2, where the distribution of change scores for sun protection are plotted by the risk feedback level and the moderators. For participants who reported that their skin does tan, those who received higher risk MC1R feedback reported significantly greater improvement in sun protection (p = 0.02) than those who received average risk MC1R feedback. For participants who believed they were unlikely to develop skin cancer at baseline, those with higher risk feedback had significantly greater improvement in sun protection (p = 0.04), compared to those who received average risk feedback. These associations did not hold for participants who reported that their skin did not tan or who reported at baseline a high likelihood of skin cancer risk. Though the stratified results for risk perception were not significant for cancer worry, the same pattern held (i.e., among those who believed that they were unlikely to develop skin cancer at baseline, those with higher risk feedback had greater increase in worry). Different patterns of moderators were seen for the effect of MC1R risk feedback level on the skin cancer screening outcome. Educational attainment, history of sunburn, and family skin cancer history stood apart as potential moderators after controlling for skin screening history, based on effect sizes. First, among those who had never received prior skin cancer screening, higher risk feedback was associated with skin cancer screening completion at 3 months among those with lower educational attainment (iOR = 8.32, p = 0.11). Cell sizes were too small to make similar inference for participants who had previously screened. Second, regardless of their skin cancer screening history, among those who had no history of sunburn, receipt of average risk results predicted 3 month skin cancer screening (iOR = 36.97, p = 0.01). Third, the family history effect was complex. Among those who had never been screened, only those with a family history of skin cancer and higher risk MC1R, or those with no family history of skin cancer and average risk MC1R, completed any screening at follow up. Among those who had never been screened, none of the participants who received only higher risk feedback, or had a family history of skin cancer, reported screening at the follow up time period (iOR = 16.65, p = 0.05).

4. Discussion

The provision of genetic feedback information to the general population is controversial, with concerns raised about the potential value of such feedback for health promotion and medical decision making, as well as whether such information might prompt health worry or distress. At the same time, the availability of genetic information to the general public has increased dramatically in recent years [3], so exploring the public reaction to such information is needed across diverse populations who have the potential to benefit from it [4]. A large proportion of individuals of European ancestry, including New Mexico Hispanics, carry at least one MC1R gene variant that confers increased melanoma risk [26,27,28], although recent evidence found that the risk conferred by such variants may differ by specific Hispanic populations [70]. Nonetheless, the inheritance of specific MC1R variants may be a robust marker of increased risk of melanoma and keratinocyte skin cancers [26,29,30,31]. In the current study, receiving higher (compared to average) risk feedback did not increase sun protection or skin cancer screening at three months. Provision of a test offer, and completion of testing, also did not influence subsequent sun protection. Given the concerns that genetic information provided by DTC channels may increase worry [4], our findings are promising in that worry outcomes were also not influenced by MC1R test offer, completion, risk feedback level. These findings are especially important, given the primary care environment utilized, with about half the sample identifying as Hispanic, a population that has received little attention in the context of DTC testing [71].
In our study population, sun protection utilization was already relatively high at baseline assessment, perhaps because Albuquerque has year-round sun exposure, which may serve to support sun protection habits over time. As such, the impact of offering MC1R testing, completing testing, and receiving risk feedback results may have been suppressed due to ceiling effects, creating reduced opportunity for behavioral impact. In previous analyses, participants who completed testing reported high levels of interest and engagement in their test feedback results and reported determination to improve sun protection behavior after receiving feedback, and such determination was significantly higher in those who received higher risk feedback, compared to average risk feedback two weeks after test receipt [55]. Those with a sunburn history were more likely to complete testing [69]. It may also be that there are elements of behavioral impact that were not measured in the current study that might ultimately contribute to the personal and health utility of MC1R feedback, such as longer-term behavioral habits, improved engagement and perceived control over sun protection choices. Fortunately, our findings indicated that receipt of average risk MC1R feedback did not lead to the reduction or elimination of sun protection. Caution has been raised about whether provision of average risk results may provide risk behavior justification [72]. These findings provide some solace that the provision of “normal” or “good news” genetic test results did not undermine protective behavior, consistent with prior work in highly educated samples [73]. Findings on the sun protection outcomes are inconsistent with meta-analytic evidence supporting the fact that genetic test findings positively impact motivation for health promotion [44], yet they nonetheless shape the important questions going forward in the area of genomics and behavior change, as well as precision approaches to cancer prevention that engage multiple types of personalized risk information, more broadly.
Based on moderation analyses examining interactions, our findings indicate some specific groups that may be particularly responsive to higher risk feedback in increasing their sun protection. These include adults with high skin tannability, and those who, at baseline, judged their skin cancer risk as “unlikely.” These individuals may have been unaware of their risk and may be more influenced by the motivational impact of the risk feedback; it may also be that unexpected or surprising results are more motivational, which is an important topic for further research. Theories of risk communication predict that messages that increase fear, such as high-risk MC1R test feedback, should motivate people to take precautions that they perceive will reduce their risk, such as sun protection [74,75]. Consistent with this, moderation analyses found that skin cancer worry was somewhat higher for those who judged their baseline skin cancer risk as “unlikely” and received higher versus average risk feedback. The risk communication theories predict that the increased surprise in learning of a higher risk produces discomfort that can be reduced by taking more sun safety precautions. Further work should examine both the psychosocial and behavioral outcomes of MC1R testing. As such, a subgroup approach, consistent with precision prevention efforts, may be a useful strategy for future research since some subgroups may benefit more from skin cancer genetic risk feedback than others.
Further, skin cancer worry was not increased by MC1R testing. This helps allay concerns [44] that the provision of such feedback may increase health worries, and/or distress, particularly in the absence of detailed genetic counseling, and in DTC testing of the general population outside of the high-risk setting. While the current study examined 3 month skin cancer worry outcomes, the findings are consistent with demonstrated short-term impact of testing; distress and negative effects were low, and satisfaction with testing and receipt of results were quite high two weeks after receiving results [55].
Future research should target subgroups of the general population who have the most to gain from skin cancer risk information that is highly personalized and may thus carry the most unique motivational potential. This may include selection for those at highest behavioral risk, such as those who engage in indoor or outdoor intentional tanning, do not use sun protection, have a history of multiple sunburns, or have low skin cancer risk awareness (e.g., low perceived risk; reports that they “don’t know” their risk). Further, selection for youth in secondary schools and universities to heighten awareness of skin cancer is a promising area for future research. This may include large portions of the general population who tan and/or do not use adequate sun protection [76]. In the current study, patients with low risk perceptions were significantly more likely to refuse study participation than those with higher risk perception [54], which may have decreased the ability to detect an overall impact of the genetic testing feedback. It is an important element of study rigor that we were able to well-characterize these study refusers. Strategies are needed to recruit populations with misplaced low risk perception who would likely benefit behaviorally from the receipt of higher-risk feedback. This finding highlights the importance of delineating the motives and attitudes of those who actively pursue testing versus those who decline it, as we did in the current study. It is also important to further specify whether such risk information must be paired with specific intervention content to enhance it, such as information to support the efficacy of sun protection to reduce risk and the ability of individuals to adequately practice sun protection over time, across diverse situations and contexts, to achieve these benefits [77].
The current study has limitations as well as strengths. First, we assessed primary care patients in only one area of the country, which already had a generally high prevalence of practicing sun protection, which reduces generalizability. Second, we relied on self-reported sun protection, although self-report has been found to be a valid indicator of objective sun protection usage [78]. Future work should confirm and expand on these findings using objective measures for UV exposure and objective sunburn, even though self-reports are appropriate for assessing cognitions, such as risk perceptions, the importance of genetic testing, and skin cancer worry. Third, given our study design and recruitment of participants, regardless of their interest in following through with MC1R testing, we had relatively small samples of those who received feedback. While this limited the statistical power to test moderation on risk feedback (higher versus average risk), our study design allowed for examination of behavioral and psychosocial outcomes critical to real-world use of skin cancer genetic testing feedback, where those who follow through with testing may differ significantly from those who do not. Future use of larger samples will also allow for further examination of moderators, such as differences in educational attainment. These important influences on behavior may not be measurable in study designs where all participants recruited are already interested in pursuing genetic testing. Our exceptionally high recruitment rates (86% of eligible primary care patients) were a study strength, along with the careful measurement of interest in, and completion of, testing; most (88%) who visited the website to consider testing went ahead and ordered a test kit [69]. Recruitment of only those who were already interested in and willing to undergo skin cancer genetic testing may have resulted in a much smaller, self-selected sample; as such, our rigorous approach closely matches the current real-world situation, where genetic technologies are available but voluntary.

5. Conclusions

Efforts to improve diversity in genomic research, both basic and translational, are a top priority [79]. The current study examined an understudied element of public health genomics [80], genetic testing for common susceptibility and behavioral and psychosocial outcomes in a diverse, primary care patient population. In particular, the study successfully engaged Hispanics in discussion and engagement with skin cancer genetic testing. We did not find main effects for MC1R testing feedback on behavioral outcomes, including sun protection and skin cancer screening, and similarly did not find any increase in skin cancer worry, an important and promising finding. However, we identified moderators with relatively large effect sizes, including low risk perceptions, which suggest targets for the deployment of tailored strategies, using skin cancer genetic testing in diverse subpopulations that may be most readily impacted by risk feedback.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.L.H. and M.B.; methodology, J.L.H., M.B.; formal analysis, E.S. and Y.L.; investigation, J.L.H., K.A.K., D.B., K.M.W., A.S., D.G., Y.T.D., E.R., M.R.S., K.H., M.B.; data curation, K.M.W. and E.R.; writing—original draft preparation, J.L.H., E.S.; writing—review and editing, J.L.H., K.A.K., D.B., E.S., K.M.W., A.S., D.G., Y.T.D., E.R., M.R.S., Y.L., K.H., M.B.; visualization, E.S.; supervision, J.L.H. and M.B.; project administration, K.M.W., A.S. and D.G.; funding acquisition, J.L.H. and M.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

Financial support for this study was provided by a grant from the National Institutes of Health, NIH R01 CA181241 to Jennifer Hay and Marianne Berwick (MPIs). Additionally, this research was partially supported by UNM Comprehensive Cancer Center Support Grant NCI P30CA118100 and the Behavioral Measurement and Population Sciences shared resource. We also acknowledge the MSK Support/Core Grant (NCI P30CA008748). The funding agreement ensured the authors’ independence in designing the study, interpreting the data, writing, and publishing the report.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board of The University of New Mexico (protocol code HRRC#15-050; date of approval: 12 May 2015).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study. Written informed consent was obtained from the patient(s) to publish this paper.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge the valued guidance of Deborah J. Bowen and Colleen McBride in initial study inception, and Fiona Puccio, in manuscript preparation.

Conflicts of Interest

David Buller is the Research Director of Klein Buendel and his spouse is an owner of Klein Buendel. The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Alvord, T.W.; Marriott, L.K.; Nguyen, P.T.; Shafer, A.; Brown, K.; Stoller, W.; Volpi, J.L.; Vandehey-Guerrero, J.; Ferrara, L.K.; Blakesley, S.; et al. Public perception of predictive cancer genetic testing and research in Oregon. J. Genet. Couns. 2020, 29, 259–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Regalado, A. More than 26 million people have taken an at-home ancestry test. MIT Technol. Rev. 2019, 11, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  3. Feero, W.G.; Wicklund, C.A. Consumer Genomic Testing in 2020. JAMA 2020, 323, 1445–1446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine; Health and Medicine Division; Board on Health Sciences Policy; Roundtable on Genomics and Precision Health. Exploring the Current Landscape of Consumer Genomics: Proceedings of a Workshop; The National Academies Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  5. Agurs-Collins, T.; Ferrer, R.; Ottenbacher, A.; Waters, E.A.; O’Connell, M.E.; Hamilton, J.G. Public Awareness of Direct-to-Consumer Genetic Tests: Findings from the 2013 U.S. Health Information National Trends Survey. J. Cancer Educ. 2015, 30, 799–807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Apathy, N.C.; Menser, T.; Keeran, L.M.; Ford, E.W.; Harle, C.A.; Huerta, T.R. Trends and Gaps in Awareness of Direct-to-Consumer Genetic Tests from 2007 to 2014. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2018, 54, 806–813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Carroll, N.M.; Blum-Barnett, E.; Madrid, S.D.; Jonas, C.; Janes, K.; Alvarado, M.; Bedoy, R.; Paolino, V.; Aziz, N.; McGlynn, E.A.; et al. Demographic differences in the utilization of clinical and direct-to-consumer genetic testing. J. Genet. Couns. 2019, 29, 634–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Wetterstrand, K. DNA Sequencing Costs: Data. Available online: https://www.genome.gov/about-genomics/fact-sheets/DNA-Sequencing-Costs-Data (accessed on 26 July 2021).
  9. Hay, J.L.; Bowers, J.M.; Hamilton, J.G. Genomics and behavior change. In The Handbook of Health Psychology, 3rd ed.; Revenson, T.A., Gurung, R.A.R., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2019; pp. 474–483. [Google Scholar]
  10. Bloss, C.S.; Ornowski, L.; Silver, E.; Cargill, M.; Vanier, V.; Schork, N.J.; Topol, E.J. Consumer perceptions of direct-to-consumer personalized genomic risk assessments. Genet. Med. 2010, 12, 556–566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Howlader, N.; Krapcho, M.; Garshell, J.; Miller, D.; Altekruse, S.F.; Kosary, C.L.; Yu, M.; Ruhl, J.; Tatalovich, Z.; Mariotto, A.; et al. SEER Cancer Statistics Review, 1975–2012; National Cancer Institute: Bethesda, MD, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  12. Weir, H.K.; Marrett, L.D.; Cokkinides, V.; Barnholtz-Sloan, J.; Patel, P.; Tai, E.; Jemal, A.; Li, J.; Kim, J.; Ekwueme, D.U. Melanoma in adolescents and young adults (ages 15–39 years): United States, 1999–2006. J. Am. Acad. Dermatol. 2011, 65, S38–S49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Watson, M.; Geller, A.C.; Tucker, M.A.; Guy, G.P., Jr.; Weinstock, M.A. Melanoma burden and recent trends among non-Hispanic whites aged 15–49 years, United States. Prev. Med. 2016, 91, 294–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. American Cancer Society. Types of Cancers That Develop in Young Adults. 2019. Available online: https://www.cancer.org/cancer/cancer-in-young-adults/cancers-in-young-adults.html (accessed on 26 July 2021).
  15. American Cancer Society. Cancer Facts and Figures. 2020. Available online: https://www.cancer.org/content/dam/cancer-org/research/cancer-facts-and-statistics/annual-cancer-facts-and-figures/2020/cancer-facts-and-figures-2020.pdf (accessed on 26 July 2021).
  16. Rogers, H.W.; Weinstock, M.A.; Feldman, S.R.; Coldiron, B.M. Incidence Estimate of Nonmelanoma Skin Cancer (Keratinocyte Carcinomas) in the U.S. Population, 2012. JAMA Dermatol. 2015, 151, 1081–1086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Karagas, M.R.; Greenberg, E.R.; Spencer, S.K.; Stukel, T.A.; Mott, L.A. Increase in incidence rates of basal cell and squamous cell skin cancer in New Hampshire, USA. New Hampshire Skin Cancer Study Group. Int. J. Cancer 1999, 81, 555–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Muzic, J.G.; Schmitt, A.R.; Wright, A.C.; Alniemi, D.T.; Zubair, A.S.; Olazagasti Lourido, J.M.; Sosa Seda, I.M.; Weaver, A.L.; Baum, C.L. Incidence and Trends of Basal Cell Carcinoma and Cutaneous Squamous Cell Carcinoma: A Population-Based Study in Olmsted County, Minnesota, 2000 to 2010. Mayo Clin. Proc. 2017, 92, 890–898. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Guy, G.P., Jr.; Machlin, S.R.; Ekwueme, D.U.; Yabroff, K.R. Prevalence and costs of skin cancer treatment in the U.S., 2002–2006 and 2007–2011. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2015, 48, 183–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  20. Armstrong, B.K.; Kricker, A. How much melanoma is caused by sun exposure? Melanoma Res. 1993, 3, 395–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Gandini, S.; Sera, F.; Cattaruzza, M.S.; Pasquini, P.; Picconi, O.; Boyle, P.; Melchi, C.F. Meta-analysis of risk factors for cutaneous melanoma: II. Sun exposure. Eur. J. Cancer 2005, 41, 45–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Gandini, S.; Autier, P.; Boniol, M. Reviews on sun exposure and artificial light and melanoma. Prog. Biophys. Mol. Biol. 2011, 107, 362–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Chang, Y.M.; Barrett, J.H.; Bishop, D.T.; Armstrong, B.K.; Bataille, V.; Bergman, W.; Berwick, M.; Bracci, P.M.; Elwood, J.M.; Ernstoff, M.S.; et al. Sun exposure and melanoma risk at different latitudes: A pooled analysis of 5700 cases and 7216 controls. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2009, 38, 814–830. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Madan, V.; Lear, J.T.; Szeimies, R.M. Non-melanoma skin cancer. Lancet 2010, 375, 673–685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Kricker, A.; Weber, M.; Sitas, F.; Banks, E.; Rahman, B.; Goumas, C.; Kabir, A.; Hodgkinson, V.S.; van Kemenade, C.H.; Waterboer, T.; et al. Early Life UV and Risk of Basal and Squamous Cell Carcinoma in New South Wales, Australia. Photochem. Photobiol. 2017, 93, 1483–1491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Pasquali, E.; Garcia-Borron, J.C.; Fargnoli, M.C.; Gandini, S.; Maisonneuve, P.; Bagnardi, V.; Specchia, C.; Liu, F.; Kayser, M.; Nijsten, T.; et al. MC1R variants increased the risk of sporadic cutaneous melanoma in darker-pigmented Caucasians: A pooled-analysis from the M-SKIP project. Int. J. Cancer 2015, 136, 618–631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Kanetsky, P.A.; Panossian, S.; Elder, D.E.; Guerry, D.; Ming, M.E.; Schuchter, L.; Rebbeck, T.R. Does MC1R genotype convey information about melanoma risk beyond risk phenotypes? Cancer 2010, 116, 2416–2428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Smit, A.K.; Collazo-Roman, M.; Vadaparampil, S.T.; Valavanis, S.; Del Rio, J.; Soto, B.; Flores, I.; Dutil, J.; Kanetsky, P.A. MC1R variants and associations with pigmentation characteristics and genetic ancestry in a Hispanic, predominately Puerto Rican, population. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 7303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Raimondi, S.; Sera, F.; Gandini, S.; Iodice, S.; Caini, S.; Maisonneuve, P.; Fargnoli, M.C. MC1R variants, melanoma and red hair color phenotype: A meta-analysis. Int. J. Cancer 2008, 122, 2753–2760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Gerstenblith, M.R.; Shi, J.; Landi, M.T. Genome-wide association studies of pigmentation and skin cancer: A review and meta-analysis. Pigment. Cell Melanoma Res. 2010, 23, 587–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Tagliabue, E.; Fargnoli, M.C.; Gandini, S.; Maisonneuve, P.; Liu, F.; Kayser, M.; Nijsten, T.; Han, J.; Kumar, R.; Gruis, N.A.; et al. MC1R gene variants and non-melanoma skin cancer: A pooled-analysis from the M-SKIP project. Br. J. Cancer 2015, 113, 354–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  32. Landi, M.T.; Kanetsky, P.A.; Tsang, S.; Gold, B.; Munroe, D.; Rebbeck, T.; Swoyer, J.; Ter-Minassian, M.; Hedayati, M.; Grossman, L.; et al. MC1R, ASIP, and DNA repair in sporadic and familial melanoma in a Mediterranean population. J. Natl. Cancer Inst. 2005, 97, 998–1007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Scherer, D.; Nagore, E.; Bermejo, J.L.; Figl, A.; Botella-Estrada, R.; Thirumaran, R.K.; Angelini, S.; Hemminki, K.; Schadendorf, D.; Kumar, R. Melanocortin receptor 1 variants and melanoma risk: A study of 2 European populations. Int. J. Cancer 2009, 125, 1868–1875. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Bonilla, C.; Parra, E.J.; Pfaff, C.L.; Dios, S.; Marshall, J.A.; Hamman, R.F.; Ferrell, R.E.; Hoggart, C.L.; McKeigue, P.M.; Shriver, M.D. Admixture in the Hispanics of the San Luis Valley, Colorado, and its implications for complex trait gene mapping. Ann. Hum. Genet. 2004, 68, 139–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Healy, M.; Edgar, H.; Mosley, C.; Hunley, K. Associations between ethnic identity, regional history, and genomic ancestry in New Mexicans of Spanish-speaking descent. Biodemography Soc. Biol. 2018, 64, 152–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Klimentidis, Y.C.; Miller, G.F.; Shriver, M.D. Genetic admixture, self-reported ethnicity, self-estimated admixture, and skin pigmentation among Hispanics and Native Americans. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 2009, 138, 375–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Kanetsky, P.A.; Hay, J.L. Marshaling the Translational Potential of MC1R for Precision Risk Assessment of Melanoma. Cancer Prev. Res. 2018, 11, 121–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Rodriguez, V.M.; Shuk, E.; Arniella, G.; Gonzalez, C.J.; Gany, F.; Hamilton, J.G.; Gold, G.S.; Hay, J.L. A Qualitative Exploration of Latinos’ Perceptions about Skin Cancer: The Role of Gender and Linguistic Acculturation. J. Cancer Educ. 2017, 32, 438–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Coups, E.J.; Stapleton, J.L.; Manne, S.L.; Hudson, S.V.; Medina-Forrester, A.; Rosenberg, S.A.; Gordon, M.; Tatum, K.S.; Robinson, J.K.; Natale-Pereira, A.; et al. Psychosocial correlates of sun protection behaviors among U.S. Hispanic adults. J. Behav. Med. 2014, 37, 1082–1090. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Friedman, L.C.; Bruce, S.; Weinberg, A.D.; Cooper, H.P.; Yen, A.H.; Hill, M. Early detection of skin cancer: Racial/ethnic differences in behaviors and attitudes. J. Cancer Educ. 1994, 9, 105–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Hu, S.; Parmet, Y.; Allen, G.; Parker, D.F.; Ma, F.; Rouhani, P.; Kirsner, R.S. Disparity in melanoma: A trend analysis of melanoma incidence and stage at diagnosis among whites, Hispanics, and blacks in Florida. Arch. Dermatol. 2009, 145, 1369–1374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Pipitone, M.; Robinson, J.K.; Camara, C.; Chittineni, B.; Fisher, S.G. Skin cancer awareness in suburban employees: A Hispanic perspective. J. Am. Acad. Dermatol. 2002, 47, 118–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Robinson, J.K.; Joshi, K.M.; Ortiz, S.; Kundu, R.V. Melanoma knowledge, perception, and awareness in ethnic minorities in Chicago: Recommendations regarding education. Psychooncology 2011, 20, 313–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Frieser, M.J.; Wilson, S.; Vrieze, S. Behavioral impact of return of genetic test results for complex disease: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Psychol. 2018, 37, 1134–1144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Oh, A.; Vinson, C.A.; Chambers, D.A. Future directions for implementation science at the National Cancer Institute: Implementation Science Centers in Cancer Control. Transl. Behav. Med. 2020, 11, 669–675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Roberts, M.C.; Clyne, M.; Kennedy, A.E.; Chambers, D.A.; Khoury, M.J. The current state of funded NIH grants in implementation science in genomic medicine: A portfolio analysis. Genet. Med. 2019, 21, 1218–1223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Hay, J.L.; Berwick, M.; Zielaskowski, K.; White, K.A.; Rodriguez, V.M.; Robers, E.; Guest, D.D.; Sussman, A.; Talamantes, Y.; Schwartz, M.R.; et al. Implementing an Internet-Delivered Skin Cancer Genetic Testing Intervention to Improve Sun Protection Behavior in a Diverse Population: Protocol for a Randomized Controlled Trial. JMIR Res. Protoc 2017, 6, 52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Kutner, M.; Greenberg, E.; Jin, Y.; Paulsen, C. The Health Literacy of America’s Adults: Results from the 2003 National Assessment of Adult Literacy (NCES 2006-483); U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics: Washington, DC, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  49. Viswanath, K.; Nagler, R.H.; Bigman-Galimore, C.A.; McCauley, M.P.; Jung, M.; Ramanadhan, S. The communications revolution and health inequalities in the 21st century: Implications for cancer control. Cancer Epidemiol. Biomarkers Prev. 2012, 21, 1701–1708. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Lorence, D.; Park, H. Group disparities and health information: A study of online access for the underserved. Health Inform. J. 2008, 14, 29–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Lorence, D.P.; Park, H.; Fox, S. Racial disparities in health information access: Resilience of the Digital Divide. J. Med. Syst. 2006, 30, 241–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Hall, M.J.; Manne, S.L.; Myers, R.E.; Keenan, E.M.; Balshem, A.M.; Weinberg, D.S. Predictors of patient uptake of colorectal cancer gene environment risk assessment. Genome Med. 2012, 4, 92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  53. Hay, J.; Kaphingst, K.A.; Baser, R.; Li, Y.; Hensley-Alford, S.; McBride, C.M. Skin cancer concerns and genetic risk information-seeking in primary care. Public Health Genom. 2012, 15, 57–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Hay, J.L.; Meyer White, K.; Sussman, A.; Kaphingst, K.; Guest, D.; Schofield, E.; Dailey, Y.T.; Robers, E.; Schwartz, M.R.; Zielaskowski, K.; et al. Psychosocial and Cultural Determinants of Interest and Uptake of Skin Cancer Genetic Testing in Diverse Primary Care. Public Health Genom. 2019, 22, 58–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Kaphingst, K.A.; Khan, E.; White, K.M.; Sussman, A.; Guest, D.; Schofield, E.; Dailey, Y.T.; Robers, E.; Schwartz, M.R.; Li, Y.; et al. Effects of health literacy skills, educational attainment, and level of melanoma risk on responses to personalized genomic testing. Patient Educ. Couns. 2021, 104, 12–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. National Cancer Institute. Anyone Can Get Skin Cancer; National Cancer Institute: Bethesda, MD, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  57. Kaphingst, K.A.; McBride, C.M.; Wade, C.; Alford, S.H.; Reid, R.; Larson, E.; Baxevanis, A.D.; Brody, L.C. Patients’ understanding of and responses to multiplex genetic susceptibility test results. Genet. Med. 2012, 14, 681–687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Glanz, K.; Yaroch, A.L.; Dancel, M.; Saraiya, M.; Crane, L.A.; Buller, D.B.; Manne, S.; O’Riordan, D.L.; Heckman, C.J.; Hay, J.; et al. Measures of sun exposure and sun protection practices for behavioral and epidemiologic research. Arch. Dermatol. 2008, 144, 217–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Lerman, C.; Trock, B.; Rimer, B.K.; Jepson, C.; Brody, D.; Boyce, A. Psychological side effects of breast cancer screening. Health Psychol. 1991, 10, 259–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Chew, L.D.; Griffin, J.M.; Partin, M.R.; Noorbaloochi, S.; Grill, J.P.; Snyder, A.; Bradley, K.A.; Nugent, S.M.; Baines, A.D.; Vanryn, M. Validation of screening questions for limited health literacy in a large VA outpatient population. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 2008, 23, 561–566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  61. Chew, L.D.; Bradley, K.A.; Boyko, E.J. Brief questions to identify patients with inadequate health literacy. Fam. Med. 2004, 36, 588–594. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  62. Nelson, D.E.; Kreps, G.L.; Hesse, B.W.; Croyle, R.T.; Willis, G.; Arora, N.K.; Rimer, B.K.; Viswanath, K.V.; Weinstein, N.; Alden, S. The Health Information National Trends Survey (HINTS): Development, design, and dissemination. J. Health Commun. 2004, 9, 443–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Weinstein, N.D. Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems. J. Behav. Med. 1982, 5, 441–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Waters, E.A.; Hay, J.L.; Orom, H.; Kiviniemi, M.T.; Drake, B.F. “Don’t know” responses to risk perception measures: Implications for underserved populations. Med. Decis. Making 2013, 33, 271–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Fitzpatrick, T.B. The validity and practicality of sun-reactive skin types I through VI. Arch. Dermatol. 1988, 124, 869–871. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Gandini, S.; Sera, F.; Cattaruzza, M.S.; Pasquini, P.; Abeni, D.; Boyle, P.; Melchi, C.F. Meta-analysis of risk factors for cutaneous melanoma: I. Common and atypical naevi. Eur. J. Cancer 2005, 41, 28–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Garamszegi, L.Z. Comparing effect sizes across variables: Generalization without the need for Bonferroni correction. Behav. Ecol. 2006, 17, 682–687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Chen, H.; Cohen, P.; Chen, S. How Big is a Big Odds Ratio? Interpreting the Magnitudes of Odds Ratios in Epidemiological Studies. Commun. Stat.—Simul. Comput. 2010, 39, 860–864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Hay, J.L.; Zielaskowski, K.; Meyer White, K.; Kaphingst, K.; Robers, E.; Guest, D.; Sussman, A.; Talamantes, Y.; Schwartz, M.; Rodriguez, V.M.; et al. Interest and Uptake of MC1R Testing for Melanoma Risk in a Diverse Primary Care Population: A Randomized Clinical Trial. JAMA Dermatol. 2018, 154, 684–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. White, K.A.M.; Dailey, Y.T.; Guest, D.D.; Zielaskowski, K.; Robers, E.; Sussman, A.; Hunley, K.; Hughes, C.R.; Schwartz, M.R.; Kaphingst, K.A.; et al. MC1R Variation in a New Mexico Population. Cancer Epidemiol. Biomark. Prev. 2019, 28, 1853–1856. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  71. Chavez-Yenter, D.; Chou, W.S.; Kaphingst, K.A. State of recent literature on communication about cancer genetic testing among Latinx populations. J. Genet. Couns 2020, 30, 911–918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. McBride, C.M.; Koehly, L.M.; Sanderson, S.C.; Kaphingst, K.A. The behavioral response to personalized genetic information: Will genetic risk profiles motivate individuals and families to choose more healthful behaviors? Annu. Rev. Public Health 2010, 31, 89–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Bloss, C.S.; Schork, N.J.; Topol, E.J. Effect of direct-to-consumer genomewide profiling to assess disease risk. N. Engl. J. Med. 2011, 364, 524–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  74. Rogers, R.W.; Prentice-Dunn, S. Protection motivation theory. In Handbook of Health Behavior Research 1: Personal and Social Determinants; Plenum Press: New York, NY, USA, 1997; pp. 113–132. [Google Scholar]
  75. Witte, K. Putting the fear back into fear appeals: The extended parallel process model. Commun. Monogr. 1992, 59, 329–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Holman, D.M.; Ding, H.; Guy, G.P., Jr.; Watson, M.; Hartman, A.M.; Perna, F.M. Prevalence of Sun Protection Use and Sunburn and Association of Demographic and Behaviorial Characteristics with Sunburn Among US Adults. JAMA Dermatol. 2018, 154, 561–568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Hay, J.L.; Shuk, E.; Schofield, E.; Loeb, R.; Holland, S.; Burkhalter, J.; Li, Y. Real-time sun protection decisions in first-degree relatives of melanoma patients. Health Psychol. 2017, 36, 907–915. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Glanz, K.; Gies, P.; O’Riordan, D.L.; Elliott, T.; Nehl, E.; McCarty, F.; Davis, E. Validity of self-reported solar UVR exposure compared with objectively measured UVR exposure. Cancer Epidemiol. Biomark. Prev. 2010, 19, 3005–3012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  79. Denny, J.C.; Collins, F.S. Precision medicine in 2030-seven ways to transform healthcare. Cell 2021, 184, 1415–1419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Roberts, M.C.; Kennedy, A.E.; Chambers, D.A.; Khoury, M.J. The current state of implementation science in genomic medicine: Opportunities for improvement. Genet. Med. 2017, 19, 858–863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Study design. Note: of the 499 who received a log-on invitation, 37 requested but did not return a kit, and 1 returned a kit but no risk level was recorded.
Figure 1. Study design. Note: of the 499 who received a log-on invitation, 37 requested but did not return a kit, and 1 returned a kit but no risk level was recorded.
Cancers 13 04053 g001
Figure 2. Depiction of notable sun protection and skin cancer worry moderator effects.
Figure 2. Depiction of notable sun protection and skin cancer worry moderator effects.
Cancers 13 04053 g002aCancers 13 04053 g002b
Table 1. Patient baseline characteristics and testing results by follow-up status (N = 600).
Table 1. Patient baseline characteristics and testing results by follow-up status (N = 600).
Ethnicity, HispanicAll, n (%)Study Completers, n (%)Lost-to-Follow-Up, n (%)p-Value
286 (48%)220 (45%)66 (63%)<0.01
Race
  American Indian or Alaskan Native15 (3%)12 (2%)3 (3%)0.11
  Asian12 (2%)10 (2%)2 (2%)
  Black or African American15 (3%)14 (3%)1 (1%)
  Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander2 (0%)1 (0%)1 (1%)
  White423 (71%)357 (72%)66 (62%)
  Other132 (22%)99 (20%)33 (31%)
Gender, female473 (79%)390 (79%)83 (78%)0.88
Education
  <HS46 (8%)28 (6%)18 (17%)<0.01
  HS or GED94 (16%)66 (13%)28 (26%)
  Some college142 (24%)121 (24%)21 (20%)
  Associates degree or higher318 (53%)279 (56%)39 (37%)
Income
  <USD 10,00075 (13%)58 (12%)17 (17%)<0.01
  USD 10,000–29,000175 (31%)135 (29%)40 (40%)
  USD 30,000–49,00097 (17%)82 (18%)15 (15%)
  USD 50,000–69,00069 (12%)58 (12%)11 (11%)
  USD 70,000–89,00049 (9%)44 (9%)5 (5%)
  ≥USD 90,000102 (18%)90 (19%)12 (12%)
Personal Cancer History, Yes95 (16%)75 (15%)20 (19%)0.36
Family History of Skin Cancer, Yes202 (35%)181 (37%)21 (21%)<0.01
  Don’t Know (abs. cont.)62 (10%)43 (9%)19 (18%)<0.01
  Absolute dichotomous, likely191 (49%)169 (52%)22 (35%)0.02
  Don’t Know (abs. dich.)207 (35%)165 (33%)42 (40%)0.20
Test Offer499 (83%)406 (82%)93 (88%)0.17
Test Completion, acceptors 1204 (41%)194 (48%)10 (11%)<0.001
Risk Feedback, higher risk 173 (60%)69 (60%)4 (67%)0.74
Skin Cancer Screening, Ever223 (37%)186 (38%)37 (35%)0.57
Note: p-values are based on Chi-square tests. Missing responses are as follows: race, n = 1; income, n = 33; ethnicity, n = 2; personal cancer history, n = 4; family skin cancer history, n = 16. 1 Test acceptance is only assessed for those offered testing (n = 499) and risk results are only assessed for those that accepted testing and received results before 3-month follow-up assessment (n = 114). Note: HS: High School; abs. cont.: absolute continuous; abs. dich.: absolute dichotomous.
Table 2. Patient continuous baseline characteristics by follow-up status (N = 600).
Table 2. Patient continuous baseline characteristics by follow-up status (N = 600).
Mean (SD)All, n (%)Study Completers, n (%)Lost-to-Follow-Up, n (%)p-Value
Age53.84 (14.3)53.22 (14.0)56.71 (15.3)0.70
Burnability0.48 (0.6)0.47 (0.6)0.53 (0.6)0.43
Tannability0.83 (0.6)0.81 (0.5)0.89 (0.6)0.50
Lifetime Number of Sunburns1.21 (1.2)1.25 (1.2)1.03 (1.3)0.10
Health Literacy10.59 (2.1)10.73 (1.9)9.92 (2.8)0.19
Importance of Genetic Testing,5.96 (1.5)5.97 (1.5)5.91 (1.7)0.69
Perceived Risk
  Absolute continuous3.95 (1.4)4.00 (1.4)3.71 (1.7)0.31
  Comparative cont.2.87 (1.0)2.90 (0.9)2.72 (1.1)0.79
Sun Protection17.07 (3.8)17.15 (3.8)16.69 (4.0)0.26
Skin Cancer Worry2.54 (1.1)2.53 (1.0)2.58 (1.2)0.68
Note: p-values are based on independent samples t-tests. Missing responses are as follows: lifetime sunburns, n = 8; importance of testing, n = 2.
Table 3. Model estimates (p-value) for test offer, completion, and receipt of risk feedback result, separately, on each study primary outcome (sun protection, skin cancer screening, and skin cancer worry) at 3 month follow-up.
Table 3. Model estimates (p-value) for test offer, completion, and receipt of risk feedback result, separately, on each study primary outcome (sun protection, skin cancer screening, and skin cancer worry) at 3 month follow-up.
GroupModel
n
Sun Protection
b (p)
Skin Cancer Screening
OR (p)
Skin Cancer Worry
b (p)
Test Offer (n = 406) vs. Usual Care (n = 87) 493−0.12 (0.680)1.24 (0.546)−0.01 (0.873)
Baseline 0.68 (<0.001)2.24 (0.002)0.08 (0.001)
Complete (n = 194) vs. Decline Testing (n = 211)4050.36 (0.148)0.97 (0.903)−0.02 (0.765)
Baseline 0.68 (<0.001)2.06 (0.013)0.07 (0.008)
Higher (n = 69) vs. Avg (n = 45) risk feedback1140.35 (0.431)0.82 (0.710)−0.01 (0.921)
Baseline 0.71 (<0.001)1.88 (0.230)0.10 (0.045)
Note: Models regress the 3 month primary outcome (e.g., sun protection score) on the effect of interest (e.g., test offer) and control for the baseline score only. This table presents the results from 9 different generalized linear models, with an identity link used for sun protection score, logistic for skin exam, and Poisson for worry. Unstandardized regression coefficients (b) and odds ratios (OR) are presented.
Table 4. Effect sizes for moderator effects of risk results (higher vs. average) on sun protection, skin exam, and worry.
Table 4. Effect sizes for moderator effects of risk results (higher vs. average) on sun protection, skin exam, and worry.
Moderator VariableSun Protection,
η2 (p-Value)
Skin Exam,
OR (p-Value)
Worry,
η2 (p-Value)
Ethnicity0.00 (0.67)1.86 (0.62)0.00 (0.97)
Race: White0.00 (0.60)1.03 (>0.99)0.00 (0.96)
Gender0.00 (0.50)2.36 (0.55)0.00 (0.94)
Education0.00 (0.70)8.32 (0.11)0.01 (0.51)
Age0.00 (0.80)1.76 (0.39)0.01 (0.68)
Income0.00 (0.64)2.29 (0.44)0.00 (0.86)
Personal Cancer History0.01 (0.19)4.92 (0.18)0.01 (0.50)
Family History of Skin Cancer0.00 (0.62)16.65 (0.05)0.00 (0.79)
Burnability0.01 (0.16)1.34 (0.58)0.01 (0.56)
Tannability0.03 (0.01)1.32 (0.59)0.01 (0.65)
Lifetime number of sunburns0.00 (0.93)36.97 (0.01)0.02 (0.41)
Health Literacy0.00 (0.49)1.46 (0.62)0.00 (0.94)
Importance of Genetic Testing0.00 (0.61)2.97 (0.16)0.00 (0.71)
Perceived Risk
  Absolute continuous0.01 (0.09)2.45 (0.13)0.00 (0.89)
  DK (abs. cont.): DK response0.01 (0.09)>99 (NaN)0.00 (>0.99)
  Absolute dichotomous: Likely0.02 (0.04)1.83 (0.63)0.05 (0.20)
  DK (abs. dich.): DK response0.00 (0.96)1.06 (0.96)0.01 (0.48)
  Comparative cont.0.01 (0.11)2.09 (0.20)0.00 (0.99)
Note: Standardized effect sizes (η2 for continuous outcomes and odds ratio for the binary outcomes) and p-values are calculated for the interaction effect between the potential moderator and risk level (higher vs. average) on the given outcome. Each 3 month outcome is regressed on the baseline measure, risk level, the potential moderator, and the interaction term. Odds ratios (ORs) for continuous variables are standardized, representing odds ratios corresponding to an increase by one standard deviation of the distribution. Maximum n for each model is N = 114. Note: DK: don’t know; abs. cont.: absolute continuous; abs. dich.: absolute dichotomous
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Hay, J.L.; Kaphingst, K.A.; Buller, D.; Schofield, E.; Meyer White, K.; Sussman, A.; Guest, D.; Dailey, Y.T.; Robers, E.; Schwartz, M.R.; et al. Behavioral and Psychological Outcomes Associated with Skin Cancer Genetic Testing in Albuquerque Primary Care. Cancers 2021, 13, 4053. https://doi.org/10.3390/cancers13164053

AMA Style

Hay JL, Kaphingst KA, Buller D, Schofield E, Meyer White K, Sussman A, Guest D, Dailey YT, Robers E, Schwartz MR, et al. Behavioral and Psychological Outcomes Associated with Skin Cancer Genetic Testing in Albuquerque Primary Care. Cancers. 2021; 13(16):4053. https://doi.org/10.3390/cancers13164053

Chicago/Turabian Style

Hay, Jennifer L., Kimberly A. Kaphingst, David Buller, Elizabeth Schofield, Kirsten Meyer White, Andrew Sussman, Dolores Guest, Yvonne T. Dailey, Erika Robers, Matthew R. Schwartz, and et al. 2021. "Behavioral and Psychological Outcomes Associated with Skin Cancer Genetic Testing in Albuquerque Primary Care" Cancers 13, no. 16: 4053. https://doi.org/10.3390/cancers13164053

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop