Next Article in Journal
Leucine Supplementation Differently Modulates Branched-Chain Amino Acid Catabolism, Mitochondrial Function and Metabolic Profiles at the Different Stage of Insulin Resistance in Rats on High-Fat Diet
Next Article in Special Issue
Nutritional Challenges in Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy
Previous Article in Journal
Feeding a Mixture of Choline Forms to Lactating Dams Improves the Development of the Immune System in Sprague-Dawley Rat Offspring
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Evolving Interplay among Abundant Adipokines in Patients with Hepatitis C during Viral Clearance
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Toxoplasma Gondii Moderates the Association between Multiple Folate-Cycle Factors and Cognitive Function in U.S. Adults

1
Department of Psychology, Brigham Young University, Provo, UT 84602, USA
2
The Neuroscience Center, Brigham Young University, Provo, UT 84602, USA
3
Department of Sociology, Brigham Young University, PProvo, UT 84602, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2017, 9(6), 564; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu9060564
Submission received: 27 March 2017 / Revised: 25 May 2017 / Accepted: 29 May 2017 / Published: 2 June 2017
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Nutrients, Infectious and Inflammatory Diseases)

Abstract

:
Toxoplasma gondii (T. gondii) is a microscopic, apicomplexan parasite that can infect muscle or neural tissue, including the brain, in humans. While T. gondii infection has been associated with changes in mood, behavior, and cognition, the mechanism remains unclear. Recent evidence suggests that T. gondii may harvest folate from host neural cells. Reduced folate availability is associated with an increased risk of neurodevelopmental disorders, neurodegenerative diseases, and cognitive decline. We hypothesized that impairment in cognitive functioning in subjects seropositive for T. gondii might be associated with a reduction of folate availability in neural cells. We analyzed data from the third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey to determine the associations between T. gondii infection, multiple folate-cycle factors, and three tests of cognitive functioning in U.S. adults aged 20 to 59 years. In these analyses, T. gondii moderated the associations of folate, vitamin B-12, and homocysteine with performance on the Serial Digit Learning task, a measure of learning and memory, as well as the association of folate with reaction time. The results of this study suggest that T. gondii might affect brain levels of folate and/or vitamin B-12 enough to affect cognitive functioning.

1. Introduction

Toxoplasma gondii (T. gondii) is an apicomplexan parasite that infects approximately 12% of the U.S. population as of 2010 [1]. Although the parasite’s definitive host is any member of the cat family, humans can become infected with T. gondii as an intermediate host via ingestion of contaminated foods or by exposure to cat feces [2]. Upon successful invasion, T. gondii infects muscle and neural tissue and encapsulates itself in a cyst that protects it against the host immune system [3]. T. gondii biology has unique features that set it apart from other intracellular parasites. For example, using precursors from the infected cell, T. gondii can synthesize dopamine [4,5], although the physiological effects of this dopamine production remain unknown.
Beyond dopamine production, T. gondii may influence other aspects of host biology. In regards to folate synthesis, T. gondii may salvage or harvest folate directly from the infected host cell [6]. It is unknown, however, whether this might affect host health. Further, it is unclear whether T. gondii depends on the folate acquired from the host or if the system is only supplementary.
In humans, folate is essential for DNA synthesis and repair and is a key factor in cell division and growth [7]. The brain requires a constant supply of dietary folate for early embryonic neurodevelopment, adult neurogenesis, and the production of neurotransmitters [8,9]. Therefore, reduced availability of folate is associated with neurodevelopmental disorders [10] and may be associated with reduced cognitive function [11,12,13,14].
In individuals who do not obtain sufficient dietary folate or who cannot metabolize folate efficiently, neural cells might be generally and persistently deprived of this essential nutrient [15,16]. If T. gondii is indeed capable of harvesting folate from host neural cells, then infected cells may be further starved of this important nutrient and thereby magnify potential impairments in cognitive functioning. Based on these factors, we hypothesized that cognitive function may be associated with an interaction between T. gondii infection and concentrations of folate and other factors related to folate metabolism such as vitamin B-12 and homocysteine. To test this, we used the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey III (NHANES III) dataset from the United States’ Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC).

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Sample

The NHANES III is a nationally representative survey conducted by the CDC from 1988 to 1994. It includes demographic, examination, laboratory, and dietary information for approximately 34,000 participants recruited from multiple locations around the U.S. Using stratified sampling methods and statistical weighting, the NHANES III was designed to represent the non-institutionalized U.S. population. In the NHANES III, a subsample of subjects aged 20 to 59 years completed tests of cognitive functioning thereby limiting the potential study sample to 4924. Data for serum concentrations of vitamin B-12 and homocysteine were only collected in subjects who were assessed during the second phase (1991–1994) of the NHANES survey, further limiting the potential study sample to 2242 subjects. Finally, we did not include in our analyses subjects who were missing data for any of the remaining variables we used in this study (i.e., serum folate concentration, T. gondii infection status, and all controlling variables), resulting in a final sample size of 2037 (Figure 1).

2.2. Toxoplasma Gondii

Detection of T. gondii-specific antibodies was accomplished by CDC laboratory technicians via indirect enzyme immunoassay. A full description of the laboratory procedures used is available in the NHANES III laboratory manual [17]. Briefly, test samples were combined with T. gondii antigens, peroxidase-labelled human gamma chain immunoglobulin (IgG), peroxidase substrate, and chromogen ortho-phenylene diamine resulting in a color change proportional to the total T. gondii antibody concentration in the sample. The color change is read as an optical density via spectrophotometer and compared to a standard curve calibrated to World Health Organization Toxo 60 serum. Possible IgG titer values ranged from 0 IU/mL to 240 IU/mL (higher concentrations were collapsed to 240 IU/mL to reduce the risk of deductive disclosure). Subjects with titer values above 7 IU/mL were considered seropositive for T. gondii exposure, whereas subjects below this level were considered negative [17].
Anti-T. gondii IgG antibodies indicate past exposure to T. gondii. Further, IgG titer levels can vary across the lifespan potentially due to multiple exposures or natural anamnestic decreases in IgG levels [18,19,20]. However, the procedures and classification system used in the NHANES III remain the industry norm (excepting minor differences in the IgG titer cut-off point for seropositivity depending on the specific assay/manufacturer) in identifying individuals exposed to and likely infected with T. gondii.

2.3. B-Vitamin Biomarkers

Serum concentrations of folate and vitamin B-12 were determined by CDC laboratory technicians using a Quantaphase Folate radioassay kit from Bio-Rad Laboratories. The Quantaphase Folate radioassay kit contains a detailed description of the methods used to prepare the folate and B-12 samples [21]. Following sample preparation, concentration is calculated from a standard curve. Serum homocysteine concentration was determined via reverse-phase, high-performance liquid chromatography and fluorescence detection as described in the NHANES III laboratory manual [17]. Importantly, data for the NHANES III was collected before the introduction of mandatory folic acid food fortification [22]. Therefore, reported average serum concentrations of folate and/or vitamin B-12 for subjects surveyed in the NHANES III may be lower than more recent averages.

2.4. Cognitive Functioning

In the NHANES III, computerized versions of the Symbol Digit Substitution (SDS), Serial Digit Learning (SDL), and Reaction Time (RT) tests assessed general cognitive function. In the SDS, subjects were required to match a series of symbols with their respective digits using a key given at the top of the computer screen for four trials. Each trial consisted of nine digit-symbol pairings, and the total latency (in seconds) for each of the nine possible symbol-digit pairings was recorded for each trial. Finally, the average of the two lowest total latencies from the four trials was used as the final score. Therefore, a lower score, or faster pairing, on the SDS indicates better performance. The SDL required subjects to memorize and recall a series of eight random digits over eight trials. Scores on the SDL reflected the number of errors made in each trial. Therefore, a lower score, or fewer errors, indicates better performance on the SDL. Subjects who successfully repeated the eight-digit sequence for two consecutive trials were not required to complete the remaining trials. Finally, RT was measured by pressing a button as quickly as possible whenever a square appeared in the center of the computer screen. The average RT (in milliseconds (ms)) over 50 trials was used as the final score with faster times indicating better RT performance. Krieg et al. [23] provides a detailed description of the three cognitive tests used in the NHANES III.

2.5. Covariates

We included a number of covariates in all analyses to control for potential confounding. Continuous covariates included age in years, poverty-to-income ratio (PIR), and years of education. PIR is a measure of an individual’s socioeconomic status computed by dividing the household income by the federal poverty level (at the time of the survey) resulting in a minimum value of zero and a maximum of five (higher values were collapsed to five to reduce the risk of deductive disclosure). Categorical variables included sex and race-ethnicity. For the race-ethnicity variable, the NHANES III survey asked all participants to self-report one of the following race-ethnicity categories: non-Hispanic white, non-Hispanic black, Mexican American, or “other”.

2.6. Statistical Analysis

We used Stata 14.2 [24] for all statistical analyses. We computed summary statistics for all variables included in our analyses and computed means and standard errors for continuous variables and proportions and standard errors for categorical variables. Ordinary least squares regression was used to test for differences between T. gondii seropositive and seronegative subjects on each of the variables included in this study. Only the p-values for these comparisons are reported in Table 1. We used ordinary least squares regression to test for interaction effects between T. gondii seropositivity and folate, vitamin B-12, or homocysteine concentrations in the prediction of SDS, SDL, or RT scores for a total of nine separate models. We included age, PIR, education, sex, and race-ethnicity as covariates in each model.
For each analysis, we report main effects for T. gondii, a folate-cycle factor, and their interaction. The coefficient for T. gondii is the score on the respective cognitive test for subjects seropositive for T. gondii infection. The coefficient for the folate-cycle factors represents the relationship between each factor and scores on each of the three cognitive tests for subjects who were seronegative for T. gondii infection. The interaction of folate-cycle factors and T. gondii is the change in the slope of the folate-cycle factor for those subjects seropositive for T. gondii relative to those seronegative for T. gondii.

3. Results

Of the 2037 subjects included in this study, approximately 20% were seropositive for T. gondii, 50% were female, and approximately 75% were non-Hispanic white. Table 1 presents these and other demographic and study-sample characteristics.
We found no significant interactions between T. gondii seropositivity and any of the folate-cycle factors in predicting performance on the SDS. In predicting performance on the SDL, T. gondii seropositivity interacted with folate (β = −1.02, (95% CI: −1.88, –0.16), p = 0.021; Table 2), vitamin B-12 (β = −1.60, (95% CI: −2.98, −0.23), p = 0.023; Table 3), and homocysteine concentrations (β = 2.40, (95% CI: 0.94, 3.85), p = 0.002; Table 4) (Figure 2). In subjects seronegative for T. gondii, SDL performance was relatively constant as folate, vitamin B-12 or homocysteine levels varied. However, in subjects seropositive for T. gondii, performance on the SDL worsened as folate and vitamin B-12 levels decreased and as homocysteine levels increased (Figure 2). An interaction between T. gondii seropositivity and folate concentration predicted RT (β = 14.88, (95% CI: 5.99, 23.76), p = 0.001; Table 2). In contrast to the SDL, subjects seropositive for T. gondii performed better on the RT assessment as folate concentration decreased.

4. Discussion and Conclusions

Using a large, nationally representative sample of U.S. adults, this study provides evidence of an interaction between T. gondii seropositivity and concentrations of several folate-cycle factors in the prediction of general cognitive functioning. In this sample of over 2000 participants, approximately 20% were seropositive for IgG antibodies specific for T. gondii. While this prevalence of seropositivity is higher than more recent estimates [1], it is within the expected range for the years in which the NHANES III was conducted (1988–1994) [25,26].
In this study, interactions between T. gondii seropositivity and folate, vitamin B-12, and homocysteine concentrations particularly affected performance on the SDL, an assessment of ability in learning and recall [23] that requires attention and intact short-term memory to perform well on the task. Concentrations of folate-cycle factors (including folate, vitamin B-12, and homocysteine) have been found to be associated with memory loss and other cognitive impairments in neurological disorders such as dementia [12,13,27], although these studies did not take into account T. gondii seropositivity. Further, deficiency in dietary folate has been linked to reduced genesis of neuroprogenitor cells of the adult hippocampus [8], a brain region that influences both short-term memory and attention [28,29]. Low concentrations of folate and/or vitamin B-12 usually result in an elevation of homocysteine, which can damage neural cells and impair cognitive functioning. Indeed, elevated homocysteine also might increase risk for neurodegenerative disorders in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex [12,30]. Therefore, reductions in cellular folate concentrations, such as in the case of folate harvesting by T. gondii, could limit the bioavailability of this important nutrient and cause elevations in homocysteine in multiple brain regions including the prefrontal cortex or hippocampus [31,32] and thus disrupt learning, attention, or other related cognitive functions. Further, this effect may be magnified in individuals who do not regularly obtain sufficient amounts of dietary folate or who cannot metabolize folate efficiently.
Another potential mechanism by which reduced folate availability might impact cognitive functioning lies in the indirect role of folate metabolism in the generation of serotonin and dopamine. Purines synthesized during folate metabolism contribute to the biosynthesis of guanosine triphosphate (GTP), a precursor of tetrahydrobiopterin (BH4). Tyrosine hydroxylase and tryptophan hydroxylase each utilize BH4 as a co-factor in the production of dopamine and serotonin, respectively. Therefore, reduced availability of dietary folate may indirectly lead to an overall decrease of dopamine and serotonin production [9,33]. In the case of T. gondii seropositivity, the combined effects of low concentrations of folate and simultaneous folate harvesting from T. gondii might particularly affect infected neural cells. Since dopamine and serotonin both influence various cognitive functions such as memory, attention, and executive function [34,35,36], this link could potentially offer an alternate explanation of the effects observed in this study.
Beyond performance on the SDL, there was a significant interaction between T. gondii and folate concentration in the prediction of RT. However, the direction of the observed relationship suggested that T. gondii seropositivity might actually be favorable in regards to RT in cases of low folate concentration and detrimental in cases of high folate concentration. Though not statistically significant, we found a similar relationship in a previous study [37] exploring interactions between the bacterium Helicobacter pylori and folate concentration in the prediction of RT. Similar to T. gondii, Helicobacter pylori might also reduce folate availability [38], though by a different mechanism. Despite these differing mechanisms of folate reduction, in both cases, RT improved, suggesting that folate deficiency associated with T. gondii might enable some advantage in RT. We are unaware of a mechanism that might explain how reductions in folate availability by infectious diseases such as T. gondii or Helicobacter pylori could be favorable towards RT performance. The data available in the NHANES III do not allow for an in-depth exploration of this association. Future research into this finding would likely require animal models or neuroimaging combined with biochemical testing to determine how regions of the brain active during RT tasks might be affected by reductions in folate availability.
Some limitations require consideration when interpreting the results of this study. The NHANES data sets are cross-sectional in design and thus preclude the ability to make casual inferences. Further, as the degree to which T. gondii harvests folate from infected host cells is not clear, it is currently unknown whether T. gondii harvests enough to affect cognitive functioning. With these data, it is also impossible to determine the time of initial infection by T. gondii or the frequency with which individuals have encountered the parasite in their lifetime. It is possible that the effects of T. gondii on folate availability and on cognition could vary based on the length of infection with the parasite or based on the number of exposures to the parasite. Finally, folate and vitamin B-12 availability can vary for a multitude of reasons including genetic mutations in key folate-cycle enzymes [7,39,40]. Although the NHANES III does include genetic data, access to the data is restricted and requires funding to obtain it. Therefore, additional research is needed to identify other factors that might modify the effects observed in this study.
This study is strengthened by using multiple controls that limit the number of potential confounds that might explain the observed effects. Specifically, by controlling for demographic and other health factors, we were more confident that the interactions between T. gondii and concentrations of folate, vitamin B-12, or homocysteine were associated with the variation in SDL or RT scores. Finally, use of the NHANES III data sets resulted in a large sample size that increases the generalizability of the findings and study power.
This study consisting of 2037 U.S. adults presents evidence of an interaction effect between T. gondii seropositivity and concentrations of multiple folate-cycle factors on cognitive functioning in adult humans. Additional research is recommended to explore the specific mechanisms involved in this association and to determine any additional potential consequences to folate and/or vitamin B-12 availability following T. gondii infection.

Author Contributions

Dawson W. Hedges and Andrew N. Berrett conceived and designed the study. Andrew N. Berrett collected the data and performed the analyses. Lance D. Erickson provided statistical and methodological support. Andrew N. Berrett authored the manuscript while Shawn D. Gale, Lance D. Erickson, Bruce L. Brown, and Dawson W. Hedges each contributed to and edited the manuscript throughout its preparation. Andrew N. Berrett designed and built the figures and tables with Lance D. Erickson providing support.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Jones, J.L.; Kruszon-Moran, D.; Rivera, H.N.; Price, C.; Wilkins, P.P. Toxoplasma gondii seroprevalence in the United States 2009–2010 and comparison with the past two decades. Am. J. Trop. Med. Hyg. 2014, 90, 1135–1139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Elmore, S.A.; Jones, J.L.; Conrad, P.A.; Patton, S.; Lindsay, D.S.; Dubey, J.P. Toxoplasma gondii: Epidemiology, feline clinical aspects, and prevention. Trends Parasitol. 2010, 26, 190–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Carruthers, V.B.; Suzuki, Y. Effects of Toxoplasma gondii infection on the brain. Schizophr. Bull. 2007, 33, 745–751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Martin, H.L.; Alsaady, I.; Howell, G.; Prandovszky, E.; Peers, C.; Robinson, P.; McConkey, G.A. Effect of parasitic infection on dopamine biosynthesis in dopaminergic cells. Neuroscience 2015, 306, 50–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Prandovszky, E.; Gaskell, E.; Martin, H.; Dubey, J.P.; Webster, J.P.; McConkey, G.A. The neurotropic parasite Toxoplasma gondii increases dopamine metabolism. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e23866. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Massimine, K.M.; Doan, L.T.; Atreya, C.A.; Stedman, T.T.; Anderson, K.S.; Joiner, K.A.; Coppens, I. Toxoplasma gondii is capable of exogenous folate transport. A likely expansion of the BT1 family of transmembrane proteins. Mol. Biochem. Parasitol. 2005, 144, 44–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Folate in Health and Disease; Marcel Dekker, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1995; ISBN 0824792807.
  8. Kruman, I.I.; Mouton, P.R.; Emokpae, R., Jr.; Cutler, R.G.; Mattson, M.P. Folate deficiency inhibits proliferation of adult hippocampal progenitors. Neuropharmacol. Neurotoxicol. 2005, 16, 1055–1059. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Temudo, T.; Rios, M.; Prior, C.; Carrilho, I.; Santos, M.; Maciel, P.; Sequeiros, J.; Fonseca, M.; Monteiro, J.; Cabral, P.; et al. Evaluation of CSF neurotransmitters and folate in 25 patients with Rett disorder and effects of treatment. Brain Dev. 2009, 31, 46–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Pu, D.; Shen, Y.; Wu, J. Association between MTHFR gene polymorphisms and the risk of autism spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis. Autism Res. 2013, 6, 384–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Hultberg, B.; Isaksson, A.; Nilsson, K.; Gustafson, L. Markers for the functional availability of cobalamin/folate and their association with neuropsychiatric symptoms in the elderly. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2001, 16, 873–878. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Mattson, M.P.; Shea, T.B. Folate and homocysteine metabolism in neural plasticity and neurodegenerative disorders. Trends Neurosci. 2003, 26, 137–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Michelakos, T.; Kousoulis, A.A.; Katsiardanis, K.; Dessypris, N.; Anastasiou, A.; Katsiardani, K.P.; Kanavidis, P.; Stefanadis, C.; Papadopoulos, F.C.; Petridou, E.T. Serum folate and B12 levels in association with cognitive impairment among seniors: Results from the Velestino study in Greece and meta-analysis. J. Aging Health 2013, 25, 589–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. McGarel, C.; Pentieva, K.; Strain, J.J.; McNulty, H. Emerging roles for folate and related B-vitamins in brain health across the lifecycle. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 2015, 74, 46–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Rai, V. Methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase (MTHFR) C677T polymorphism and Alzheimer disease risk: A meta-analysis. Mol. Neurobiol. 2016, 54, 1173–1186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Hua, Y.; Zhao, H.; Kong, Y.; Ye, M. Association between the MTHFR gene and Alzheimer’s disease: A meta-analysis. Int. J. Neurosci. 2011, 121, 462–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Gunter, E.W.; Lewis, B.G.; Koncikowski, S.M. Laboratory Procedures Used for the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES III), 1988–1994; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services: Atlanta, GA, USA, 1996.
  18. Wyman, C.P.; Gale, S.D.; Hedges-Muncy, A.; Erickson, L.D.; Wilson, E.; Hedges, D.W. Association between Toxoplasma gondii seropositivity and memory function in nondemented older adults. Neurobiol. Aging 2017, 53, 76–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Konishi, E. Annual change in immunoglobulin G and M antibody levels to Toxoplasma gondii in human sera. Microbiol. Immunol. 1989, 33, 403–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Havlíček, J.; Gašová, Z.; Smith, A.P.; Zvára, K.; Flegr, J. Decrease of psychomotor performance in subjects with latent ‘asymptomatic’ toxoplasmosis. Parasitology 2001, 122, 515–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Laboratory Procedure Manual. Available online: https://wwwn.cdc.gov/nchs/data/nhanes/2003-2004/labmethods/l06_c_met_folates-b12.pdf (accessed on 2 June 2017).
  22. Kessler, D.A.; Shalala, D.E. Food Standards: Amendment of Standards of Identity for Enriched Grain Products to Require Addition of Folic Acid; Department of Health and Human Services: Food and Drug Administration; Federal Register: Washington, DC, USA, 1996; Volume 61.
  23. Edward, F.; Krieg, J.; Chrislip, D.W.; Letz, R.E.; Otto, D.A.; Crespo, C.J.; Brightwell, W.S.; Ehrenberg, R.L. Neurobehavioral test performance in the third national health and nutrition examination survey. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 2001, 23, 569–589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software: Release 14, StataCorp LP: College Station, TX, USA, 2015.
  25. Jones, J.L.; McAuley, J.B.; Kruszon-Moran, D.; Wilson, M.; McQuillan, G.; Navin, T.; McAuley, J.B. Toxoplasma gondii infection in the United States: Seroprevalence and risk factors. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2001, 154, 357–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Pappas, G.; Roussos, N.; Falagas, M.E. Toxoplasmosis snapshots: Global status of Toxoplasma gondii seroprevalence and implications for pregnancy and congenital toxoplasmosis. Int. J. Parasitol. 2009, 39, 1385–1394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Wahlin, T.-B.R.; Wahlin, A.; Winblad, B.; Backman, L. The influence of serum vitamin B12 and folate status on cognitive functioning in very old age. Biol. Psychol. 2001, 56, 247–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Goldfarb, E.V.; Chun, M.M.; Phelps, E.A. Memory-guided attention: Independent contributions of the hippocampus and striatum. Neuron 2016, 89, 317–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Hannula, D.E.; Ranganath, C. Medial temporal lobe activity predicts successful relational memory binding. J. Neurosci. 2008, 28, 116–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Kruman, I.I.; Kumaravel, T.S.; Lohani, A.; Pedersen, W.A.; Cutler, R.G.; Kruman, Y.; Haughey, N.; Lee, J.; Evans, M.; Mattson, M.P. Folic acid deficiency and homocysteine impair DNA repair in hippocampal neurons and sensitize them to amyloid toxicity in experimental models of Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurosci. 2002, 22, 1752–1762. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  31. Madsen, S.K.; Rajagopalan, P.; Joshi, S.H.; Toga, A.W.; Thompson, P.M.; the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI). Higher homocysteine associated with thinner cortical gray matter in 803 participants from the Alzheimer’s disease neuroimaging initiative. Neurobiol. Aging 2015, 36 (Suppl. 1), S203–S210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Rajagopalan, P.; Hua, X.; Toga, A.W.; Jack, C.R., Jr.; Weiner, M.W.; Thompson, P.M. Homocysteine effects on brain volumes mapped in 732 elderly individuals. Neuroreport 2011, 22, 391–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Ramaekers, V.T.; Thony, B.; Sequeira, J.M.; Ansseau, M.; Philippe, P.; Boemer, F.; Bours, V.; Quadros, E.V. Folinic acid treatment for schizophrenia associated with folate receptor autoantibodies. Mol. Genet. Metab. 2014, 113, 307–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Nakajima, S.; Gerretsen, P.; Takeuchi, H.; Caravaggio, F.; Chow, T.; Le Foll, B.; Mulsant, B.; Pollock, B.; Graff-Guerrero, A. The potential role of dopamine D3 receptor neurotransmission in cognition. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. J. Eur. Coll. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2013, 23, 799–813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Paul, R.T.; McDonnell, A.P.; Kelly, C.B. Folic acid: Neurochemistry, metabolism and relationship to depression. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 2004, 19, 477–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Terry, A.V., Jr.; Buccafusco, J.J.; Wilson, C. Cognitive dysfunction in neuropsychiatric disorders: Selected serotonin receptor subtypes as therapeutic targets. Behav. Brain Res. 2008, 195, 30–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Berrett, A.N.; Gale, S.D.; Erickson, L.D.; Brown, B.L.; Hedges, D.W. Folate and inflammatory markers moderate the association between Helicobacter pylori exposure and cognitive function in us adults. Helicobacter 2016, 21, 471–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Kountouras, J.; Gavalas, E.; Boziki, M.; Zavos, C. Helicobacter pylori may be involved in cognitive impairment and dementia development through induction of atrophic gastritis, vitamin B-12–folate deficiency, and hyperhomocysteinemia sequence. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2007, 86, 805–809. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  39. Cheng, D.M.; Jiang, Y.G.; Huang, C.Y.; Kong, H.Y.; Pang, W.; Yang, H.P. Polymorphism of MTHFR C677T, serum vitamin levels and cognition in subjects with hyperhomocysteinemia in China. Nutr. Neurosci. 2010, 13, 175–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Ho, V.; Massey, T.E.; King, W.D. Effects of methionine synthase and methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase gene polymorphisms on markers of one-carbon metabolism. Genes Nutr. 2013, 8, 571–580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Flow diagram of sampling selection and final number of subjects included in all analyses.
Figure 1. Flow diagram of sampling selection and final number of subjects included in all analyses.
Nutrients 09 00564 g001
Figure 2. Significant interaction between folate-cycle factors and Toxoplasma gondii predicting cognitive performance in U.S. adults. Notes: Higher values on each cognitive assessment indicate worse performance. Values for folate, vitamin B-12, and homocysteine were logarithmically transformed.
Figure 2. Significant interaction between folate-cycle factors and Toxoplasma gondii predicting cognitive performance in U.S. adults. Notes: Higher values on each cognitive assessment indicate worse performance. Values for folate, vitamin B-12, and homocysteine were logarithmically transformed.
Nutrients 09 00564 g002
Table 1. Weighted summary statistics of U.S. adults 20 to 59 year olds, National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey III 1988–1994.
Table 1. Weighted summary statistics of U.S. adults 20 to 59 year olds, National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey III 1988–1994.
T. Gondii Seropositive
N = 417
T. Gondii Seronegative
N = 1620
Mean or FrequencySEMean or FrequencySEp
Sociodemographic Factors
Age (years)41.541.0136.160.63<0.001
Poverty-to-income Ratio3.230.253.380.130.316
Education (years)12.360.2313.130.12<0.001
Female45.63%2.65%51.55%1.08%0.050
Race-ethnicity
Non-Hispanic White72.73%3.92%77.98%1.27%0.159
Non-Hispanic Black10.77%1.64%10.55%0.47%0.896
Hispanic5.87%0.81%5.50%0.40%0.691
Other10.62%3.25%5.97%1.14%0.097
Biochemistry
Folate (ng/mL)7.080.516.890.240.807
Vitamin B-12 (pg/mL)521.8344.25474.017.080.623
Homocysteine (umol/L)9.360.279.560.250.282
Cognitive Testing
Symbol Digit Substitution Score2.850.052.550.03<0.001
Serial Digit Learning Score5.450.313.910.15<0.001
Reaction Time (sec)240.13.03234.471.710.070
Abbreviations: SE = Standard Error.
Table 2. Two-way interaction effects of serum folate concentration and Toxoplasma gondii infection status predicting SDS, SDL, and RT cognitive assessment scores for U.S. 20 to 59 year olds.
Table 2. Two-way interaction effects of serum folate concentration and Toxoplasma gondii infection status predicting SDS, SDL, and RT cognitive assessment scores for U.S. 20 to 59 year olds.
Symbol Digit SubstitutionSerial Digit LearningReaction Time
β95% CIpβ95% CIpβ95% CIp
Folate0.01(−0.04, 0.05)0.7860.10(−0.33, 0.53)0.641−1.19(−5.55, 3.16)0.585
T. gondii0.15(−0.08, 0.38)0.1852.48(1.02, 3.94)0.001−20.60(−34.38, −6.83)0.004
T. gondii × Folate Interaction−0.07(−0.20, 0.07)0.344−1.13(−1.97, −0.29)0.00913.07(4.20, 21.93)0.005
Race-Ethnicity
Non-Hispanic White (ref)---------
Non-Hispanic Black0.30(0.21, 0.39)<0.0011.72(1.15, 2.29)<0.0015.92(0.14, 11.70)0.045
Mexican American0.27(0.18, 0.37)<0.0012.52(1.95, 3.09)<0.0011.23(−6.86, 9.32)0.761
Other0.29(0.10, 0.47)0.0031.88(0.69, 3.08)0.003−4.15(−14.36, 6.06)0.418
Female−0.17(−0.24, −0.10)<0.0010.08(−0.23, 0.38)0.6118.63(1.60, 15.66)0.017
Age (years)0.03(0.03, 0.03)<0.0010.09(0.07, 0.11)<0.0010.36(0.14, 0.58)0.002
PIR−0.03(−0.04, −0.02)<0.001−0.25(−0.35, −0.14)<0.001−3.13(−4.33, −1.92)<0.001
Education (years)−0.10(−0.11, −0.08)<0.001−0.46(−0.57, −0.35)<0.001−2.93(−4.08, −1.79)<0.001
Constant2.91(2.70, 3.12)<0.0016.77(4.96, 8.57)<0.001267.68(250.01, 285.34)<0.001
Notes: Age, sex, education, race-ethnicity, and poverty-to-income ratio included as controls in all models. Abbreviations: SDS, the Symbol Digit Substitution; SDL, Serial Digit Learning; RT, Reaction Time; CI = Confidence Interval; ref = Reference category; PIR, poverty-to-income ratio. N = 2037.
Table 3. Two-way interaction effects of serum vitamin B-12 concentration and Toxoplasma gondii infection status predicting SDS, SDL, and RT cognitive assessment scores for U.S. 20 to 59 year olds.
Table 3. Two-way interaction effects of serum vitamin B-12 concentration and Toxoplasma gondii infection status predicting SDS, SDL, and RT cognitive assessment scores for U.S. 20 to 59 year olds.
Symbol Digit SubstitutionSerial Digit LearningReaction Time
β95% CIpβ95% CIpβ95% CIp
Vitamin B-120.05(−0.00, 0.11)0.0570.23(−0.26, 0.72)0.346−3.04(−8.77, 2.68)0.291
T. gondii1.06(−0.12, 2.25)0.07610.24(2.14, 18.33)0.0145.26(−123.23, 133.74)0.935
T. gondii Vitamin B-12 Interaction−0.17(−0.36, 0.03)0.087−1.59(−2.92, −0.26)0.020−0.61(−21.40, 20.19)0.954
Race-Ethnicity
Non-Hispanic White (ref)---------
Non-Hispanic Black0.30(0.21, 0.39)<0.0011.78(1.19, 2.37)<0.0016.09(0.18, 12.01)0.044
Mexican American0.27(0.18, 0.37)<0.0012.53(1.96, 3.10)<0.0011.81(−6.22, 9.84)0.653
Other0.29(0.10, 0.47)0.0031.87(0.67, 3.08)0.003−4.59(−14.98, 5.81)0.380
Female−0.17(−0.24, −0.10)<0.0010.08(−0.21, 0.36)0.5838.52(1.64, 15.40)0.016
Age (years)0.03(0.03, 0.03)<0.0010.09(0.07, 0.11)<0.0010.40(0.17, 0.63)0.001
PIR−0.03(−004, −0.02)<0.001−0.24(−0.34, −0.14)<0.001−3.14(−4.32, −1.97)<0.001
Education (years)−0.10(−0.11, −0.08)<0.001−0.47(−0.58, −0.35)<0.001−2.83(−4.03, −1.64)<0.001
Constant2.61(2.22, 2.99)<0.0015.68(2.42, 8.93)0.001281.51(239.25, 323.77)<0.001
Notes: Age, sex, education, race-ethnicity, and poverty-to-income ratio included as controls in all models. Abbreviations: CI = Confidence Interval, ref = Reference category. N = 2037.
Table 4. Two-way interaction effects of serum homocysteine concentration and Toxoplasma gondii infection status predicting SDS, SDL, and RT cognitive assessment scores for U.S. 20 to 59 year olds.
Table 4. Two-way interaction effects of serum homocysteine concentration and Toxoplasma gondii infection status predicting SDS, SDL, and RT cognitive assessment scores for U.S. 20 to 59 year olds.
Symbol Digit SubstitutionSerial Digit LearningReaction Time
β95% CIpβ95% CIpβ95% CIp
Homocysteine−0.05(−0.13, 0.02)0.166−0.59(−1.22, 0.03)0.060−2.72(−9.05, 3.62)0.393
T. gondii−0.35(−1.17, 0.48)0.403−4.87(−7.88, −1.85)0.002−10.87(−45.43, 23.70)0.531
T. gondii × Homocysteine Interaction0.18(−0.20, 0.56)0.3512.49(1.09, 3.89)0.0015.67(−11.10, 22.43)0.501
Race-Ethnicity
Non-Hispanic White (ref)---------
Non-Hispanic Black0.31(0.22, 0.40)<0.0011.79(1.19, 2.38)<0.0015.50(−0.33, 11.34)0.064
Mexican American0.27(0.17, 0.37)<0.0012.52(1.96, 3.08)<0.0011.41(−6.81, 9.62)0.732
Other0.29(0.11, 0.48)0.0021.98(0.81, 3.15)0.001−3.92(−13.95, 6.11)0.436
Female−0.18(−0.25, −0.10)<0.0010.02(−0.31, 0.34)0.9268.13(1.12, 15.13)0.024
Age (years)0.03(0.03, 0.03)<0.0010.09(0.07, 0.11)<0.0010.42(0.18, 0.66)0.001
PIR-0.03(−0.05, −0.02)<0.001−0.25(−0.35, −0.15)<0.001−3.21(−4.45, −1.98)<0.001
Education (years)-0.10(−0.11, −0.08)<0.001−0.47(−0.59, −0.36)<0.001−2.83(−4.02, −1.64)<0.001
Constant3.04(2.78, 3.30)<0.0018.36(6.08, 10.65)<0.001268.43(243.08, 293.78)<0.001
Notes: Age, sex, education, race-ethnicity, and poverty-to-income ratio included as controls in all models. Abbreviations: CI = Confidence Interval, ref = Reference category. N = 2037.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Berrett, A.N.; Gale, S.D.; Erickson, L.D.; Brown, B.L.; Hedges, D.W. Toxoplasma Gondii Moderates the Association between Multiple Folate-Cycle Factors and Cognitive Function in U.S. Adults. Nutrients 2017, 9, 564. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu9060564

AMA Style

Berrett AN, Gale SD, Erickson LD, Brown BL, Hedges DW. Toxoplasma Gondii Moderates the Association between Multiple Folate-Cycle Factors and Cognitive Function in U.S. Adults. Nutrients. 2017; 9(6):564. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu9060564

Chicago/Turabian Style

Berrett, Andrew N., Shawn D. Gale, Lance D. Erickson, Bruce L. Brown, and Dawson W. Hedges. 2017. "Toxoplasma Gondii Moderates the Association between Multiple Folate-Cycle Factors and Cognitive Function in U.S. Adults" Nutrients 9, no. 6: 564. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu9060564

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop