Next Article in Journal
Possible Measures to Improve Both Participation and Response Quality in Japan’s National Health and Nutrition Survey: Results from a Workshop by Local Government Personnel in Charge of the Survey
Previous Article in Journal
Decreasing Vitamin C Intake, Low Serum Vitamin C Level and Risk for US Adults with Diabetes
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Increased Habit Frequency in the Daily Lives of Patients with Acute Anorexia Nervosa

1
Translational Developmental Neuroscience Section, Division of Psychological and Social Medicine and Developmental Neurosciences, Faculty of Medicine, Technische Universität Dresden, 01307 Dresden, Germany
2
Eating Disorder Treatment and Research Center, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Technische Universität Dresden, 01307 Dresden, Germany
3
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Carl Gustav Carus University Hospital Dresden, 01307 Dresden, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2022, 14(19), 3905; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14193905
Submission received: 8 August 2022 / Revised: 10 September 2022 / Accepted: 16 September 2022 / Published: 21 September 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Nutrition and Public Health)

Abstract

:
Strict eating routines and frequent rigid behavior patterns are commonly observed in patients with anorexia nervosa (AN). A recent theory proposes that while these behaviors may have been reinforced initially, they later become habitual. To date, however, research has been overly focused on eating-disorder (ED)-related habits. Over the course of seven days, we applied an ecological momentary assessment (EMA) to investigate the habit frequency and strength of ED-specific (food intake) and ED-unspecific (hygiene) habits in the daily lives of a sample of n = 57 AN and n = 57 healthy controls (HC). The results of the hierarchical models revealed that habits were significantly more likely in patients compared with HC for both categories, independently. Furthermore, a lower body mass index (BMI) was associated with increased habit frequency in AN. Our study strengthens the habit theory of AN by showing the relevance of habits beyond ED-specific behavioral domains. This also supports the development of innovative therapeutic interventions targeting habitual behavior in EDs.

1. Introduction

“For the past several months, my daughter has been cutting her food into identical little squares, arranging them in a circle on the plate and then eating them slowly in counterclockwise order during every meal”. Strict eating routines and frequent ritualized behavior patterns are commonly observed in individuals suffering from anorexia nervosa (AN). Therapeutic change of these routines and restrictive eating can be challenging [1,2]. Therefore, the importance of improving our understanding of how such behaviors are learned and become persistent and automatic (i.e., habitual) in AN has been emphasized [2,3].
Habits are defined as learned, repetitive, motor, or cognitive behavioral sequences elicited by contextual cues that, once initiated, can be carried out automatically, without conscious oversight [4,5]. Habit formation is thought to follow the principle of stimulus–response learning, according to which (with sufficient repetition) originally goal-directed actions gradually become less sensitive to outcomes [5,6] and, importantly, more closely linked to contextual cues. By shifting from goal-directed to stimulus–response reactions, habitual behavior eventually also becomes less cognitively demanding [7]. Building on this idea, Walsh [2] proposed that AN-related behavior may not be a product of ongoing goal-directed and consciously initiated action, but rather the result of a maladaptive habit formation, i.e., behavior that may have been rewarded initially (e.g., positive social feedback after weight loss), but has become independent from reinforcement over time. The fact that habitual behavior is outcome-independent makes it highly resistant to change, possibly reflecting the treatment resistance often seen in individuals with AN [8]. Therefore, these maladaptive behavior patterns may persist even after weight rehabilitation treatment [9,10,11].
As most research into habits in AN has focused on restrictive eating behavior, it remains unknown whether a tendency toward habitual behavior in AN is specific or domain-independent [12]. The latter would go in line with previous notions of an increased tendency toward rigid routines and strict behavior patterns in response to food restriction [13,14] and in individuals suffering from AN, as well as high rates of compulsive symptoms [15,16,17]. Of note, imbalances between goal-directed and habitual systems have also been implicated in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) [18,19,20], which is also characterized by maladaptive, repetitive, habit-like behaviors and is genetically correlated with AN [21]. Taken together, these previous observations may suggest that individuals with AN generally show more habits in many behavioral domains, unrelated to eating and body weight, possibly due to accelerated habit formation. However, this theory has yet to be confirmed [12,22].
Although the habit theory in AN has enjoyed popularity in recent literature [1,3,12,23,24], only a few studies have tried to investigate more detailed characteristics of these habitual behavior sequences in AN patients [1,25], and experimental evidence is sparse [12]. Supporting the relevance of habits in AN, two studies in adults have found that habit strength (i.e., the degree of behavioral automaticity) of specific food restriction-related behaviors was related to eating-disorder (ED) severity (ED symptom scores), as well as illness duration [1,25]. Interestingly, biological markers that are altered in AN, such as leptin [26], show associations with rigid thinking patterns, such as rumination about food and weight [27]. However, none of these studies have assessed ED-unspecific behaviors, and data were collected only at a single point in time.
Measurement problems have often hindered documenting habitual behavior in real life, as habits have traditionally been assessed as behavioral frequency, which might be difficult to quantify accurately using retrospective methods. Furthermore, studying habits in the laboratory is challenging and several tasks have failed to reliably induce habit-like behaviors [28]. Applying ecological momentary assessment (EMA [29] a method of gathering data from participants in their natural environment in real-time, through a smartphone, using multiple questionnaires a day), might be an advantageous approach to assess and understand habits in AN, because it circumvents many limitations and biases of an artificial laboratory setup, such as inaccurate memory of the exact temporal order of behavioral sequences.
Here, we applied EMA to investigate habit frequency and strength of both ED-specific (food) and ED-unspecific (hygiene) habit categories in a sample of acutely underweight female patients with AN and healthy controls (HC) over the course of seven days. We expected habit frequency and strength for ED-specific and ED-unspecific behavior to be elevated in patients compared with the controls. Furthermore, it was expected that the severity of ED-pathology would be correlated with habit frequency and strength.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Data were collected from 154 individuals—65 AN patients and a total of 89 HC. For the current analysis, only participants with an EMA compliance rate >40% (i.e., more than 40% of situational data was available) were included, which resulted in the exclusion of 6 AN patients and 16 HC. To account for potential developmental effects and to optimize the comparisons between AN and HC, we implemented a pairwise age-matching algorithm [30] to minimize age differences between individual pairs. The final sample consisted of 57 female patients with acute AN according to Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5) [31] (12.2–24.1 years old) and 57 female HC (12.9–24.3 years old) from Germany. Despite the age-matching procedure, groups showed a small, but significant, age difference (see Table 1) with a mean age difference of 1.6 years between matched pairs. AN patients were admitted to ED programs of a German university child and adolescent psychiatry and psychosomatic medicine department, including a behaviorally oriented nutritional rehabilitation program. Current AN was diagnosed using a modified version of the German expert form of the Structured Interview for Anorexia and Bulimia Nervosa (SIAB-EX [32]) and required a body mass index (BMI) below 17.5 kg/m2 (or below the 10th age percentile, if younger than 15.5 years). Information about comorbid diagnoses in AN patients was obtained from the SIAB-EX and medical records, and was confirmed by an expert clinician. To be included in the HC group, participants had to be of normal weight and eumenorrheic. Normal weight was defined as a BMI above 18.5 kg/m2 (if older than 16 years)/above the 10th age percentile (if younger than 16 years) and a BMI below 30 kg/m2 (if older than 16 years)/below the 97th age percentile (if younger than 16 years). History of any ED or psychiatric disorder was an exclusion criterion. HC were recruited through advertisement among middle school, high school, and university students. Inclusion and exclusion criteria, as well as possible confounding variables, were obtained using semi-structured research interviews—the SIAB-EX [32] and the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I. [33]). For further details regarding the diagnostic procedures, see Supplementary Materials File S1.
An a priori power analysis using G*power (Version 3.1., Heinreich Heine Universität Düsseldorf, Germany) [34] on the basis of previously published group differences in disorder-related habit strength between acute AN and HC [25] gauged a sample size of n = 17 per group, assuming an effect size that is comparable to the group differences observed in Davis et al. [25] (Cronbach’s alpha = 1.2) and an alpha-error probability of 5% (and a power of 95%).
Study data were collected and managed using secure, web-based electronic data capture tools REDcap (Version 12.0.28, Research Electronic Data Capture, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA [35]). This study was approved by the local institutional review board (Institutional Review Board of TU Dresden, Germany, EK536122015), and all participants (if underage, their guardians) gave written informed consent.

2.2. Clinical Measures

To complement the information obtained with the clinical interviews, we assessed ED-specific psychopathology using the Eating Disorders Inventory (EDI-2 [36]), depressive symptoms using the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II [37]), trait anxiety symptoms using the State and Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI/STAI-K (for children) [38]), and obsessive-compulsive traits using a validated German self-report questionnaire to assess obsessive-compulsive traits “Zwangsinventar für Kinder und Jugendliche” (ZWIK [39,40]); for details, see Supplementary Materials File S2.1. The BMI and BMI-standard deviation score (BMI-SDS [41,42]) were measured at baseline one day before starting the EMA assessment. In a subset of the current study sample (n = 46 AN and n = 56 HC), venous blood for leptin analysis was collected after an overnight fast (see Supplementary Materials File S2.2).

2.3. EMA Measures and Materials

Participants were provided with a study smartphone. The app-based questionnaire was designed via an online platform (MovisensXS, Karlsruhe, Germany, library version 4574, www.movisens.com (accessed on 7 August 2022)), which also managed data collection and immediate server upload. When participants received the study smartphone, they were instructed on how to respond to prompts and were briefed on what types of behaviors constituted habits. The EMA asked participants at each prompt whether they had carried out a habit in the last 60 min, and if so, in which of several categories (e.g., food intake, hygiene, or public transport; see Supplementary Materials File S2.1) the activity could be classified. Given that AN patients in the current study were predominantly inpatients, which restricts daily routine, e.g., related to activities in traffic or meal preparation, the focus of the current study was on habits that occurred in one ED-specific (food intake) and in one ED-unspecific (hygiene) category only. Both categories were assumed to be compatible with daily life while participating in a behaviorally-oriented intensive treatment program and were comparable to the data assessed during the daily life of the HC group. After assigning a behavioral sequence to a respective category, participants were asked to write down the habitual behavior in more detail, e.g., for a hygiene habit: “When I brush my teeth I always take the toothbrush with my left hand, apply the tooth paste with my right hand and start brushing my teeth from the left top side, to the left down side, to the front, to the right top side and the right bottom side”. Lastly, participants completed the 10-item Self-Report Habit Index (SRHI [43]) for each reported habit once, so as to assess habit strength, as used in other studies of habitual behaviors in other conditions including EDs [18,25,44]. If the same habit occurred in a previous prompt, the SRHI was not completed again. At each prompt, participants could enter up to five different habits that had occurred since the last prompt.

2.4. Procedure

Participants were initially screened; weighed; interviewed, and, as in our previous studies [45,46], received detailed instructions on how to handle the smartphone, the app, and the content of the questionnaires. For the latter, each participant completed a tutorial, during which they received five example descriptions of habitual behavior sequences of different categories and were asked to fill out the SRHI. They were instructed to answer the questionnaires as soon as the alarm appeared, but were given an additional 30 min after the prompt if they were unable to reply (e.g., during class or work, during mealtimes, or therapy sessions). As in our previous EMA studies [45,46], sampling started the day after screening and lasted for a period of seven days via the signal-contingent assessment method: Alarms occurred at eight semi-random times during a 14 h period that was adapted for each individual to suit different daily routines. Compensation was provided at the end of the study, in accordance with compliance rates.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

Because our EMA research design produces nested data [47] in which prompts nest within participants, we conducted two hierarchical generalized linear models (HGLM) via a population-average Bernoulli model for binomial outcome variables (logistic regression for multilevel data) using the software HLM version 8 (Scientific Software International, Inc., Chapel Hill, NC, USA) [48]. These models were chosen as they can account for the correlations within single levels, as well as the interaction between them. The population-average model was favored over the unit-specific model, as we examined the average differences in habit likelihood in each of the two sub-populations [49]. We specified the occurrence of a habit (1 for either food intake (model 1), or hygiene (model 2)) or no habit (0) as a primary outcome measure of interest. The main predictor at the person level was group (1 AN, −1 HC). For both models, age of participant and compliance rate were also entered as control variables, as the groups significantly differed in these variables, which might have influenced reports of habit frequency. Additionally, the variable “trigger”, representing the study progress, was entered at the situation level to account for possible effects of study duration or reactivity to EMA. As habitual behavior might be closely related to compulsions or be different between AN subtypes (restricting or binge-eating/purging), we further validated the main models by (a) excluding two patients with co-morbid OCD and (b) excluding participants with the binge-eating/purging subtype of AN for the models in the supplementary analyses.
Next, to validate the HGLM approach, a variable presenting summarized habit frequency was calculated for each category separately, as the number of times participants answered a prompt and reported a habit that could be assigned to either the food intake (frequency, food) or hygiene (frequency, hygiene) category, divided by the total number of answered prompts. We conducted an independent samples t-test to compare habit frequency between groups. To investigate associations of frequency, food and frequency, hygiene with variables of interest (sensitivity analysis), including BMI-SDS, leptin (i.e., logleptin, see Supplementary Materials File S2.3.), and ED symptoms (EDI-2), as well as with possibly confounding variables such as age, depressive and anxious symptoms (BDI-II; STAI), obsessive-compulsive traits (ZWIK), and duration of illness (DOI), we conducted Pearson correlations and linear regression analysis. Correction for multiple testing was done via the false discovery rate (FDR [50]).
In addition to habit frequency, we also calculated the average habit strength for each category (averaged SRHI, food and SRHI, hygiene) for each participant. As habit strength was not assessed on a momentary level but only once per habit, we used an independent samples t-test to investigate group differences between AN and HC for the average habit strength for each category.

3. Results

3.1. Sample

Table 1 summarizes the results of the group comparisons for the demographic and clinical variables. As expected, BMI-SDS and leptin were lower in AN than HC, and patients had higher ED (EDI-2) and depressive (BDI-II), anxious (STAI), and obsessive-compulsive (ZWIK) symptoms. HC were slightly older than AN (see Methods). As in previous studies [46], compliance in EMA was generally good in both groups, but significantly higher in AN than HC, with patients answering on average 83.87% of their prompts compared with 76.35% in HC. For basic sociodemographic characteristics (ethnicity, education, and socioeconomic status), see Supplementary Materials Table S1.

3.2. EMA Data

Altogether, participants provided 4729 data points, during which either food intake (n = 1458), hygiene (n = 1642), or no (n = 1629) habit was reported. The results of the HGLMs showed a significant effect of the diagnostic group on habit occurrence for both outcomes (food intake, model 1 and hygiene, model 2). Specifically, the likelihood that a habit was reported was significantly increased in patients with AN compared with HC (food intake: odds ratio (95% confidence interval (CI)): 1.78 (1.35, 2.35)) and hygiene: odds ratio (95% CI): 1.78 (1.16, 1.88); Table 2) when controlling for age, compliance rates, and study progress (i.e., trigger). Excluding two participants with co-morbid OCD diagnosis or 14 AN subtype binge-eating–purging, did not change the results of the main models (Supplementary Materials Tables S2 and S3).
The results of the two hierarchical generalized linear models, as implemented in HLM, used the food habits (model 1) and hygiene habits (model 2) as outcomes. The results reported the coefficients, standard error, and p values of the population average model with robust standard errors.
The group differences were confirmed by simple descriptive analyses showing that, over the course of the study, frequency, food and frequency, hygiene were significantly higher in patients with AN compared with HC, whereas the number of prompts that were answered but where no habit was indicated, were not different between groups (Table 1). An exploratory analysis excluding patients that were not treated as inpatients (n = 5) had no influence on the statistical significance of the group comparisons. An independent samples t-test did not reveal any general group difference in SRHI, food and SRHI, hygiene (Table 1).
The results of the sensitivity analysis revealed a significant association between frequency, hygiene and BMI-SDS (r = −0.357, p = 0.006), and a weaker association with leptin in the AN sample (r = −0.31, p = 0.036), indicating that patients characterized by more severe underweight (and to some degree by lower leptin values, which, however, did not survive correction for multiple testing) displayed a higher number of habits during the week of EMA sampling (Supplementary Materials Table S4). No associations between frequency, food and BMI-SDS (r = −0.214, p = not significant (n.s.)) or leptin (r = −0.062. p = n.s.) were observed. Analyses of potentially confounding variables (e.g., age, BDI-II, STAI, ZWIK (including the washing and contamination compulsion subscale), or DOI) did not show any significant correlation with frequency, food or frequency, hygiene, or SRHI, food and SRHI, hygiene (Supplementary Materials Table S4).

4. Discussion

The aim of the current study was to investigate ED-specific and ED-unspecific habitual behavior and its potential relationships with ED symptoms and severity in the daily lives of acutely underweight AN patients compared with HC. In line with our hypotheses, patients with AN reported carrying out not only ED-specific (food intake), but also ED-unspecific (hygiene) habitual behavior more frequently than HC. These findings provide evidence that there might be a general tendency for behavior to be guided by habits in AN.
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to apply EMA to a sample of patients with AN to probe habitual behavior in daily life. Using EMA helps circumvent the challenges of assessing these often unconscious, “automatic” behaviors in a laboratory setting (as outlined in the introduction). Supported by two different analytic approaches, our results indicate increased habit frequency, independent of habit category. Moreover, lower BMI-SDS (as well as plasma leptin levels at trend level) in AN patients was associated with an even higher frequency of ED-unspecific habits, indicating a relationship between the severity of the disorder (and degree of undernutrition) and proneness to habitual behaviors. This is in line with previous reports of increased psychological symptoms (depressive, anxious, or OCD symptoms) with lower BMI [51]. Therefore, the current EMA results not only validate previous findings suggesting increased reliance of AN patients on habitual behavior in ED-related behavior patterns (e.g., food restriction, compensatory behavior, and eating rituals [25,52,53]), but also hint toward the importance of habitual behavior in this patient population in general. Moreover, they emphasize the importance of widening the scope to not only focus on a narrow range of behaviors and symptoms of a disorder [54], which has implications for both future research and the development of new therapeutic approaches.
What could the reasons be that some individuals have a stronger tendency to engage in habitual behaviors than others? It has been assumed that habits may have an adaptive function by enabling complex, cognitively costly actions to be carried out with little deliberation, thereby “freeing up” the processing capacity for higher-order strategic tasks [55,56]. For example, there have been suggestions that patients with AN are characterized by increased goal pursuit and proactive control processes [52,57,58,59]. In order to be able to exert such high levels of control successfully, it might be necessary to free up additional cognitive resources by relying on behavior patterns that do not draw upon additional resources. Consequently, the efficiency of habits appears particularly relevant under conditions of heavy load, such as exhaustion or pressure [43]. This also seems to be relevant considering the extreme burden that is associated with the acutely underweight state in patients with AN, which is often associated with neuroendocrinological or metabolic alterations [60], as well as depressive and anxious symptoms [61]. Together, these strains might cause symptomatic patients to operate in their daily lives on a more efficient “habit-driven” mode. If that is the case, we would not expect to see the same pattern in patients following weight restoration or recovery.
Yet another alternative explanation is based on previous research suggesting that rigid, repetitive eating behavior sequences might help reduce anxiety during mealtimes [62,63]. Indeed, although habits were not related to person-level anxiety symptoms in our sample, such behavioral sequences were found to be elevated in patients who had at least one comorbid diagnosis of anxiety disorder [64]. As rigid, repetitive behavior patterns have been reported to significantly decrease in AN patients after treatment [64,65], it would be interesting to look more closely at the changes in habit frequency in patients with AN during weight gain, as well as momentary associations between anxiety and habitual behavior. Of note, in the current study, habit frequency (of either category) was neither related to obsessive-compulsive symptoms nor did the results change when participants with a comorbid OCD diagnosis were excluded. This finding distinguishes our habit assessment from purely compulsive behavior patterns.
In the present study, we did not find increased habit strength (as assessed with SRHI) in AN patients, neither across nor within the two habit categories. As habitual behavior is acquired through repetition, the frequency of a specific behavior is assumed to be related to the degree of automaticity [66,67]. Previous research by Davis and colleagues has shown that the habit strength of ED-specific habits was stronger in AN patients than HC, and was related to ED symptom severity and illness duration [25]. However, Davis et al. [25] only targeted specific ED-relevant behavior patterns (food restriction, eating delay, compensatory behaviors, and eating rituals) that might not be directly comparable to our “food intake” category and were only assessed once. Additionally, with the item “the respective behavior x is typical me”, the SRHI also extends the definition of habits by connecting it to concepts of the self-identity [68]. It might be that the extent to which eating-related and hygiene habits are part of an individual’s self-identity are different between AN and HC, as the latter has been shown to be altered in AN [69,70].
Two studies in patients with AN using task-based assessments of habits and habit formation have shown heterogeneous results: An outcome-devaluation task designed to measure the conflict of habit learning with goal-directed behavior has not been able to confirm theories of a generally faster habit formation in AN [22]. However, the designs of outcome-devaluation tasks have generally been criticized for tapping predominantly into goal-directed behavior rather than habit formation [28,71]. This might be because training durations in these tasks are often insufficient to instill sufficiently strong habits [28]. Another study used a two-step decision task and a computational model that assessed the degree of model-based (goal-directed) versus model-free (habitual) learning in AN [52]. Rigid behavior patterns across psychiatric disorders have been attributed to an imbalance of the two learning approaches [72,73]. In line with the current results, this imbalance was also found in AN, as model-based learning in a sample of AN patients was decreased compared with HC [52] for both ED-specific (food snack) and ED-unspecific (money) domains. Notions of possible subgroups within AN patients regarding goal-directed vs. habitual aspects of learning might also play a role [74]; however, no such subgroups were visible in the current study. Further research, possibly using other tasks and setups, might be necessary to further establish the role of habits in AN.
Our findings should be interpreted while taking the following limitations into account. First, opportunities to carry out food-related habits might have been different for patients, given the intensive treatment setting. The same limitation might apply to certain (unusual) habits related to hygiene. However, if that was the case, the true group difference would be even larger than the one observed here. Second, because of limitations associated with the inpatient treatment, the exploration of habits of other categories, such as public transportation, was not feasible. Third, our data come from a rather young, non-chronic sample of patients with AN. It might be possible that a longer illness duration (which would translate into more repetitions of specific behavior), or chronicity might increase habit strength [25], which should be taken into account in future research. In addition, the present study did not investigate the possible differences between AN participants with a restrictive vs. binge–purge subtype, but merely confirmed the results in a purely restrictive subsample. On a related note, orthorexia and hygiene-related habits associated with food may constitute a dimension of AN in some individuals; however, this was not formally assessed by our diagnostic procedures. Even though EMA generally entails less memory bias than retrospective measures, one should still be cautious using self-report measures to assess behavior that is assumed to be carried out without consciously initiating it. Lastly, although we identified more habitual behavior in acutely underweight patients with AN, the current study did not allow us to determine whether the found effects were primarily associated with the underweight state (given the correlation with BMI and leptin), or whether they might reflect a trait, possibly playing a role in the etiology and maintenance of the disorder. Therefore, future research should attempt to disambiguate between the state and trait factors. Furthermore, as leptin did not show a strong association with habitual behaviors, it would also be interesting to investigate other nutrition-related parameters such as albumin, zinc, or vitamins [75]. Last, but not least, it might be promising for future research to disentangle the specific association between habitual behavior and ED symptoms more closely, e.g., via different statistical approaches such as network analysis [76], potentially also in other eating disorders such as bulimia nervosa.

5. Conclusions

Taken together, using EMA, we found increased habit frequency in ED-specific and ED-unspecific behavior sequences in patients with acute AN. While previous studies in patients with AN using outcome-devaluation tasks designed to measure the conflict of habit learning with goal-directed behavior have not been able to confirm theories of altered habit formation in AN [20], our study strengthens the habit theory in AN by showing the relevance of habits in daily life for these patients beyond the ED symptom level. This also supports the development and application of innovative therapeutic interventions targeting habitual behavior in EDs, for instance habit-reversal training [47], which has been successfully applied to other psychiatric disorders such as OCD [69] or tic disorders [70].

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/nu14193905/s1. File S1: Participants; File S2: Methods; File S2.1: Clinical and ecological momentary assessment (EMA) measures [39,40]; File S2.2: Blood samples [26]; File S2.3: Statistical Analyses [77,78]; Table S1: Sociodemographic sample characteristics [79]; Table S2: Multilevel estimates for models predicting habit occurrence excluding AN with comorbid obsessive-compulsive disorder (n = 2); Table S3: Multilevel estimates for models predicting habit occurrence excluding AN with binge-eating-purging subtype (n = 14); Table S4: Associations between habit frequency and clinical variables.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.S., S.F., V.R. and S.E., methodology, M.S. and S.F.; formal analysis, M.S., S.F. and S.E.; investigation, M.S., S.F., J.A.K., M.-L.W., N.L., I.B. and J.H.; resources, K.G., V.R. and S.E.; data curation, M.S., J.A.K., M.-L.W., N.L., J.H., K.G. and I.B.; writing—original draft preparation, M.S., J.A.K. and S.E.; writing—review and editing, all authors; supervision, S.E.; project administration, M.S., S.F., J.A.K., I.B., J.H. and S.E.; funding acquisition, V.R. and S.E. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the German Research Foundation DFG SFB940/2.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and was approved by the Institutional Review Board of TU Dresden, Germany EK536122015 on 1 March 2016.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are available upon reasonable request from the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

In the last two years, Roessner has received payment for consulting and writing activities from Lilly, Novartis, and Shire Pharmaceuticals; lecture honoraria from Lilly, Novartis, Shire Pharmaceuticals, and Medice Pharma; and support for research from Shire and Novartis. He has carried out (and is currently carrying out) clinical trials in cooperation with the Novartis, Shire, and Otsuka companies.

References

  1. Coniglio, K.A.; Becker, K.R.; Franko, D.L.; Zayas, L.V.; Plessow, F.; Eddy, K.T.; Thomas, J.J. Won’t Stop or Can’t Stop? Food Restriction as a Habitual Behavior among Individuals with Anorexia Nervosa or Atypical Anorexia Nervosa. Eat. Behav. 2017, 26, 144–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Walsh, B.T. The Enigmatic Persistence of Anorexia Nervosa. Am. J. Psychiatry 2013, 170, 477–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Steinglass, J.E.; Walsh, B.T. Neurobiological Model of the Persistence of Anorexia Nervosa. J. Eat. Disord. 2016, 4, 19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Dickinson, A. Actions and Habits: The Development of Behavioural Autonomy. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 1985, 308, 67–78. [Google Scholar]
  5. Graybiel, A.M. Habits, Rituals, and the Evaluative Brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2008, 31, 359–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Dolan, R.J.; Dayan, P. Goals and Habits in the Brain. Neuron 2013, 80, 312–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Haith, A.M.; Krakauer, J.W. The Multiple Effects of Practice: Skill, Habit and Reduced Cognitive Load. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2018, 20, 196–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Abbate-Daga, G.; Buzzichelli, S.; Marzola, E.; Amianto, F.; Fassino, S. Clinical Investigation of Set-Shifting Subtypes in Anorexia Nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 2014, 219, 592–597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Mayer, L.E.; Schebendach, J.; Bodell, L.P.; Shingleton, R.M.; Walsh, B.T. Eating Behavior in Anorexia Nervosa: Before and after Treatment. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2012, 45, 290–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Schebendach, J.E.; Mayer, L.E.; Devlin, M.J.; Attia, E.; Contento, I.R.; Wolf, R.L.; Walsh, B.T. Food Choice and Diet Variety in Weight-Restored Patients with Anorexia Nervosa. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2011, 111, 732–736. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Sysko, R.; Walsh, B.T.; Schebendach, J.; Wilson, G.T. Eating Behavior among Women with Anorexia Nervosa. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2005, 82, 296–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Uniacke, B.; Timothy Walsh, B.; Steinglass, J.E.; Steinglass, J. The Role of Habits in Anorexia Nervosa: Where We Are and Where to Go from Here? Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2018, 20, 61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Keys, A.; Brozek, J.; Henschel, A.; Michelsen, O.; Taylor, H.L.; Simonson, E.; Skinner, A.S.; Wells, S.M. The Biology of Human Starvation; University of Minnesota Press: Minneapolis, MN, USA, 1950; Volumes 1 and 2. [Google Scholar]
  14. Hebebrand, J.; Hildebrandt, T.; Schlögl, H.; Seitz, J.; Denecke, S.; Vieira, D.; Gradl-Dietsch, G.; Peters, T.; Antel, J.; Lau, D.; et al. The Role of Hypoleptinemia in the Psychological and Behavioral Adaptation to Starvation: Implications for Anorexia Nervosa. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2022, 141, 104807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Cederlöf, M.; Thornton, L.M.; Baker, J.; Lichtenstein, P.; Larsson, H.; Rück, C.; Bulik, C.M.; Mataix-Cols, D. Etiological Overlap between Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder and Anorexia Nervosa: A Longitudinal Cohort, Multigenerational Family and Twin Study. World Psychiatry 2015, 14, 333–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Davis, C.; Kaptein, S. Anorexia Nervosa with Excessive Exercise: A Phenotype with Close Links to Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2006, 142, 209–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Godier, L.R.; Park, R.J. Compulsivity in Anorexia Nervosa: A Transdiagnostic Concept. Front. Psychol. 2014, 17, 778. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Ferreira, G.M.; Yucel, M.; Dawson, A.; Lorenzetti, V.; Fontenelle, L.F. Investigating the Role of Anticipatory Reward and Habit Strength in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. CNS Spectr. 2017, 22, 295–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Gillan, C.M.; Papmeyer, M.; Morein-Zamir, S.; Sahakian, B.J.; Fineberg, N.A.; Robbins, T.W.; de Wit, S. Disruption in the Balance between Goal-Directed Behavior and Habit Learning in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 2011, 168, 718–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Gillan, C.M.; Morein-Zamir, S.; Urcelay, G.P.; Sule, A.; Voon, V.; Apergis-Schoute, A.M.; Fineberg, N.A.; Sahakian, B.J.; Robbins, T.W. Enhanced Avoidance Habits in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 2014, 75, 631–638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Yilmaz, Z.; Halvorsen, M.; Bryois, J.; Yu, D.; Thornton, L.M.; Zerwas, S.; Micali, N.; Moessner, R.; Burton, C.L.; Zai, G.; et al. Examination of the Shared Genetic Basis of Anorexia Nervosa and Obsessive–Compulsive Disorder. Mol. Psychiatry 2020, 25, 2036–2046. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Godier, L.R.; de Wit, S.; Pinto, A.; Steinglass, J.E.; Greene, A.L.; Scaife, J.; Gillan, C.M.; Walsh, B.T.; Simpson, H.-B.; Park, R.J. An Investigation of Habit Learning in Anorexia Nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 2016, 244, 214–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Haynos, A.F.; Hall, L.M.; Lavender, J.M.; Peterson, C.B.; Crow, S.J.; Klimes-Dougan, B.; Cullen, K.R.; Lim, K.O.; Camchong, J. Resting State Functional Connectivity of Networks Associated with Reward and Habit in Anorexia Nervosa. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2019, 40, 652–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Steinglass, J.E.; Walsh, B.T. Habit Learning and Anorexia Nervosa: A Cognitive Neuroscience Hypothesis. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2006, 39, 267–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Davis, L.; Walsh, B.T.; Schebendach, J.; Glasofer, D.R.; Steinglass, J.E. Habits Are Stronger with Longer Duration of Illness and Greater Severity in Anorexia Nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2020, 53, 683–689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Hebebrand, J.; Muller, T.D.; Holtkamp, K.; Herpertz-Dahlmann, B. The Role of Leptin in Anorexia Nervosa: Clinical Implications. Mol. Psychiatry 2007, 12, 23–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Fürtjes, S.; Seidel, M.; King, J.A.; Biemann, R.; Roessner, V.; Ehrlich, S. Rumination in Anorexia Nervosa: Cognitive-Affective and Neuroendocrinological Aspects. Behav. Res. Ther. 2018, 111, 92–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. de Wit, S.; Kindt, M.; Knot, S.L.; Verhoeven, A.A.; Robbins, T.W.; Gasull-Camos, J.; Evans, M.; Mirza, H.; Gillan, C.M. Shifting the Balance between Goals and Habits: Five Failures in Experimental Habit Induction. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2018, 147, 1043. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Gillan, C.M.; Rutledge, R.B. Smartphones and the Neuroscience of Mental Health. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2021, 44, 129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Munkres, J. Algorithms for the Assignment and Transportation Problems. J. Soc. Ind. Appl. Math. 1957, 5, 32–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013; ISBN 978-0-89042-555-8. [Google Scholar]
  32. Fichter, M.; Quadflieg, N. The Structured Interview for Anorexic and Bulimic Disorders for DSM-IV and ICD-10 (SIAB-EX): Reliability and Validity. Eur. Psychiatry 2001, 16, 38–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Sheehan, D.V.; Lecrubier, Y.; Sheehan, K.H.; Amorim, P.; Janavs, J.; Weiller, E.; Hergueta, T.; Baker, R.; Dunbar, G.C. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The Development and Validation of a Structured Diagnostic Psychiatric Interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry 1998, 59, 22–33; quiz 34–57. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  34. Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Lang, A.-G.; Buchner, A. G* Power 3: A Flexible Statistical Power Analysis Program for the Social, Behavioral, and Biomedical Sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 2007, 39, 175–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Harris, P.A.; Taylor, R.; Thielke, R.; Payne, J.; Gonzalez, N.; Conde, J.G. Research Electronic Data Capture (REDCap)—A Metadata-Driven Methodology and Workflow Process for Providing Translational Research Informatics Support. J. Biomed. Inform. 2009, 42, 377–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Thiel, A.; Jacobi, C.; Horstmann, S.; Paul, T.; Nutzinger, D.O.; Schüßler, G. Eine Deutschsprachige Version Des Eating Disorder Inventory EDI-2. [German Translation of the Eating Disorder Inventory EDI-2.]. PPmP Psychother. Psychosom. Med. Psychol. 1997, 47, 365–376. [Google Scholar]
  37. Hautzinger, M.; Keller, F.; Kühner, C. Beck Depressions Inventar: Revised; Huber: Bern, Switzerland, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  38. Spielberger, C.D.; Gorsuch, R.L.; Lushene, R.E. STAI Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory; Consulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA, USA, 1970. [Google Scholar]
  39. Goletz, H.; Döpfner, M. Diagnostik von Zwangsstörungen Im Kindes-Und Jugendalter. Kindh. Entwickl. 2007, 16, 117–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Goletz, H.; Döpfner, M. ZWIK, Zwangsinventar Für Kinder Und Jugendliche. Klin. Psychiatr. Rat. Für Kindes Jugendalter. Hogrefe Göttingen 2011, 6, 489–493. [Google Scholar]
  41. Hemmelmann, C.; Brose, S.; Vens, M.; Hebebrand, J.; Ziegler, A. Perzentilen Des Body-Mass-Index Auch Für 18-Bis 80-Jährige? Daten Der Nationalen Verzehrsstudie II. DMW-Dtsch. Med. Wochenschr. 2010, 135, 848–852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Kromeyer-Hauschild, K.; Wabitsch, M.; Kunze, D.; Geller, F.; Geiß, H.C.; Hesse, V.; Hippel, A.; von Jaeger, U.; Johnsen, D.; Korte, W.; et al. Perzentile für den Body-mass-Index für das Kindes- und Jugendalter unter Heranziehung verschiedener deutscher Stichproben. Mon. Kinderheilkd 2001, 149, 807–818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Verplanken, B.; Orbell, S. Reflections on Past Behavior: A Self-Report Index of Habit Strength. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 33, 1313–1330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Piquet-Pessôa, M.; Chamberlain, S.R.; Lee, R.S.; Ferreira, G.M.; Cruz, M.S.; Ribeiro, A.P.; De Menezes, G.B.; Albertella, L.; Yücel, M.; Fontenelle, L.F. A Study on the Correlates of Habit-, Reward-, and Fear-Related Motivations in Alcohol Use Disorder. CNS Spectr. 2019, 24, 597–604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Fürtjes, S.; Seidel, M.; King, J.A.; Boehm, I.; Jaite, C.; Roessner, V.; Ehrlich, S. A Naturalistic Investigation of Cognitive-Affective Dysfunction in Anorexia Nervosa: The Role of Inefficiency. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2019, 53, 239–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Seidel, M.; Petermann, J.; Diestel, S.; Ritschel, F.; Boehm, I.; King, J.A.; Geisler, D.; Bernardoni, F.; Roessner, V.; Goschke, T.; et al. A Naturalistic Examination of Negative Affect and Disorder-Related Rumination in Anorexia Nervosa. Eur. Child. Adolesc. Psychiatry 2016, 25, 1207–1216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Hox, J.J. Multilevel Analysis of Grouped and Longitudinal Data. Modeling Longitudinal and Multilevel Data: Practical Issues, Applied Approaches, and Specific Examples; Routledge: London, UK, 2000; pp. 15–32. [Google Scholar]
  48. Raudenbush, S.W.; Bryk, A.S.; Cheong, Y.F.; Congdon, R. HLM 8 for Windows (Version 8) [Computer Software]; Scientific Software International: Skokie, IL, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  49. Young, M.L.; Preisser, J.S.; Qaqish, B.F.; Wolfson, M. Comparison of Subject-Specific and Population Averaged Models for Count Data from Cluster-Unit Intervention Trials. Stat. Methods Med. Res. 2007, 16, 167–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Benjamini, Y.; Hochberg, Y. Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B Methodol. 1995, 57, 289–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Pleple, A.; Lalanne, C.; Huas, C.; Mattar, L.; Hanachi, M.; Flament, M.F.; Carchon, I.; Jouen, F.; Berthoz, S.; Godart, N. Nutritional Status and Anxious and Depressive Symptoms in Anorexia Nervosa: A Prospective Study. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Foerde, K.; Daw, N.D.; Rufin, T.; Walsh, B.T.; Shohamy, D.; Steinglass, J.E. Deficient Goal-Directed Control in a Population Characterized by Extreme Goal Pursuit. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2021, 33, 463–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Steinglass, J.E.; Glasofer, D.R.; Walsh, E.; Guzman, G.; Peterson, C.B.; Walsh, B.T.; Attia, E.; Wonderlich, S.A. Targeting Habits in Anorexia Nervosa: A Proof-of-Concept Randomized Trial. Psychol. Med. 2018, 48, 2584–2591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Haynos, A.; Widge, A.; Anderson, L.; Redish, A.D. Beyond Description and Deficits: How Computational Psychiatry Can Enhance an Understanding of Decision-Making in Anorexia Nervosa. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2021, 24, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Förster, J.; Jostmann, N.B. What Is Automatic Self-Regulation? Z. Für Psychol. 2015, 220, 147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Wood, W. Habit in Personality and Social Psychology. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2017, 21, 389–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Bartholdy, S.; Rennalls, S.J.; Jacques, C.; Danby, H.; Campbell, I.C.; Schmidt, U.; O’Daly, O.G. Proactive and Reactive Inhibitory Control in Eating Disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2017, 255, 432–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. King, J.A.; Geisler, D.; Bernardoni, F.; Ritschel, F.; Bohm, I.; Seidel, M.; Mennigen, E.; Ripke, S.; Smolka, M.N.; Roessner, V.; et al. Altered Neural Efficiency of Decision Making During Temporal Reward Discounting in Anorexia Nervosa. J. Am. Acad Child. Adolesc. Psychiatry 2016, 55, 972–979. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Weinert, T.; King, J.A.; Böldt, L.; Gronow, F.; Roessner, V.; Ehrlich, S. Increased Self-Reported Delay of Gratification in Acutely Underweight, but Not Remitted Anorexia Nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2021, 55, 135–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Kaye, W.H.; Wierenga, C.E.; Bailer, U.F.; Simmons, A.N.; Bischoff-Grethe, A. Nothing Tastes as Good as Skinny Feels: The Neurobiology of Anorexia Nervosa. Trends Neurosci. 2013, 36, 110–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Salbach-Andrae, H.; Lenz, K.; Simmendinger, N.; Klinkowski, N.; Lehmkuhl, U.; Pfeiffer, E. Psychiatric Comorbidities among Female Adolescents with Anorexia Nervosa. Child. Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2008, 39, 261–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Gianini, L.; Liu, Y.; Wang, Y.; Attia, E.; Walsh, B.T.; Steinglass, J. Abnormal Eating Behavior in Video-Recorded Meals in Anorexia Nervosa. Eat. Behav. 2015, 19, 28–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Steinglass, J.E.; Sysko, R.; Glasofer, D.; Albano, A.M.; Simpson, H.B.; Walsh, B.T. Rationale for the Application of Exposure and Response Prevention to the Treatment of Anorexia Nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2011, 44, 134–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Calugi, S.; Chignola, E.; Dalle Grave, R. A Longitudinal Study of Eating Rituals in Patients with Anorexia Nervosa. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Jordan, J.; Joyce, P.R.; Carter, F.A.; McIntosh, V.V.; Luty, S.E.; McKenzie, J.M.; Frampton, C.M.; Bulik, C.M. The Yale–Brown–Cornell Eating Disorder Scale in Women with Anorexia Nervosa: What Is It Measuring? Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2009, 42, 267–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Gardner, B. A Review and Analysis of the Use of ‘Habit’in Understanding, Predicting and Influencing Health-Related Behaviour. Health Psychol. Rev. 2015, 9, 277–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Orbell, S.; Verplanken, B. The Automatic Component of Habit in Health Behavior: Habit as Cue-Contingent Automaticity. Health Psychol. 2010, 29, 374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Verplanken, B.; Sui, J. Habit and Identity: Behavioral, Cognitive, Affective, and Motivational Facets of an Integrated Self. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 1504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. McAdams, C.J.; Krawczyk, D.C. Who Am I? How Do I Look? Neural Differences in Self-Identity in Anorexia Nervosa. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2014, 9, 12–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  70. Oldershaw, A.; Startup, H.; Lavender, T. Anorexia Nervosa and a Lost Emotional Self: A Psychological Formulation of the Development, Maintenance, and Treatment of Anorexia Nervosa. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Watson, P.; de Wit, S. Current Limits of Experimental Research into Habits and Future Directions. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2018, 20, 33–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Gillan, C.M.; Kosinski, M.; Whelan, R.; Phelps, E.A.; Daw, N.D. Characterizing a Psychiatric Symptom Dimension Related to Deficits in Goal-Directed Control. eLife 2016, 5, e11305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Voon, V.; Reiter, A.; Sebold, M.; Groman, S. Model-Based Control in Dimensional Psychiatry. Biol. Psychiatry 2017, 82, 391–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Steding, J.; Boehm, I.; King, J.A.; Geisler, D.; Ritschel, F.; Seidel, M.; Doose, A.; Jaite, C.; Roessner, V.; Smolka, M.N. Goal-Directed vs. Habitual Instrumental Behavior during Reward Processing in Anorexia Nervosa: An FMRI Study. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 1–9. [Google Scholar]
  75. Hanachi, M.; Dicembre, M.; Rives-Lange, C.; Ropers, J.; Bemer, P.; Zazzo, J.-F.; Poupon, J.; Dauvergne, A.; Melchior, J.-C. Micronutrients Deficiencies in 374 Severely Malnourished Anorexia Nervosa Inpatients. Nutrients 2019, 11, 792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Spanakis, G.; Weiss, G.; Boh, B.; Roefs, A. Network Analysis of Ecological Momentary Assessment Data for Monitoring and Understanding Eating Behavior. In ICSH; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2015; pp. 43–54. [Google Scholar]
  77. Lazar, C. ImputeLCMD: A Collection of Methods for Left-Censored Missing Data Imputation. In R Package, Version 2.0. 2015. Available online: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/imputeLCMD/imputeLCMD.pdf (accessed on 7 August 2022).
  78. Wei, R.; Wang, J.; Jia, E.; Chen, T.; Ni, Y.; Jia, W. GSimp: A Gibbs Sampler Based Left-Censored Missing Value Imputation Approach for Metabolomics Studies. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2018, 14, e1005973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  79. Ganzeboom, H.B. International Standard Classification of Occupations ISCO-08 with ISEI-08 Scores; International Labour Office: Geneva, Switzerland, 2010. [Google Scholar]
Table 1. Descriptive sample characteristics.
Table 1. Descriptive sample characteristics.
Descriptivesn (AN/HC)ANHCtp
Age57/5715.89 ± 2.0517.46 ± 3.013.250.002
BMI57/5714.46 ± 1.3421.01 ± 2.0420.28<0.001
BMI-SDS57/57−3.33 ± 1.09−0.03 ± 0.6119.96<0.001
Log10Leptin46/56−0.31 ± 0.801.01 ± 0.2910.60<0.001
Duration of Illness57/-11.66 ± 11.49
EDI-250/57217.88 ± 43.16132.65 ± 24.95−12.69<0.001
BDI-II54/5725.61 ± 10.314.98 ± 5.02−13.51<0.001
STAI(K)-t52/5749.31 ± 12.9334.16 ± 8.617.13<0.001
ZWIK51/5749.25 ± 17.1738.26 ± 9.933.64<0.001
Ecological Momentary Assessment
Compliance (%)57/5783.87 ± 14.15%76.35 ± 15.57−2.700.008
Frequency, hygiene57/570.40 ± 0.290.23 ± 0.154.09<0.001
Frequency, food57/570.39 ± 0.230.19 ± 0.145.05<0.001
Prompts accepted but no habit56/5713.12 ± 11.9515.45 ± 11.271.07n.s.
SRHI, hygiene57/5466.39 ± 8.3468.26 ± 8.181.19n.s.
SRHI, food56/5156.71 ± 13.0859.52 ± 10.941.19n.s.
The results of the group comparisons of descriptive data using independent samples t-tests reported the mean, standard deviation, t values, and statistical significance. Frequency, food/hygiene is the total amount of reported food/hygiene habits divided by the total amount of answered prompts over the duration of seven days. n = 43 (75.4%) of AN patients were of the restricting subtype. Abbreviations used are as follows: AN = acute anorexia nervosa, BMI = body mass index, BMI-SDS = body mass index standard deviation score, BDI-II = Beck Depression Inventory II, compliance = percentage of answered prompts with a possible total of 56, duration of illness is given in months, EDI-2 = Eating Disorder Inventory 2, HC = healthy control, n.s. = not significant, SRHI = Self-Report Habit Index, STAI (K)-t = State and trait Anxiety Inventory (children version)-trait anxiety score, ZWIK = Zwangsinventar für Kinder und Jugendliche.
Table 2. Multilevel estimates for models predicting habit occurrence.
Table 2. Multilevel estimates for models predicting habit occurrence.
Model 1: Frequency-Food
(n = 3087)
Model 2: Frequency-Hygiene
(n = 3271)
ParameterCoefficientSEpCoefficientSEp
Intercept0.340.120.0060.470.11<0.001
Group0.570.13<0.0010.390.130.002
Age−0.010.04n.s.−0.020.04n.s.
Compliance−2.420.69<0.001−1.490.76n.s.
Trigger−0.010.00<0.001−0.010.00<0.001
Trigger × Group0.000.00n.s.0.000.00n.s.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Seidel, M.; King, J.A.; Fürtjes, S.; Labitzke, N.; Wronski, M.-L.; Boehm, I.; Hennig, J.; Gramatke, K.; Roessner, V.; Ehrlich, S. Increased Habit Frequency in the Daily Lives of Patients with Acute Anorexia Nervosa. Nutrients 2022, 14, 3905. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14193905

AMA Style

Seidel M, King JA, Fürtjes S, Labitzke N, Wronski M-L, Boehm I, Hennig J, Gramatke K, Roessner V, Ehrlich S. Increased Habit Frequency in the Daily Lives of Patients with Acute Anorexia Nervosa. Nutrients. 2022; 14(19):3905. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14193905

Chicago/Turabian Style

Seidel, Maria, Joseph A. King, Sophia Fürtjes, Natalie Labitzke, Marie-Louis Wronski, Ilka Boehm, Julius Hennig, Katrin Gramatke, Veit Roessner, and Stefan Ehrlich. 2022. "Increased Habit Frequency in the Daily Lives of Patients with Acute Anorexia Nervosa" Nutrients 14, no. 19: 3905. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14193905

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop