Next Article in Journal
Relationship between 25 Hydroxyvitamin D, Overweight/Obesity Status, Pro-Inflammatory and Oxidative Stress Markers in Patients with Type 2 Diabetes: A Simplified Empirical Path Model
Previous Article in Journal
Sargassum fusiforme Alginate Relieves Hyperglycemia and Modulates Intestinal Microbiota and Metabolites in Type 2 Diabetic Mice
Previous Article in Special Issue
Do Breastfeeding History and Diet Quality Predict Inhibitory Control at Preschool Age?
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Parental Reports on Early Autism Behaviors in Their Children with Fragile X Syndrome as a Function of Infant Feeding

Department of Neurology, Molecular & Environmental Toxicology Center, University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI 53706, USA
Nutrients 2021, 13(8), 2888; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13082888
Submission received: 10 July 2021 / Revised: 13 August 2021 / Accepted: 20 August 2021 / Published: 22 August 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Nutrition in Early Life and Health Outcome)

Abstract

:
This study evaluates the prevalence of autistic behaviors in fragile X syndrome as a function of infant diet. Retrospective survey data from the Fragile X Syndrome Nutrition Study, which included data on infant feeding and caregiver-reported developmental milestones for 190 children with fragile X syndrome enrolled in the Fragile X Online Registry with Accessible Database (FORWARD), were analyzed. Exploratory, sex-specific associations were found linking the use of soy-based infant formula with worse autistic behaviors related to language in females and self-injurious behavior in males. These findings prompt prospective evaluation of the effects of soy-based infant formula on disease comorbidities in fragile X syndrome, a rare disorder for which newborn screening could be implemented if there was an intervention. Gastrointestinal problems were the most common reason cited for switching to soy-based infant formula. Thus, these findings also support the study of early gastrointestinal problems in fragile X syndrome, which may underly the development and severity of disease comorbidities. In conjunction with comorbidity data from the previous analyses of the Fragile X Syndrome Nutrition Study, the findings indicate that premutation fragile X mothers should be encouraged to breastfeed.

1. Introduction

Autism is a complex neurological disorder with core features of impaired communication and social interaction and repetitive stereotypical behavior [1]. Recent prevalence estimates indicate that 1 in 54 children in the United States have an autism spectrum disorder (ASD), with rates 4.3-fold higher in males compared to females [2]. The global prevalence of autism is in the range of 0.8–93 per 1000 [3]. In the majority of cases, the underlying cause of the ASD is not known; however, genetic as well as environmental factors are suspected to contribute to the development and severity of autism [4,5,6,7]. For example, FXS is the leading known genetic cause of autism, with half of males and approximately 20% of females with FXS meeting autism diagnostic criteria and accounting for 5% of autism cases [8,9,10]. There is a dearth of studies assessing the effects of environment on ASD and FXS phenotypes, albeit accumulating evidence suggests that postnatal diet is associated with seizure and autistic behavioral outcomes in autism and FXS models.
Single-source soy-based diets are associated with increased sensitivity to audiogenic-induced seizures in Fmr1KO mice [11]; increased prevalence of febrile seizures, simple partial seizures and epilepsy comorbidity in autism [12]; deficits in language, communication, social overtures and hypersensitivity to environmental stimuli in autistic children [13]; and increased comorbidity of autism, gastrointestinal problems and allergies in FXS [14]. Conversely, long-term infant feeding with breast milk is associated with decreased prevalence of autism in FXS [15]. There are several potential bioactive components in soy that could reduce seizure threshold and exacerbate autistic behaviors, i.e., soy phytoestrogens and agrochemicals [16,17].
The aim of this study was to begin to fill an important gap in the literature regarding the potential effects of early-life feeding on autistic behavioral outcomes in FXS. Retrospective survey data from the Fragile X Syndrome Nutrition Study, which included data on infant feeding and caregiver-reported developmental milestones and autistic behaviors for 190 children with fragile X syndrome enrolled in the Fragile X Online Registry with Accessible Database (FORWARD), were analyzed. FORWARD is a longitudinal database that collects clinician- and parent-reported data on individuals diagnosed with FXS [18]. Exploratory, sex-specific associations were found linking the use of soy-based infant formula with worse autistic behaviors related to language in females and self-injurious behavior in males. This is important considering the dearth of prior diet studies in FXS and the growing consensus that 40–50% of ASD variance could be due to environmental factors including nutrition [7].

2. Methods and Subjects

Study Design: The impact of early infant diet on the severity of common FXS phenotypes was assessed through a retrospective survey study utilizing the FORWARD as a sampling frame. The study design, Fragile X Syndrome Nutrition Study questionnaire, study population, recruitment success, and data collection have been previously described [14,15]. Data collection for the survey began in November of 2019 and lasted 3 months. The hypothesis explored herein is that early infant diet is associated with specific autistic behaviors in FXS.
Sample Selection and Demographic Characteristics: Inclusion criteria for participation were full-mutation FXS status, caregiver willingness to participate in the study, and completion of behavior-related questions (Q)40, Q41 and Q43–46 on the survey. The dataset included 190 participants who were 74% male and predominantly white (Table 1). The average age of participants on the date of the survey was 18 years (SEM = 0.74). Specific data on socioeconomic status (SES) and educational attainment were not collected.
Ethics Approvals: This study was reviewed by the University of Wisconsin-Madison Health Sciences IRB and determined to meet the criteria for exempt Human Subjects research in accordance with Category 2 defined under 45 CFR-46. Participants provided written informed consent through their enrollment in FORWARD.
Data Collection: Data related to normal child development and autistic behaviors were collected on Q40, Q41 and Q43–Q46 of the Fragile X Syndrome Nutrition Study questionnaire (Table 2). Questions were adapted from several standardized instruments including cognitive tools such as the Ages and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ) for 36-months as well as autistic tools such as the Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers-Revised (M-CHAT-R) and the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) parental survey. Standardized tests were not used due to copyright issues and the inability to make modifications to the questions, for example, verb tense for a retrospective survey. Caregivers were asked to rank the behavior of their child with FXS in seven categories (Language, Cognitive, Child Play, Motor Skills, Autistic Behavior, Hypersensory, and Parent Thought there was a Problem) on a 5-point Likert scale (never, rarely, sometimes, very often, and extremely often), and responses were converted to numerical values (1–5, respectively). Caregivers were instructed to remember how their child behaved when he or she was approximately 3 years old and were told that they could skip any of the questions or the entire section if they did not remember.
Data Analysis: Data were analyzed in accordance with STROBE guidelines. Percentages, means, standard error of the means (SEM) and Student t-tests were computed to describe the cohorts. Two-way ANOVA with a Tukey multiple comparison correction was applied to the analysis of individual questions and behavior categories (grouped questions) to examine the null hypotheses that the severity of autistic behaviors in FXS is the same in infants regardless of diet using Prism version 9.0.1 (128) (GraphPad Software, LLC, San Diego, CA, USA). Statistical significance was defined as * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, and **** p < 0.0001.

3. Results

3.1. Study Population

The study population included full-mutation FXS males and females, with an average age of 18 years at the time of the survey (Table 1). Caregivers retrospectively answered survey questions regarding disease comorbidities, infant diet and autistic behaviors. Of relevance to this analysis, multi-part questions from survey questions Q40, Q41 and Q43–Q46, which elicited Likert-scale responses regarding normal child development and the severity of autistic phenotypes, were posed to caregivers. Considering the average age at the time of the survey (18 years old) and the retrospective age of the queries (3 years old), there was a high response rate of 98% or greater for each question related to child development or autistic behavior despite caregivers being instructed to skip questions that they did not remember (Table 2). Only 9 out of 199 caregivers skipped the entire section of behavior questions and 13 skipped 1–3 individual questions. Two of the nine participants with blank answers to all of the behavior questions were adopted. Thus, there was an overall high response rate to questions on early childhood behavior indicating confidence on the part on the respondents regarding recall.

3.2. Autism Behavior Scores

A total FXS toddler behavior score was calculated by subtracting the point total for caregiver-reported autistic-related behaviors (Q43c + Q44a + Q44b +Q44c + Q44d + Q44e + Q44f + Q45a + Q45b + Q45c + Q45d + Q46a + Q46b + Q46c + Q46d) from the point total for typical childhood behaviors (Q40a + Q40b + Q40c + Q40d + Q40e + Q41a + Q41b + Q41c + Q41d + Q41e + Q41f + Q43a + Q43b + Q43d + Q43e + Q43f + Q45e). This calculated behavior score based on 32 behaviors for participants whose caregivers reported an answer of “No” to Q1a: “Does your child with Fragile X Syndrome also have Autism?” was 15 (SEM 2.0, N = 83) compared to those who answered “Yes” to Q1a having an average score of −2.1 (SEM 1.7, N = 83) with a p value = 7.8 x 10-10 by Student t-test. Thus, caregiver-reported incidence of autism (Q1a) strongly agreed with reported Likert-scale scores for autistic behaviors in their children with FXS.
The behavior questions were binned into categories based on relevance to “Language” (Q40a, Q40b, Q40c, Q40d, Q40e), “Cognitive” (Q41a, Q41b, Q41c, Q41d, Q41e, Q41f), “Child Play” (Q43a, Q45e), “Motor Skills” (Q43b, Q43d, Q43e, Q43f), “Autistic Behaviors” (Q44b, Q44f, Q43c), “Hypersensory” (Q44a, Q44c, Q44d, Q44e, Q45a, Q45b, Q45c, Q45d) and “Parent Thought Problem” (Q46a, Q46b, Q46c, Q46d) and analyzed by two-way ANOVA as a function of sex and diet. First, the data were analyzed as a function of breast milk consumption (Q18) and then as a function of soy-infant formula consumption (Q28). For the breast milk cohorts [breast milk (n = 33 female and 102 male) versus no breast milk (n = 17 female and 35 male)], sex-specific differences were found in Language, Cognition, Child Play, Motor Skills, and Autistic Behaviors, but not in Hypersensory or Parent Thought Problem categories (Table 3, Supplementary Figure S1, Supplementary Table S1). Multiple comparisons analyses found higher language and lower autistic behavior scores in females compared to males with breast milk feeding, and higher cognition scores in females compared to males with formula feeding (Table 3). No diet-based differences (breast versus no breast milk) were found with grouped or individual questions (Table 3, Supplementary Figures S1–S8, Supplementary Table S1). The total behavior score was 3.4-fold higher in females than males fed breast milk, with trends for increased scores in both females and males fed breast milk compared to no breast milk (Table 3, Supplementary Figure S1, Supplementary Table S1).
Analysis of subjects exclusively fed breast milk for 12 months or longer (i.e., no reporting of any formula use, n = 14 females and 41 males) indicated sex-specific differences in Language, Cognition, Motor Skills, and Autistic Behaviors and diet-specific differences in Language, Autistic Behaviors, and Hypersensory compared to the no breast milk cohort (n = 17 females and 35 males) (Supplementary Table S2). Multiple comparison analysis confirmed higher language scores in females versus males fed breast milk and higher cognitive scores in females versus males with formula feeding, but found no significant differences as a function of diet (Supplementary Figure S9).
For the no-soy versus soy-based infant formula cohorts, data were analyzed by two-way ANOVA based on sex and diet [soy-based infant formula (n = 10 female and 32 male) versus no soy (n = 36 female and 92 male)] with sex-specific differences found in Language, Cognition, Motor Skills, and Autistic Behaviors, but not in the Child Play, Hypersensory or Parent Thought Problem categories (Table 4, Supplementary Figure S10). Soy diet-specific differences were observed for Language, Autistic Behaviors, and Parent Thought Problem (Supplementary Table S3). Multiple comparisons analyses indicated a significantly higher language category score in females not fed soy-based infant formula compared to all other groups as well as higher cognition and motor skills scores and decreased autistic behaviors in females versus males not fed soy-based infant formula (Table 4). The autistic behaviors score was the highest in males fed soy-based infant formula and significantly reduced in males not fed soy. The total behavior score was 3.3-fold higher in females than males in the no soy cohort with trends for decreased scores in both females and males fed soy compared to no soy (Table 4, Supplementary Figure S10, Supplementary Table S3).

3.3. Indiviudal Autistic Behaviors

Analysis of individual questions related to Language indicated a 1.5-fold decrease in Q40a (talk), a 1.7-fold decrease in Q40d (three word sentences), and a 1.6-fold decrease in Q40e (use words to request things) in females such that females reporting the use of soy-based infant formula exhibited decreased expressive language scores that matched the language scores of males on either non-soy or soy-based infant formula (Table 4, Supplementary Figure S11). Analysis of individual questions for the Cognition category indicated a 1.2-fold decrease in Q41a (correctly ID) and a 1.5-fold decrease in Q41e (play pretend) in males compared to females not fed soy with the sex-specific differences lost with soy-based infant formula (Table 4, Supplementary Figure S12). Analysis of individual questions for the Autism Behaviors category indicated a 1.6-fold increase in Q44b (rocking/hand flapping/spinning) when comparing females to males not fed soy, and the sex-specific difference was lost with soy-based infant formula (Table 4, Supplementary Figure S13). There was a 1.4-fold increase in Q44f (injure self) for males fed soy-based infant formula. Analyses of individual questions for the Child Play, Motor Skills and Hypersensory categories indicated no significant differences as a function of soy-based infant formula for boys or girls with FXS (Supplementary Figures S14–S16). There was a sex-specific 1.3-fold decrease in Q43f (dress self) in males compared to females not fed soy (Table 4, Supplementary Figure S15). Analysis of individual questions related to Parent Thought Problem indicated a 1.2-fold increase in Q46d (learning problem) in females fed soy and when comparing females to males not fed soy (Table 4, Supplementary Figure S17).
There were a small number of subjects reporting use of a single diet [only breast milk (n = 8 female and 32 male), cow milk-based formula (n = 7 female and 9 male) or soy-based formula (n = 2 female and 4 male)]. Analysis as a function of single diets indicated an interaction between sex and diet for Motor Skills but no other sex- or diet-specific differences with the small cohort sizes (Supplementary Figure S18, Supplementary Table S4).

4. Discussion

There is a paucity of studies examining the effects of diet in FXS [11,14,15,19,20,21,22,23,24]. Herein, diet-responsive autistic behaviors in FXS included three behaviors in girls within the Language category (talk, speak in 3-word sentences and use words to request things), one behavior in girls within the Parent Thought Problem category (learning problem), and one behavior in boys within the Autistic Behaviors category (injure self) in association with soy-based infant formula. Comparison of these exploratory autistic phenotypes in FXS, which are all worse in association with soy-based infant formula, with an earlier retrospective analysis in subjects with autism from the Simons Foundation Autism Research Initiative (SFARI) indicate overlap in the areas of language in females and self-injurious behavior in males. Specifically, sub-scale 5 of the Aberrant Behavior Checklist (ABC), Inappropriate Speech, is worse in SFARI females fed soy-based infant formula. There is a statistically significant increase in the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) Total Restricted, Repetitive and Stereotyped Behavior (RRSB) score in SFARI males. There is also a 1.3-fold increase in self-injurious behavior in SFARI males fed soy-based infant formula as evidenced by line-item question #83 of the ADI-R [13]. Overall, these data suggest that expressive language in females and more severe self-injurious behavior in males are associated with a soy-enriched diet early in life.
Sex-specific differences in autism outcomes in FXS are not unexpected. ASD is far more prevalent in males. Females with FXS demonstrate better receptive and expressive vocabulary than boys with FXS [25,26]. Males with comorbid FXS and autism exhibit worse communication, social, functional academic and daily living skills, increased behavior problems, and greater cognitive disability [27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35]. Self-injurious behavior is a common phenotype in FXS, affecting 31–79% of males but only 10–17% of females, with onset occurring within the first 3 years of age for 60% of subjects [36,37].
Expressive language sampling is under development as an outcome measure for FXS clinical trials [38,39]. Infants and toddlers with FXS exhibit deficits in early language skills compared to typically developing peers [40,41,42]. Children with FXS and comorbid autism are more impaired in nonverbal cognition and expressive language while receptive language is a relative strength for children with FXS (without autism) [43]. Furthermore, expressive language is associated with quantitative reasoning whereas receptive language is associated with adaptive behavior in girls with FXS [44]. Our findings suggest that expressive language in females can is associated with early-life diet.
The methylation silencing and mosaicism status of the FMR1 gene in the FXS participants in this study are not known. DNA methylation status of the CGG repeats in the FMR1 gene promoter at birth is predictive of later intellectual function and autism features [45]. The majority of males with FXS express some FMR1 mRNA and this incomplete silencing of the promoter is associated with increased autistic features [46,47,48,49], In males, full mutation FXS with complete or incomplete methylation silencing of the FMR1 gene is associated with increased inappropriate speech on the ABC-C sub-scale compared to the FXS group mosaic for pre- and full mutation alleles, and mosaicism in either sex is associated with less maladaptive behaviors [50,51]. These data suggest that comorbid autism phenotypes in FXS could be affected by gene–environmental interactions that regulate FMR1 methylation and/or expression.
At this time, soy bioactive component(s) that are associated with worse autism behaviors have not been identified. Potential candidates include endogenous phytoestrogens and contaminating agrochemicals. The soy phytoestrogen daidzein is associated with increased seizure activity in mice [11], and the widely used herbicide glyphosate and its metabolite aminomethylphosphonic acid (AMPA) are found in soy-based infant formulas [52]. Increased severity and frequency of disease symptoms were found in FXS premutation carriers in the agricultural community of Ricaurte, Colombia and hypothesized to be associated with higher exposure to neurotoxic pesticides [53]. It remains to be determined if a bioactive component of soy or an altered amino acid profile contributes to an association with more severe autistic behaviors.
The limitations of this study include the small study size, the data are retrospective and dependent on parental recall without clinical documentation (average age of participants was 18 years), this study was not powered to detect multiple hypotheses, there were no age-matched control data in typically developing children, and standardized autism diagnostic tests were not used. However, several standardized instruments were used to inform the survey questions, including cognitive tools such as the Ages and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ) for 36-months as well as autistic tools such as the Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers-Revised (M-CHAT-R) and the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) parental survey. Despite these limitations, several exploratory autistic behaviors were identified that are associated with the consumption of soy-based infant formula. Of particular interest is the association between soy-based infant formula and worse language scores in the female participants with FXS. A possible criticism of this work is that children could have been switched from breast milk to infant formula for various diagnosed or undiagnosed health issues that predisposed them to develop more severe autistic behaviors. A prospective study will be required to help address these confounding issues.
The strengths of the study design include the study is the first to link caregiver-reported data on infant feeding practices in children with FXS to specific developmental and autistic behaviors in those children; FORWARD offers a unique study population of subjects with established FXS; and the caregivers are a highly motivated group of parents eager to participate in research studies.
If early-life nutrition provides a practical intervention to reduce the severity of autism phenotypes in FXS, it would be imperative to implement newborn screening (NBS) protocols for the early identification of infants with FXS that could benefit from nutritional management. Currently, genetic testing for FXS is not included in the NBS panel because there are no proven medical treatments [54]. Thus, the majority of children with FXS are not identified until at least 3 years of age [55]. The human brain is highly plastic during the first two years of life, suggesting that earlier intervention would be more therapeutic. Voluntary NBS for FXS indicates that only 62% of parents agree to have their child screened, suggesting a need for stronger evidence of benefit before implementation of a nationwide screening program [56].

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, the investigation of dietary effects on FXS phenotypes is an understudied field with significant potential to provide a safe, easily implemented intervention to reduce GI problems, autism and other disease comorbidities in FXS. The retrospective survey findings reported herein suggest that the consumption of soy-based infant formula is associated with deficits in language, repetitive behavior and self-injurious behavior in FXS. In conjunction with the previous analyses of the questionnaire, the findings indicate that breastmilk is associated with improved outcomes in FXS compared to soy-based infant formula and that mothers should be encouraged to breastfeed. It remains to be determined if soy-based infant formula use is a cause or consequence of FXS comorbidities. Future research directions should include increasing the sample size and ethnic diversity of the Fragile X Syndrome Nutrition Study as well as prospective evaluation of comorbidities in participants with FXS as a function of diet. The findings have relevance to all ASD, which are estimated at 52 million cases, or 1–2% of children, worldwide [57]. Early gastrointestinal dysfunction is an endophenotype of ASD that is mitigated with breast milk [58]. It remains to be determined if soy-based infant formula exacerbates gastrointestinal problems in ASD.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/nu13082888/s1, Figure S1: Average behavior scores as a function of breast milk, Figure S2: Average language scores as a function of breast milk, Figure S3: Average cognition scores as a function of breast milk, Figure S4: Average child play a function of breast milk, Figure S5: Average motor skills as a function of breast milk, Figure S6: Average autism behavior scores as a function of breast milk, Figure S7: Average hypersensory scores as a function of breast milk, Figure S8: Average parent thought there was a problem scores as a function of breast milk, Figure S9: Average behavior scores as a function of breast milk for 12 months, Figure S10: Average behavior scores as a function of soy-based infant formula, Figure S11: Average language scores as a function of soy-based infant formula, Figure S12: Average cognition scores as a function of soy-based infant formula, Figure S13: Average autism behavior scores as a function of soy-based infant formula, Figure S14: Average child play scores as a function of soy-based infant formula, Figure S15: Average motor skills scores as a function of soy-based infant formula, Figure S16: Average hypersensory scores as a function of soy-based infant formula, Figure S17: Average parents thought there was a problem scores as a function of soy-based infant formula, Figure S18: Average behavior scores as a function of single milk, Table S1: Average behavior category statistics as a function of breast milk, Table S2: Average behavior category statistics as a function of breast milk 12 months, Table S3: Average behavior category statistics as a function of soy formula, Table S4: Average behavior category statistics as a function of single diet.

Funding

This research was funded by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD), grant number HD097067, and the United States Department of Agriculture (USDA), grant number 2018-67001-28266. FORWARD was supported by cooperative agreements U01DD000231, U19DD000753 and U01DD001189, funded by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. This article’s contents are solely the responsibility of the author and do not necessarily represent the official views of the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention or the Department of Health and Human Services.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and determined to meet the criteria for exempt Human Subjects research in accordance with Category 2 defined under 45 CFR-46 by the Health Sciences Institutional Review Board of the University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI, USA.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in this study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are contained within the article, Supplementary Material or cited work, or available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

The author thanks FORWARD, the FORWARD-associated clinics and the University of Wisconsin Survey Center (UWSC) for assistance with patient recruitment and survey administration for the Fragile X Syndrome Nutrition Study [14], in particular Elizabeth Berry-Kravis and Chad Kniss. The author thanks the families who participated in this study.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, DSM-5, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  2. Maenner, M.J.; Shaw, K.A.; Baio, J.; Washington, A.; Patrick, M.; DiRienzo, M.; Christensen, D.L.; Wiggins, L.D.; Pettygrove, S.; Andrews, J.G.; et al. Prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorder among Children Aged 8 Years—Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 Sites, United States, 2016. MMWR Surveill. Summ. 2020, 69, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Chiarotti, F.; Venerosi, A. Epidemiology of Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Review of Worldwide Prevalence Estimates Since 2014. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Newschaffer, C.J.; Croen, L.A.; Daniels, J.; Giarelli, E.; Grether, J.K.; Levy, S.E.; Mandell, D.S.; Miller, L.A.; Pinto-Martin, J.; Reaven, J.; et al. The Epidemiology of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Annu. Rev. Public Health 2007, 28, 235–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Hessl, D.; Dyer-Friedman, J.; Glaser, B.; Wisbeck, J.; Barajas, R.G.; Taylor, A.; Reiss, A.L. The Influence of Environmental and Genetic Factors on Behavior Problems and Autistic Symptoms in Boys and Girls with Fragile X Syndrome. Pediatrics 2001, 108, E88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Spencer, C.M.; Alekseyenko, O.; Hamilton, S.M.; Thomas, A.M.; Serysheva, E.; Yuva-Paylor, L.A.; Paylor, R. Modifying Behavioral Phenotypes in Fmr1KO Mice: Genetic Background Differences Reveal Autistic-Like Responses. Autism Res. 2011, 4, 40–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Masini, E.; Loi, E.; Vega-Benedetti, A.F.; Carta, M.; Doneddu, G.; Fadda, R.; Zavattari, P. An Overview of the Main Genetic, Epigenetic and Environmental Factors Involved in Autism Spectrum Disorder Focusing on Synaptic Activity. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2020, 21, 8290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. McCary, L.M.; Roberts, J.E. Early Identification of Autism in Fragile X Syndrome: A Review. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2013, 57, 803–814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Clifford, S.; Dissanayake, C.; Bui, Q.M.; Huggins, R.; Taylor, A.K.; Loesch, D.Z. Autism Spectrum Phenotype in Males and Females with Fragile X Full Mutation and Premutation. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2007, 37, 738–747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Kaufmann, W.E.; Kidd, S.A.; Andrews, H.F.; Budimirovic, D.B.; Esler, A.; Haas-Givler, B.; Stackhouse, T.; Riley, C.; Peacock, G.; Sherman, S.L.; et al. Autism Spectrum Disorder in Fragile X Syndrome: Cooccurring Conditions and Current Treatment. Pediatrics 2017, 139, S194–S206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Westmark, C.J.; Westmark, P.R.; Malter, J.S. Soy-Based Diet Exacerbates Seizures in Mouse Models of Neurological Disease. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2013, 33, 797–805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Westmark, C.J. Soy Infant Formula and Seizures in Children with Autism: A Retrospective Study. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e80488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Westmark, C.J. Soy Infant Formula may be Associated with Increased Autistic Behaviors. Autism-Open Access 2013, 3, 20727. [Google Scholar]
  14. Westmark, C.J.; Kniss, C.; Sampene, E.; Wang, A.; Milunovich, A.; Elver, K.; Hessl, D.; Talboy, A.; Picker, J.; Haas-Givler, B.; et al. Soy-Based Infant Formula is Associated with an Increased Prevalence of Comorbidities in Fragile X Syndrome. Nutrients 2020, 12, 3136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Westmark, C.J. Consumption of Breast Milk is Associated with Decreased Prevalence of Autism in Fragile X Syndrome. Nutrients 2021, 13, 1785. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Westmark, C.J. A Hypothesis regarding the Molecular Mechanism Underlying Dietary Soy-Induced Effects on Seizure Propensity. Front. Neurol. 2014, 5, 169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Westmark, C.J. Soy-Based Therapeutic Baby Formulas: Testable Hypotheses regarding the Pros and Cons. Front. Nutr. 2017, 3, 59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Sherman, S.L.; Kidd, S.A.; Riley, C.; Berry-Kravis, E.; Andrews, H.F.; Miller, R.M.; Lincoln, S.; Swanson, M.; Kaufmann, W.E.; Brown, W.T. FORWARD: A Registry and Longitudinal Clinical Database to Study Fragile X Syndrome. Pediatrics 2017, 139, S183–S193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  19. Nolan, S.O.; Hodges, S.L.; Okoh, J.T.; Binder, M.S.; Lugo, J.N. Prenatal High-Fat Diet Rescues Communication Deficits in Fmr1 Mutant Mice in a Sex-Specific Manner. Dev. Neurosci. 2020, 42, 94–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Schiavi, S.; Carbone, E.; Melancia, F.; Buzzelli, V.; Manduca, A.; Campolongo, P.; Pallottini, V.; Trezza, V. Perinatal Supplementation with Omega-3 Fatty Acids Corrects the Aberrant Social and Cognitive Traits Observed in a Genetic Model of Autism Based on FMR1 Deletion in Rats. Nutr. Neurosci. 2020, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Westmark, P.R.; Gutierrez, A.; Gholston, A.K.; Wilmer, T.M.; Westmark, C.J. Preclinical Testing of the Ketogenic Diet in Fragile X Mice. Neurochem. Int. 2020, 134, 104687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Pietropaolo, S.; Goubran, M.G.; Joffre, C.; Aubert, A.; Lemaire-Mayo, V.; Crusio, W.E.; Laye, S. Dietary Supplementation of Omega-3 Fatty Acids Rescues Fragile X Phenotypes in Fmr1-Ko Mice. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2014, 49, 119–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Schaevitz, L.; Berger-Sweeney, J.; Ricceri, L. One-Carbon Metabolism in Neurodevelopmental Disorders: Using Broad-Based Nutraceutics to Treat Cognitive Deficits in Complex Spectrum Disorders. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2014, 46 Pt 2, 270–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Barnhill, K.; Devlin, M.; Moreno, H.T.; Potts, A.; Richardson, W.; Schutte, C.; Hewitson, L. Brief Report: Implementation of a Specific Carbohydrate Diet for a Child with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Fragile X Syndrome. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Martin, G.E.; Bush, L.; Klusek, J.; Patel, S.; Losh, M. A Multimethod Analysis of Pragmatic Skills in Children and Adolescents with Fragile X Syndrome, Autism Spectrum Disorder, and Down Syndrome. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2018, 61, 3023–3037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Komesidou, R.; Brady, N.C.; Fleming, K.; Esplund, A.; Warren, S.F. Growth of Expressive Syntax in Children with Fragile X Syndrome. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2017, 60, 422–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Raspa, M.; Franco, V.; Bishop, E.; Wheeler, A.C.; Wylie, A.; Bailey, D.B., Jr. A Comparison of Functional Academic and Daily Living Skills in Males with Fragile X Syndrome with and without Autism. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2018, 78, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Bailey, D.B., Jr.; Hatton, D.D.; Mesibov, G.; Ament, N.; Skinner, M. Early Development, Temperament, and Functional Impairment in Autism and Fragile X Syndrome. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2000, 30, 49–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Kaufmann, W.E.; Cortell, R.; Kau, A.S.; Bukelis, I.; Tierney, E.; Gray, R.M.; Cox, C.; Capone, G.T.; Stanard, P. Autism Spectrum Disorder in Fragile X Syndrome: Communication, Social Interaction, and Specific Behaviors. Am. J. Med. Genet. A 2004, 129A, 225–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Reisinger, D.L.; Roberts, J.E. Differential Relationships of Anxiety and Autism Symptoms on Social Skills in Young Boys with Fragile X Syndrome. Am. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2017, 122, 359–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Lewis, P.; Abbeduto, L.; Murphy, M.; Richmond, E.; Giles, N.; Bruno, L.; Schroeder, S. Cognitive, Language and Social-Cognitive Skills of Individuals with Fragile X Syndrome with and without Autism. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2006, 50, 532–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Smith, L.E.; Barker, E.T.; Seltzer, M.M.; Abbeduto, L.; Greenberg, J.S. Behavioral Phenotype of Fragile X Syndrome in Adolescence and Adulthood. Am. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2012, 117, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Budimirovic, D.B.; Bukelis, I.; Cox, C.; Gray, R.M.; Tierney, E.; Kaufmann, W.E. Autism Spectrum Disorder in Fragile X Syndrome: Differential Contribution of Adaptive Socialization and Social Withdrawal. Am. J. Med. Genet. A 2006, 140A, 1814–1826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Kau, A.S.; Tierney, E.; Bukelis, I.; Stump, M.H.; Kates, W.R.; Trescher, W.H.; Kaufmann, W.E. Social Behavior Profile in Young Males with Fragile X Syndrome: Characteristics and Specificity. Am. J. Med. Genet. A 2004, 126A, 9–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Brock, M.; Hatton, D. Distinguishing Features of Autism in Boys with Fragile X Syndrome. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2010, 54, 894–905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Hardiman, R.L.; McGill, P. How Common are Challenging Behaviours Amongst Individuals with Fragile X Syndrome? A Systematic Review. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2018, 76, 99–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Symons, F.J.; Byiers, B.J.; Raspa, M.; Bishop, E.; Bailey, D.B. Self-Injurious Behavior and Fragile X Syndrome: Findings from the National Fragile X Survey. Am. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2010, 115, 473–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Abbeduto, L.; Berry-Kravis, E.; Sterling, A.; Sherman, S.; Edgin, J.O.; McDuffie, A.; Hoffmann, A.; Hamilton, D.; Nelson, M.; Aschkenasy, J.; et al. Expressive Language Sampling as a Source of Outcome Measures for Treatment Studies in Fragile X Syndrome: Feasibility, Practice Effects, Test-Retest Reliability, and Construct Validity. J. Neurodev Disord. 2020, 12, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  39. Shaffer, R.C.; Schmitt, L.; John Thurman, A.; Abbeduto, L.; Hong, M.; Pedapati, E.; Dominick, K.; Sweeney, J.; Erickson, C. The Relationship between Expressive Language Sampling and Clinical Measures in Fragile X Syndrome and Typical Development. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Zhang, D.; Roche, L.; Bartl-Pokorny, K.D.; Krieber, M.; McLay, L.; Bolte, S.; Poustka, L.; Sigafoos, J.; Gugatschka, M.; Einspieler, C.; et al. Response to Name and its Value for the Early Detection of Developmental Disorders: Insights from Autism Spectrum Disorder, Rett Syndrome, and Fragile X Syndrome. A Perspectives Paper. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2018, 82, 95–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Roche, L.; Zhang, D.; Bartl-Pokorny, K.D.; Pokorny, F.B.; Schuller, B.W.; Esposito, G.; Bolte, S.; Roeyers, H.; Poustka, L.; Gugatschka, M.; et al. Early Vocal Development in Autism Spectrum Disorder, Rett Syndrome, and Fragile X Syndrome: Insights from Studies using Retrospective Video Analysis. Adv. Neurodev Disord. 2018, 2, 49–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Reisinger, D.L.; Shaffer, R.C.; Pedapati, E.V.; Dominick, K.C.; Erickson, C.A. A Pilot Quantitative Evaluation of Early Life Language Development in Fragile X Syndrome. Brain Sci. 2019, 9, 27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Philofsky, A.; Hepburn, S.L.; Hayes, A.; Hagerman, R.; Rogers, S.J. Linguistic and Cognitive Functioning and Autism Symptoms in Young Children with Fragile X Syndrome. Am. J. Ment. Retard. 2004, 109, 208–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Joga-Elvira, L.; Jacas, C.; Joga, M.L.; Roche-Martinez, A.; Brun-Gasca, C. Fragile X Syndrome in Young Females: Influence of Executive Function on the Neurocognitive Profile and Adaptive Behavior. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2021, 112, 103912. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Kraan, C.M.; Baker, E.K.; Arpone, M.; Bui, M.; Ling, L.; Gamage, D.; Bretherton, L.; Rogers, C.; Field, M.J.; Wotton, T.L.; et al. DNA Methylation at Birth Predicts Intellectual Functioning and Autism Features in Children with Fragile X Syndrome. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2020, 21, 7735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Tassone, F.; Hagerman, R.J.; Chamberlain, W.D.; Hagerman, P.J. Transcription of the FMR1 Gene in Individuals with Fragile X Syndrome. Am. J. Med. Genet. 2000, 97, 195–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Tassone, F.; Hagerman, R.J.; Taylor, A.K.; Hagerman, P.J. A Majority of Fragile X Males with Methylated, Full Mutation Alleles have Significant Levels of FMR1 Messenger RNA. J. Med. Genet. 2001, 38, 453–456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Stoger, R.; Genereux, D.P.; Hagerman, R.J.; Hagerman, P.J.; Tassone, F.; Laird, C.D. Testing the FMR1 Promoter for Mosaicism in DNA Methylation among CpG Sites, Strands, and Cells in FMR1-Expressing Males with Fragile X Syndrome. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e23648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Baker, E.K.; Arpone, M.; Aliaga, S.M.; Bretherton, L.; Kraan, C.M.; Bui, M.; Slater, H.R.; Ling, L.; Francis, D.; Hunter, M.F.; et al. Incomplete Silencing of Full Mutation Alleles in Males with Fragile X Syndrome is Associated with Autistic Features. Mol. Autism 2019, 10, 21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  50. Baker, E.K.; Arpone, M.; Kraan, C.; Bui, M.; Rogers, C.; Field, M.; Bretherton, L.; Ling, L.; Ure, A.; Cohen, J.; et al. FMR1 mRNA from Full Mutation Alleles is Associated with ABC-CFX Scores in Males with Fragile X Syndrome. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 11701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Baker, E.K.; Arpone, M.; Vera, S.A.; Bretherton, L.; Ure, A.; Kraan, C.M.; Bui, M.; Ling, L.; Francis, D.; Hunter, M.F.; et al. Intellectual Functioning and Behavioural Features Associated with Mosaicism in Fragile X Syndrome. J. Neurodev Disord. 2019, 11, 41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Rodrigues, N.R.; de Souza, A.P.F. Occurrence of Glyphosate and AMPA Residues in Soy-Based Infant Formulas Sold in Brazil. Food Addit. Contam. Part A 2018, 35, 723–730. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Saldarriaga, W.; Salcedo-Arellano, M.J.; Rodriguez-Guerrero, T.; Rios, M.; Fandino-Losada, A.; Ramirez-Cheyne, J.; Lein, P.J.; Tassone, F.; Hagerman, R.J. Increased Severity of Fragile X Spectrum Disorders in the Agricultural Community of Ricaurte, Colombia. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 2019, 72, 1–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Okoniewski, K.C.; Wheeler, A.C.; Lee, S.; Boyea, B.; Raspa, M.; Taylor, J.L.; Bailey, D.B., Jr. Early Identification of Fragile X Syndrome through Expanded Newborn Screening. Brain Sci. 2019, 9, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Bailey, D.B., Jr.; Raspa, M.; Bishop, E.; Holiday, D. No Change in the Age of Diagnosis for Fragile x Syndrome: Findings from a National Parent Survey. Pediatrics 2009, 124, 527–533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Bailey, D.B., Jr.; Berry-Kravis, E.; Gane, L.W.; Guarda, S.; Hagerman, R.; Powell, C.M.; Tassone, F.; Wheeler, A. Fragile X Newborn Screening: Lessons Learned from a Multisite Screening Study. Pediatrics 2017, 139, S216–S225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Hahler, E.-M.; Elsabbagh, M. Autism: A Global Perspective. Curr. Dev. Disord. Rep. 2015, 2, 58–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Penn, A.H.; Carver, L.J.; Herbert, C.A.; Lai, T.S.; McIntire, M.J.; Howard, J.T.; Taylor, S.F.; Schmid-Schonbein, G.W.; Dobkins, K.R. Breast Milk Protects Against Gastrointestinal Symptoms in Infants at High Risk for Autism During Early Development. J. Pediatr. Gastroenterol. Nutr. 2016, 62, 317–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Table 1. Demographics.
Table 1. Demographics.
Race/EthnicityFemale (N = 50)Male (N = 140)
American Indian or Alaskan Native (%)00.71
Asian (%)21.4
Black or African American (%)05
Hispanic or Latino (%)85
Native Hawaiian or Other Pacific Islander (%)00
White (%)9294
Other Race or Ethnicity (%)20
Average Age at Time of Survey (Years, SEM)18 (1.45)18 (0.87)
Table 2. Assessed Behaviors.
Table 2. Assessed Behaviors.
Question: How Often Did Your Child…NClass *
Language
Q40a…talk?187T
Q40b…say his or her name when asked?189T
Q40c…respond when spoken to, for example, did your child look at you when you called his or her name?190T
Q40d…speak in sentences of at least 3 words?190T
Q40e…use words to request things, for example when he or she wanted a cookie?189T
Cognition
Q41a…correctly identify people or objects when you pointed to them and asked what they were, for example, “mommy”, “daddy”, “dog”, or “airplane”?187T
Q41b…follow simple directions, for example “sit down” or “get your shoes”?190T
Q41c…point at things to request them, for example, a toy on a shelf?190T
Q41d…copy others, for example, clapping their hands or waving?189T
Q41e…play pretend, for example, rocking a doll to sleep or feeding a stuffed animal?189T
Q41f…have savant ability, a restricted skill superior to their age group, for example reading early, or memorizing books?190T
Child Play
Q43a…like motion activities, for example, to be swung or bounced?189T
Q45e…like to play with other children?190T
Motor Skills
Q43b…walk?188T
Q43d…pick up small objects, for example, Cheerios?189T
Q43e…feed him or herself with a spoon?189T
Q43f…help dress him or herself, for example pull up their pants?190T
Autistic Behaviors
Q44b…do rocking, hand flapping or spinning over and over again?190A
Q44f…try to injure him or herself, for example, head banging?190A
Q43c…toe walk?187A
Hypersensory
Q44a…get upset by loud noises, for example, the vacuum cleaner or microwave?189A
Q44c…cry excessively over small hurts?190A
Q44d…have temper outbursts if he or she did not get their way?190A
Q44e…isolate him or herself?189A
Q45a…get upset by minor changes to their daily routine?190A
Q45b…have difficulty expressing his or her needs and desires?190A
Q45c…hate crowds, for example, difficulties in restaurants or the grocery store?190A
Q45d…not like to be touched or held?190A
Parent Thought Problem
Q46a…had an anxiety problem?190A
Q46b…had a hearing problem?190A
Q46c…had a vision problem?190A
Q46d…had a learning problem?190A
* Behaviors from Q40, Q41 and Q43–Q46 of the Fragile X Syndrome Nutrition Study questionnaire were classified as typical (T) or autistic (A). N is the number of respondents who answered the question by ranking their child’s behavior on a 5-point Likert scale (never, rarely, sometimes, very often, and extremely often).
Table 3. Caregiver-reported autism phenotypes as a function of breast milk.
Table 3. Caregiver-reported autism phenotypes as a function of breast milk.
CategoryFemale
No BM *
Female
Plus BM
Male
No BM
Male
Plus BM
p#
Language (average, SEM, N)
Q40a (talk)3.00, 0.26, 173.28, 0.25, 322.23, 0.18, 352.58, 0.11, 100d,e
Q40b (say name)2.82, 0.30, 173.12, 0.27, 332.00, 0.18, 352.27, 0.12, 101d,e
Q40c (respond)3.82, 0.26, 174.18, 0.13, 333.31, 0.17, 353.68, 0.10, 102d
Q40d (3 word sentences)2.65, 0.34, 172.73, 0.27, 331.74, 0.17, 351.95, 0.12, 102d,e
Q40e (words to request things)2.69, 0.33, 163.09, 0.26, 332.17, 0.19, 352.44, 0.12, 102d
Language Sub-Score3.00, 0.23, 173.27, 0.22, 332.29, 0.14, 352.58, 0.09, 102d,e
Cognition (average, SEM, N)
Q41a (correctly identify)3.38, 0.29, 163.47, 0.25, 322.77, 0.20, 352.98, 0.12, 101-
Q41b (follow directions)3.29, 0.25, 173.52, 0.19, 332.71, 0.15, 352.90, 0.11, 102d,e
Q41c (point to request)3.82, 0.23, 173.52, 0.21, 333.14, 0.18, 353.08, 0.12, 102-
Q41d (copy others)3.65, 0.21, 173.42, 0.20, 332.77, 0.19, 353.16, 0.11, 101b
Q41e (play pretend)2.88, 0.31, 172.63, 0.25, 321.71, 0.19, 351.96, 0.12, 102b,c,d,e
Q41f (have savant ability)1.71, 0.33, 171.27, 0.13, 331.20, 0.09, 351.30, 0.08, 102-
Cognitive Sub-Score3.11, 0.19, 172.97, 0.15, 332.39, 0.12, 352.56, 0.08, 102b,c,d
Child Play (average, SEM, N)
Q43a (likes motion)4.29, 0.19, 174.00, 0.19, 333.97, 0.16, 344.12, 0.10, 102-
Q45e (like to play with other kids)3.00, 0.28, 173.15, 0.17, 332.40, 0.17, 352.88, 0.12, 102d
Child Play Sub-Score3.65, 0.018, 173.58, 0.13, 333.14, 0.12, 353.50, 0.08, 102-
Motor Skills (average, SEM, N)
Q43b (walk)4.13, 0.22, 164.39, 0.14, 333.85, 0.19, 344.21, 0.08, 102-
Q43d (picks up small objects)4.00, 0.27, 174.09, 0.19, 323.51, 0.18, 353.54, 0.10, 102e
Q43e (feeds self with spoon)3.53, 0.29, 173.55, 0.23, 332.69, 0.19, 353.06, 0.13, 101d
Q43f (help dress self)3.24, 0.34, 173.12, 0.23, 332.26, 0.18, 352.61, 0.12, 102b,d
Motor Skills Sub-Score3.72, 0.23, 173.79, 0.16, 333.07, 0.15, 353.35, 0.08, 102d
Autistic Behaviors (average, SEM, N)
Q44b (rocking, hand flapping)3.06, 0.39, 172.36, 0.26, 333.71, 0.24, 353.74, 0.13, 102d,e
Q44f (injure self)1.53, 0.21, 171.48, 0.17, 331.77, 0.21, 352.06, 0.14, 102-
Q43c (toe walk)2.41, 0.42, 172.30, 0.24, 332.38, 0.23, 322.05, 0.12, 102-
Autistic Behavior Sub-Score2.33, 0.23, 172.05, 0.17, 332.67, 0.17, 352.61, 0.09, 102d,e
Hypersensory (average, SEM, N)
Q44a (upset by loud noises)3.71, 0.28, 173.00, 0.22, 333.21, 0.21, 343.17, 0.13, 102-
Q44c (cry excessively)2.88, 0.32, 172.61, 0.23, 332.46, 0.19, 352.26, 0.11, 102-
Q44d (temper outbursts)3.12, 0.30, 173.06, 0.21, 332.86, 0.19, 352.97, 0.12, 102-
Q44e (isolate self)2.94, 0.35, 172.09, 0.16, 332.51, 0.19, 352.56, 0.12, 101-
Q45a (upset by minor changes)3.41, 0.26, 173.09, 0.18, 333.34, 0.17, 353.31, 0.11, 102-
Q45b (difficulty expressing needs) 3.82, 0.20, 173.70, 0.20, 334.23, 0.14, 354.03, 0.09, 102-
Q45c (hate crowds)3.35, 0.28, 173.12, 0.23, 333.57, 0.17, 353.47, 0.13, 102-
Q45d (not liked to be touched)2.47, 0.26, 172.12, 0.18, 332.54, 0.19, 352.39, 0.12, 102-
Hypersensory Sub-Score3.21, 0.18, 172.85, 0.11, 333.09, 0.11, 353.02, 0.07, 102-
Parent Thought Problem (average, SEM, N)
Q46a (anxiety problem)3.53, 0.31, 173.24, 0.24, 333.51, 0.24, 353.41, 0.13, 102-
Q46b (hearing problem)1.76, 0.29, 171.55, 0.17, 331.74, 0.19, 351.83, 0.12, 102-
Q46c (vision problem)1.47, 0.27, 171.76, 0.21, 331.71, 0.20, 351.69, 0.12, 102-
Q46d (learning problem)4.18, 0.30, 174.03, 0.22, 334.60, 0.14, 354.47, 0.08, 102-
Parent Thought Problem Sub-Score3.10, 0.14, 172.92, 0.07, 332.95, 0.07, 353.01, 0.04, 102-
Total Behavior Score (average, SEM, N)9.53, 4.39, 1516.57, 3.49, 30−0.31, 2.64, 264.84, 1.95, 92d,e
* No BM = no breast milk, Plus BM = fed breast milk. # Statistically significant results by Tukey’s multiple comparison test are denoted: (a) female no BM versus female plus BM, (b) female no BM versus male no BM, (c) female no BM versus male plus BM, (d) female plus BM versus male no BM, (e) female plus BM versus male plus BM, and (f) male no BM versus male plus BM.
Table 4. Caregiver-reported autism phenotypes as a function of soy-based infant formula.
Table 4. Caregiver-reported autism phenotypes as a function of soy-based infant formula.
CategoryFemale
No Soy *
Female
Plus Soy
Male
No Soy
Male
Plus Soy
p#
Language (average, SEM, N)
Q40a (talk)3.44, 0.20, 362.22, 0.22, 92.51, 0.11, 912.35, 0.23, 31a,b,c
Q40b (say name)3.22, 0.24, 362.30, 0.37, 102.22, 0.12, 912.03, 0.22, 32b,c
Q40c (respond)4.06, 0.13, 363.90, 0.38, 103.58, 0.11, 923.53, 0.20. 32-
Q40d (3 word sentences)2.94, 0.25, 361.70, 0.21, 101.89, 0.11, 921.75, 0.22, 32a,b,c
Q40e (words to request things)3.23, 0.23, 352.00, 0.21, 102.34, 0.11, 922.25, 0.23, 32a,b,c
Language Sub-Score3.38, 0.18, 362.42, 0.17, 102.50, 0.09, 922.38, 0.18, 32a,b,c
Cognition (average, SEM, N)
Q41a (correctly identify)3.57, 0.22, 352.90, 0.38, 102.91, 0.12, 912.81, 0.26, 32b
Q41b (follow directions)3.31, 0.18, 363.40, 0.27, 102.87. 0.11, 922.72, 0.19, 32-
Q41c (point to request)3.56, 0.17, 363.70, 0.42, 103.03, 0.12, 922.97, 0.24, 32-
Q41d (copy others)3.47, 0.16, 363.30, 0.42, 103.00, 0.12, 913.09, 0.21, 32-
Q41e (play pretend)2.74, 0.23, 352.40, 0.43, 101.79, 0.11, 921.91, 0.24, 32b,c
Q41f (have savant ability)1.39, 0.18, 361.50, 0.27, 101.23, 0.07, 921.41, 0.17, 32-
Cognitive Sub-Score3.00, 0.14, 362.87, 0.24, 102.47, 0.08, 922.48, 0.17, 32b,c
Child Play (average, SEM, N)
Q43a (likes motion)4.11, 0.16, 364.40, 0.22, 103.98, 0.11, 924.29, 0.16, 31-
Q45e (like to play with other kids)3.19, 0.18, 362.90, 0.28, 102.72, 0.13, 922.78, 0.19, 32-
Child Play Sub-Score3.65, 0.13, 363.65, 0.15, 103.35, 0.09, 923.48, 0.14, 32-
Motor Skills (average, SEM, N)
Q43b (walk)4.31, 0.14, 364.22, 0.28, 94.14, 0.09, 924.10, 0.16, 31-
Q43d (picks up small objects)3.94, 0.20, 354.20, 0.25, 103.52, 0.10, 923.53, 0.18, 32-
Q43e (feeds self with spoon)3.61, 0.20, 363.30, 0.42, 103.01, 0.13, 912.78, 0.22, 32c
Q43f (help dress self)3.25, 0.22, 363.10, 0.41, 102.54, 0.11, 922.28, 0.25, 32b,c
Motor Skills Sub-Score3.78, 0.16, 363.69, 0.21, 103.30, 0.08, 923.16, 0.16, 32b,c
Autistic Behaviors (average, SEM, N)
Q44b (rocking, hand flapping)2.28, 0.24, 363.50, 0.48, 103.55, 0.14, 924.16, 0.23, 32b,c
Q44f (injure self)1.39, 0.13, 361.90, 0.43, 101.77, 0.13, 922.47, 0.28, 32c,f
Q43c (toe walk)2.36, 0.25, 362.20, 0.47, 102.12, 0.13, 912.42, 0.27, 31-
Autistic Behavior Sub-Score2.01, 0.15, 362.53, 0.39, 102.48, 0.08, 923.04, 0.17, 32b,c,f
Hypersensory (average, SEM, N)
Q44a (upset by loud noises)3.22, 0.19, 363.60, 0.48, 102.96, 0.13, 913.41, 0.25, 32-
Q44c (cry excessively)2.78, 0.20, 362.30, 0.50, 102.27, 0.12, 922.34, 0.23, 32-
Q44d (temper outbursts)3.06, 0.20, 363.30, 0.45, 102.84, 0.12, 923.25, 0.22, 32-
Q44e (isolate self)2.58, 0.20, 362.20, 0.36, 102.47, 0.12, 922.81, 0.20, 32-
Q45a (upset by minor changes)3.17, 0.18, 363.40, 0.31, 103.28, 0.11, 923.41, 0.23, 32-
Q45b (difficulty expressing needs) 3.61, 0.19, 364.10, 0.18, 103.99, 0.09, 924.38, 0.15, 32c
Q45c (hate crowds)3.17, 0.22, 363.50, 0.31, 103.32, 0.13, 923.72, 0.21, 32-
Q45d (not liked to be touched)2.00, 0.15, 362.70, 0.40, 102.32, 0.12, 922.47, 0.23, 32-
Hypersensory Sub-Score2.95, 0.12, 363.14, 0.22, 102.92, 0.07, 923.22, 0.14, 32-
Parent Thought Problem (average, SEM, N)
Q46a (anxiety problem)3.25, 0.22, 363.60, 0.40, 103.38, 0.14, 923.34, 0.26, 32-
Q46b (hearing problem)1.61, 0.19, 361.70, 0.30, 101.75, 0.12, 921.94, 0.23, 32-
Q46c (vision problem)1.67, 0.20, 361.50, 0.40, 101.48, 0.11, 922.06, 0.24, 32-
Q46d (learning problem)3.86, 0.23, 364.80, 0.13, 104.51, 0.08, 924.44, 0.18, 32a,b
Parent Thought Problem Sub-Score2.94, 0.08, 363.12, 0.11, 102.92, 0.04, 923.12, 0.08, 32-
Total Behavior Score (average, SEM, N)16.19, 3.14, 325.78, 4.99, 94.98. 1.83, 83 0.30, 4.11, 27b,c
* No Soy = no soy-based infant formula, Plus Soy = fed soy-based infant formula. # Statistically significant results by Tukey’s multiple comparison test are denoted: (a) female no soy versus female plus soy, (b) female no soy versus male no soy, (c) female no soy versus male plus soy, (d) female plus soy versus male no soy, (e) female plus soy versus male plus soy, and (f) male no soy versus male plus soy.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Westmark, C.J. Parental Reports on Early Autism Behaviors in Their Children with Fragile X Syndrome as a Function of Infant Feeding. Nutrients 2021, 13, 2888. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13082888

AMA Style

Westmark CJ. Parental Reports on Early Autism Behaviors in Their Children with Fragile X Syndrome as a Function of Infant Feeding. Nutrients. 2021; 13(8):2888. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13082888

Chicago/Turabian Style

Westmark, Cara J. 2021. "Parental Reports on Early Autism Behaviors in Their Children with Fragile X Syndrome as a Function of Infant Feeding" Nutrients 13, no. 8: 2888. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13082888

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop