Next Article in Journal
Relative Validity and Reproducibility of a Short Food Frequency Questionnaire to Assess Nutrient Intakes of New Zealand Adults
Next Article in Special Issue
Correlation between Macular Pigment Optical Density and Neural Thickness and Volume of the Retina
Previous Article in Journal
Survival of Recombinant Monoclonal Antibodies (IgG, IgA and sIgA) Versus Naturally-Occurring Antibodies (IgG and sIgA/IgA) in an Ex Vivo Infant Digestion Model
Previous Article in Special Issue
Carotenoids and Periodontal Infection
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Effects of Lutein and Astaxanthin Intake on the Improvement of Cognitive Functions among Healthy Adults: A Systematic Review of Randomized Controlled Trials

1
Department of Cognitive Health Science, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer (IDAC), Tohoku University, Sendai 980-8575, Japan
2
Smart Aging Research Center (S.A.R.C.), Tohoku University, Seiryo-machi 4-1, Sendai 980-8575, Japan
3
Research and Development Headquarters, Lion Corporation, Edogawa, Tokyo 132-0035, Japan
4
Department of Functional Brain Imaging, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer (IDAC), Tohoku University, Sendai 980-8575, Japan
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2020, 12(3), 617; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12030617
Submission received: 28 January 2020 / Revised: 22 February 2020 / Accepted: 24 February 2020 / Published: 27 February 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Carotenoids in Human Nutrition)

Abstract

:
Background: Fruits and vegetables are generally rich in antioxidants such as carotenoids. Consumption of carotenoids is expected to have benefits on cognitive functions in humans. However, previous randomized controlled trials (RCT) using carotenoids have reported inconsistent results. Therefore, this systematic review (SR) aimed to summarize the effect of carotenoid intake on cognitive functions in humans. Method: PubMed, Cochrane Library, Web of Science, and PsychoINFO were searched for research papers on carotenoid intake with the criteria that 1) oral carotenoid intake was evaluated using RCTs, 2) participants were healthy young, middle-aged, or older, and 3) cognitive functions were measured using RCTs. Results: Five studies using lutein and two studies using astaxanthin met the inclusion criteria. Consumption of lutein and its isomer showed consistent results in selective improvement of visual episodic memory in young and middle-aged adults while inhibition was observed in middle-aged and older adults. One of the two included astaxanthin studies reported a significant improvement of verbal episodic memory performance in middle-aged adults. Conclusion: This SR showed that the 10 mg lutein per day for twelve months can lead to improvement of cognitive functions. Due to the small number of studies, it is difficult to conclude whether astaxanthin would have a positive effect on cognitive functions.

1. Introduction

Cognitive functions are mental processes that include memory, processing speed, executive function, and attention. Cognitive functions change with age. The peak of cognitive function is around twenty or thirty years of age, and then cognitive functions decline after fifty or sixty years of age [1]. Cognitive functions are affected by dietary habits and nutrition consumption [2]. For example, eating breakfast affects cognitive functions in children, young adults, and older adults [3,4]. Mediterranean diets have positive effects on cognitive functions [5]. Many cross-sectional and longitudinal cohort studies have revealed that consumption of fruits and vegetables is positively associated with cognitive functions [6,7]. Fruits and vegetables are generally rich in antioxidants such as flavonoids and carotenoids, which are associated with higher cognitive functions [8] and a lower risk of dementia [9] when included in diets. Nutritional intervention studies have demonstrated that intake of flavonoids and carotenoids improve cognitive functions [10,11]. Although at least two systematic reviews (SR) and meta-analyses have concluded that flavonoids benefit cognitive health [12,13], no SR has investigated the benefits of carotenoids on cognition, therefore necessitating this SR.
Carotenoids are a widely distributed group of naturally occurring pigments, usually red, orange or yellow in color, of which over 750 compounds have been identified [14]. Two chemical classes of carotenoids have been established; the hydrocarbon-based carotenes and the xanthophylls, which also contain oxygen apart from the hydrocarbon component. Generally, carotenoids are one of the derivatives of tetraterpenoids, having a basic skeleton containing C40H56 and produced from eight isoprenoid units. Because of their characteristically long polyene structure comprising 9 to 11 conjugated double bonds, carotenoids demonstrate strong antioxidant effects on singlet oxygen, which is one of the reactive oxygen species (ROS) [15], as well as protective effects from optical damage through absorption of blue to green light energy [16]. Humans cannot synthesize carotenoids but need them for various functions [17], therefore human levels of carotenoids mainly come from green leafy vegetables.
Meta-analyses and SRs have suggested an association between carotenoids and a reduced risk of several chronic health disorders including some forms of cancer, heart disease, and eye degeneration [18]. In addition, narrative or hand reviews have indicated that the intake of carotenoids affects cognitive functions [19]. Notably, recent intervention studies using randomized controlled trials (RCTs) have demonstrated a positive effect of the intake of carotenoids on cognitive functions [20,21]. However, to the best of our knowledge, there is no SR of RCTs to assess the benefits of carotenoids on cognitive function. Therefore, we aimed to review the scientific evidence related to the beneficial effects of carotenoid on cognition.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. The SR Protocol and Registration

The SR protocol was designed using the International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO) with the registration number CRD42018110984 (https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?RecordID=110984). The protocol followed the statement and general principles of Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA)[22] (Supplementary Table S1).

2.2. Search Strategy

Our review question was “Do xanthophyll and carotene carotenoids improve cognitive functions in healthy adults?” To that effect, the electronic databases PubMed, the Cochrane Library, Web of Science, and PsycINFO were searched using specific search terms (Supplementary Table S2). Databases within the Cochrane Library included the Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, the Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials, the Cochrane Methodology Register, the Database of Abstracts of Reviews of Effects, the Health Technology Assessment Database, and the National Health Service Economic Evaluation Database. The search strategy comprised of terms related to or describing the intervention, and the search terms were adapted based on database-specific filters of each bibliographic database where available. Only English language articles were included and their publication period was unrestricted.

2.3. Detail of Included Studies

2.3.1. Types of Study

Only RCTs assessing the benefits of carotenoid intake were included.

2.3.2. Participants/Population

Inclusion in the SR was reserved to studies where the trial participants were men and women aged 18 years or older. For trials that included a mix of individuals older and younger than 18 years, a study was included if at least 90% of its trial participants were aged 18 years or older at baseline, or if the outcomes for the two age groups could be separated. This SR only included studies of individuals without cognitive impairment. Within any set of participants that fit the inclusion criteria, participants with a current diagnosis or history of Alzheimer’s disease, dementia, stroke, head injury, depression, or other neurologic disorders were excluded.

2.3.3. Intervention and Control

Only studies where the intake of carotenoids was oral were included in the review. Participants also had to have taken only one type of carotenoid, however, the simultaneous intake of two or more types of carotenoids was allowed if the combined carotenoids were of similar function and biodistribution (e.g., lutein and zeaxanthin) [23]. Studies were only admitted to the SR if two or more groups of carotenoids were administered and an orally taken placebo was included in the treatment design.

2.4. Main Outcomes

The following indicators of cognitive function were considered based on availability of a validated measurement technique: Overall cognitive function, memory, executive function, attention, verbal fluency, and reasoning. Computerized validated measurement techniques used included the Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale cognitive subscale (ADAS-cog), Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS), Cambridge Cognition Examination (CAMCOG), the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB), and the Central Nervous System Vital Signs test battery (CNSVS).

2.5. Data Extraction (Selection and Coding)

The titles or abstracts of studies were retrieved using the SR’s search strategy. Two reviewers independently screened any studies from additional sources for their fulfilment of the inclusion criteria. Full study reports of all potentially eligible studies were then retrieved and two reviewers independently assessed them for eligibility. Any disagreements between the two reviewers over the eligibility of particular studies were resolved through discussion [24].

2.6. Risk of Bias Assessment

Two reviewers independently evaluated the risk of bias in the included studies by using the modified Delphi list [25]. This list was designed based on the Delphi list [26]. To enhance the quality assessment, additional considerations included: Details of random allocation methods, an adequate description of the control/comparison group, between-group statistical comparisons, reports of dropouts, and reports of the consolidated standards of the reporting trials’ (CONSORT) statement [27]. Disagreements between the reviewers on the risk of bias in particular studies were resolved by discussion [24].

2.7. Data Synthesis

A qualitative synthesis was carried out to summarize each ingredient’s effectiveness in order to account for the heterogeneity of outcome assessments. We calculated the effect size as the standardized mean difference (SMD), which is equal to Hedge’s g, of the pre-intervention and post-intervention cognitive test score by using Review Manager 5.3 (Cochrane Collaboration, Copenhagen, Denmark).

3. Results

3.1. Results of the Search

The electronic database searches performed between November and December 2018 retrieved 1095 abstracts. After deduplication and screening titles or abstracts, 20 potentially eligible studies were identified. Among the 20 studies, 13 were excluded for not having groups treated with carotenoids alone (n = 11), having no measurement of cognitive function (n = 1), or no RCT design (n = 1). The study selection process is presented in the PRISMA flow chart (Figure 1).

3.2. Included Studies

Seven trials fulfilled the inclusion criteria of this systematic review, and all included studies had parallel designs (Table 1). Two studies [20,28] were performed as sub-studies of clinical trials that investigated the effects of macula carotenoid intake for improvement in visual function in a normal population and prevention of age-related macular degeneration.

3.3. Participants

The sample sizes ranged from 44 to 91. The mean age of participants ranged from 21.21 to 73.74 years. Among the seven studies, one study included young college students [29], three studies included middle-aged adults (aged 40–60 years) [20,30,31], and three studies included elderly individuals (aged over 60 years) [21,28,32]. Four of the trials were conducted in the United States [21,28,29,32], two in Japan [30,31], and one was conducted in Ireland [20].
All trials were restricted to cognitively healthy individuals and all participants were free of retinal disease. One study reported that the amount of Macular Pigment (MP) at baseline was low (i.e., MP at 0.23° of eccentricity ≤0.55 optical density units). MP can directly reflect the function of eye protection from high energy lights and correlate with concentrations of macular carotenoids in the brain [33].

3.4. Intervention

Intervention durations of the included studies were as follows: Four studies were conducted for 1 year [20,21,29,32], one study was 4 months long [28], one study was conducted for 12 weeks [30] and one study was conducted for 8 weeks [31].
In five studies [20,21,28,29,32], participants received lutein and its isomer in various capsulated amounts. One study reported that participants consumed a capsule containing 10 mg lutein, 2 mg zeaxanthin, and 10 mg meso-zeaxanthin with a meal [20]. Three studies reported that participants consumed a capsule containing 10 mg lutein and 2 mg zeaxanthin with the highest fat meal [21,29,32]. One study reported that participants consumed a capsule containing 12 mg lutein with a fatty energy drink [28].
In two studies [30,31], participants received pills or jellies containing various amounts of astaxanthin. One such study reported that participants consumed pills containing 6 mg or 12 mg of astaxanthin after breakfast [24], The other study reported that participants consumed jellies containing 8 mg of astaxanthin after breakfast and dinner [25].

3.5. Outcome

The outcome measures employed by all the studies were as shown in Table 2, and the results of their qualitative synthesis were as described in Table 3. Overall, 80 cognitive test outcomes were used. In this SR, the cognitive tests were divided into seven cognitive domains (verbal and visual episodic memory, short-term memory/working memory, reasoning, attention, inhibition, shifting, processing speed). Serum lutein (SL), which is correlated with cognitive function [34], and MP were measured. Lutein trials [20,21,28,29,32] and astaxanthin trials [30,31] were not integrated because lutein (and its isomers) and astaxanthin showed different pharmacokinetics [35]. Lutein trials using lutein and its isomers were integrated and discussed because the lutein and its isomers showed similar in vivo distribution and biological function [36]. Published per-protocol data [21,28,29,30,31,32] and unpublished data were used as received from Dr. Power [20] for conducting qualitative synthesis.

3.6. Lutein (and its isomers) Intervention

3.6.1. Macular Pigment (MP)

Four studies measured MP [20,21,29,32], with three measuring it as macular pigment optical density (MPOD) using customized heterochromatic photometry (cHFP) [21,29,32]. The mean of MPOD after the intervention was statistically higher than before intervention (MPOD change shows +0.07~0.09) and improved compared to the control group. The remaining study measured MP volume using dual-wavelength autofluorescence (AF) which operates based on different principles to cHFP [20]. MP volume is the sum of MPOD. MPOD is dimensionless. Therefore, MP volume is also unitless [37]. Like in the other studies, MP volume after the intervention was also higher than before (MP volume change shows +2558).

3.6.2. Serum Lutein (SL)

Four studies [20,21,28,29] measured SL, mostly using the same method. The SL was statistically improved compared to the control group (active: +0.475~0.773 umol/L, control: −0.018~+0.158 umol/L). When middle-aged and senior participants took lutein and zeaxanthin, there was a trend of increasing SL (change SL: 0.773~0.647 umol/L) compared to younger people (change SL: 0.475 umol/L).

3.6.3. Episodic Memory (Visual Stimuli)

Seven episodic memory (visual stimuli) outcomes were measured in four studies (Table 3) [20,21,28,29]. using; CANTAB [38], PAL, CNSVS [39], VIM, and the MIR apartment test [40]. Statistically significant correlations between SL concentration and amount of MP were reported in only PAL (errors adjusted for stage 6) (SL: r = −0.346, p = 0.006, MP: r = −0.342, p = 0.005). The tests of episodic memory (visual stimuli) performed for younger and middle-aged individuals (PAL memory score, PAL errors adjusted for stage 6, visual memory of CNSVS) [20,29] indicated statistically significant improvement, but not in older individuals [21,28]. However, the SMD of the post-intervention (effect size) in all studies was small (SMD < 0.5) or could not be calculated because the score of the post-intervention was not reported.

3.6.4. Episodic Memory (verbal stimuli)

Twenty-one Episodic Memory (verbal stimuli) outcomes were measured in five studies [20,21,28,29,32] (Table 3) using CANTAB VRM, Wechsler memory scale paired associates learning test [41], CNSVS VEM, Shopping List Task [42], and Word List Memory Test [43]. Only the Number of Trials to learn the list of Shopping List Task indicated statistically significant correlation with SL (correlation coefficient indicated 0.30, p < 0.05). VRM-delayed intrusion errors indicated statistically significant correlation with MP (r = 0.306, p = 0.033). Only VRM-delayed intrusion errors [20] showed statistically significant improvement, and the SMD of the outcome was small.

3.6.5. Short-term Memory/Working Memory

Four short-term memory/working memory outcomes were measured in one study [28] (Table 3) using forward and backward digit span [44]. No statistically significant correlations were reported with SL and there was no statistically significant improvement in the forward and backward digit span; the SMD of the outcome was also small.

3.6.6. Reasoning

Two reasoning outcomes were measured in two studies [21,29] (Table 3) using the CNSVS NVRT. The correlation between NVRT score and MP in older adults was statistically significant (r = 0.45, p = 0.04) [21], but there was no report of the correlations in younger adults [29]. Correlations with SL were not reported in the two studies. No intervention effects were observed in older adults [21], however, a sub-analysis in younger adults showed that the score change of NVRT statistically improved in MP increasers compared with participants in which no change was observed [29]. The SMD of the post-intervention could not be calculated because the score of the post-intervention was not reported.

3.6.7. Attention

Two Complex attention outcomes were measured in two studies [21,29] (Table 3) using CNSVS ST & SAT, and CPT. The correlation between the complex attention score of the CNSVS and MP in older adults allocated to the intervention group was statistically significant (r = −0.31, p = 0.04) [21], but the correlation was not reported in younger adults [29]. Correlation with SL was not reported in two studies. Additionally, intervention effects were observed in older adults [21], and a sub-analysis detected an improvement in young MP increasers [29]. The SMD of the post-intervention also could not be calculated because the score of the post-intervention was not reported.

3.6.8. Inhibition

Seven Inhibition outcomes were measured in four studies [20,21,28,29] (Table 3) using CANTAB AST, CNSVS ST & SAT, CNSVS SAT, and the Stroop test [45]. Two outcomes assessed in middle-aged or older individuals indicated statistically significant improvements (CANTAB AST congruency cost and CNSVS ST & SAT) [20,21], but no improvements were indicated on the other outcomes assessed for any age [28,29]. The correlation between the inhibition score of CNSVS ST & SAT and MP in older adults allocated to the intervention group was not statistically significant (r = 0.20, p = 0.10) [21], and correlations were not reported in the other studies [20,28,29]. A correlation with SL was not reported in all four studies [20,21,28,29]. SMDs of post-intervention in two studies were not statistically significant [20,28] and not reported in the other two studies [21,29].

3.6.9. Shifting

Five Shifting outcomes were measured in two studies [20,28] (Table 3) using Phonemic Fluency, Semantic Fluency, CNSVS SAT, and Verbal Fluency [46,47]. The outcome assessed for older individuals showed a statistically significant improvement within the group (Semantic Fluency) [28], but not correlated to SL (r = 0.03). The other outcomes assessed in middle-aged individuals were not statistically significant [20]. Correlations with MP were not reported in any studies. SMDs of the post-intervention in two studies were not statistically significant [20,28].

3.6.10. Processing Speed

Ten Processing speed outcomes were measured in four studies [20,21,28,29] using CNSVS psychomotor speed, NES2 Pattern Comparison Test [21,29], CANTAB comprehension speed of response [20], and the Stroop Test (Mean RTs read word-black, Mean RTs read word-color, and Mean RTs name colors) [28]. No correlation was reported between processing speed and SL or MP. There was also no statistically significant improvement in all outcomes, and the SMD of the outcome was small.

3.7. Adverse Events

One study mentioned measuring adverse events by phone bimonthly but did not report the results [32]. Four studies did not report information on adverse events [20,21,28,29].

3.8. Astaxanthin Intervention

3.8.1. Episodic Memory (visual stimuli)

Two outcomes were measured using the visual delayed recall test in CogHealth [30]. The response time was not changed among the groups. Only a high -dosage group showed significant improvement in the Delayed-Recall test score at 12 weeks of the intervention period compared to the baseline.

3.8.2. Episodic Memory (Verbal Stimuli)

Two outcomes were measured in one study by using the Delayed Word Memory test with an original test battery [31]. The Delayed-Recall score of the Word Memory test improved compared with the initial within-group score in subjects aged >55 years [31].

3.8.3. Short-term Memory/Working Memory

Six outcomes were measured in two studies by using the Visual Working Memory test with CogHealth [30], GMLT (Galton Maze Learning Test) [30], and Word Memory test using an original test battery [31]. Response time of Visual Working Memory test using CogHealth improved from baseline only in the 12 mg astaxanthin group [30]. GMLT performance at 8 weeks and 12 weeks of the treatment period in all groups was improved compared to the baseline performance [30]. Total errors in GMLT were reduced at 4, 8, 12 weeks of the intervention period in astaxanthin groups compared to the baseline [30]. The placebo group also showed a significant decrease in total errors in GMLT at 8 and 12 weeks compared to the baseline [30].

3.8.4. Inhibition

Two outcomes were measured in one study [31] using the paper-pencil version Stroop test (Step 2, 4) [48]. For example, in the paper-pencil version Stroop test, “GREEN” word is presented in red color. In the step 2 (the reverse Stroop interference task), participants are asked to answer the ink color of the stimulus (correct answer is red). In the step 4 (the Stroop interference task), participants are asked to answer the color name of the stimulus (correct answer is green). The large sample study using the same paper-pencil version Stroop test reported the no gender differences of the reverse Stroop interference and the Stroop interference tasks in any age-range [49]. The result was consistent with the previous studies using oral response version and PC version Stroop tests [50,51].
The RCT showed that no significant differences were observed between or within groups [31].

3.8.5. Attention

One outcome was measured in one study by divided attention using CogHealth [30]. No significant differences were observed between or within groups.

3.8.6. Shifting

Three outcomes were measured in one trial by using the Semantic Fluency and Phonemic Fluency [31] (Table 2) tests. No significant differences were observed between or within groups.

3.8.7. Processing Speed

Four outcomes were measured in two studies by (Table 2) [30,31] using the Stroop test Step 1, Stroop test Step 3, Simple reaction test and Choice reaction test. No significant differences were observed between or within groups.

3.9. Adverse Events

One study [30] reported that hematological tests, urinary tests, and oral consultation after 12 weeks of astaxanthin administration revealed no confirmed adverse effects. Another study [31] reported adverse events “unrelated” to astaxanthin intake, and physical measurements (i.e., blood and urine tests) showed minor changes.

3.10. Quality Assessment

3.10.1. Methodological Quality in Included Studies

An assessment of the methodological quality of the included studies is presented in Table 4. The range of the quality assessment score was 9 to 11, with an average of 9.7 (SD = 0.76). All included studies had sufficient methodological quality. The scores of Item 9 (intention-to-treat analysis) and 14 (reporting CONSORT statement) were low. However, all studies fulfilled the methodological qualities of Item 1 (Random Allocation), Item 4 (Eligibly Criteria Specified), Item 5 (Blinded Outcome Assessor), Item 6 (Care Provider Blinded), and Item 7 (Patient Blinded). In addition, six studies fulfilled the requirements of Item 2 (Treatment Allocation Concealed) [20,21,28,29,30,32], Item 12 (Between-group Statistical Comparison) [20,21,29,30,31,32], and Item 13 (Reporting Dropouts) [21,28,29,30,31,32].

3.10.2. Other Potential Sources of Bias

Power, 2018 was funded by the European Research Council. Lindberg 2018 was funded in part by Abbott Nutritional Products and the University of Georgia’s Bio-Imaging Research Center. Johnson 2008 was supported by USDA 1950-5100-065, Mead Johnson Nutritionals, and Martek Biosciences Corporation. Katagiri 2012 was supported by a grant from Yamaha Motors Co., Ltd. Two authors (M.H. and T.I.) of Hayashi 2018 were employees of JXTG Nippon Oil & Energy Corporation. Additionally, three studies were supported by Abbott Nutrition and used intervention supplements supplied by DSM Nutritional Products [21,29,32].

3.11. Excluded Studies

We excluded 13 relevant studies for using interventions other than carotenoids or its derivatives alone [52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61], not conducting cognitive assessment [62], and lack of a RCT [63].

4. Discussion

This study firstly summarized the beneficial effects of carotenoids on cognition in healthy adults. Five studies using lutein (and its isomers) and two studies using astaxanthin met the inclusion criteria of the SR. The SR revealed that three of the five studies with lutein reported significant benefit on several cognitive functions (visual episodic memory, verbal episodic memory, inhibition, and attention) compared to the placebo control group. However, the lutein consumption would not have beneficial effects on verbal episodic memory, working memory, reasoning, shifting, and processing speed. These results suggest that consumption of lutein would have a positive effect on cognitive functions in healthy adults. For astaxanthin, one of two studies showed significant improvements in memory performance compared to the placebo group. However, these findings should be interpreted with limitations due to the small number of studies. Therefore, we mainly discuss the result of lutein in the following sections.
For the benefits of lutein consumption, the SR obtained consistent results in visual episodic memory and inhibition between different age groups and between cognitive functional measurements. For visual episodic memory, two of the four studies that measured it showed significant performance improvement in young [29] and middle-aged adults [20]. For inhibition, two of the four studies that measured it showed significant performance improvements in middle-aged adults [20] and older adults [21]. Interestingly, the three studies which reported the cognitive benefits of lutein used the similar methods in terms of the intervention period (12 months) and the dose of lutein (10 mg per day). Based on these findings, 10 mg lutein consumption for twelve months can lead to improvement of visual episodic memory performance in young and middle-aged adults and improvement of inhibition performance in middle-aged and older adults.
On the contrary, inconsistent results were found in verbal episodic memory and attention domains. For verbal episodic memory, four of the five studies that measured verbal memory performance did not report significant group differences in young [29] and older adults [21,28,32], but one study showed significant group differences of in middle-aged adults [20]. Participants in the lutein group showed significantly fewer intrusion errors during the Delayed-Memory Recall compared to the control group [20]. However, it is important to note that the score of the intrusion errors at the baseline and the 12-month intervention period in both groups was almost zero (Table 3). In this case, the statistically significant finding would be clinically meaningful [20]. The study also measured several verbal episodic memory performances (e.g., immediate free recall and delayed free recall), but did not find any significant improvement in the memory performance. This study [20] used a similar dose of lutein (10 mg per day) and the same intervention period (12 months) to other three studies [21,29,32]. Based on these results, there seems to be no possibility that lutein consumption would lead to an improvement of verbal episodic memory performance.
Two of the five studies measured attention performance. Compared to the control group, a significant improvement was found in only older adults [21], but not in young adults [29]. The two studies used the same cognitive tests (CNSVS), dose of carotenoid (lutein: 10 mg per day and zeaxanthin: 2 mg per day), and intervention period (12 months). The difference in attention performance at the baseline would affect this inconsistent result. These studies calculated attention performance by combining the number of errors in attention tasks (a lower score means a few errors). The attention performance at the baseline was different between young adults (7.38) [29] and older adults (12.27) [21]. It could be difficult to find the improvement of attention performance in young adults because they had better attention at the baseline. Based on the inconsistent results, it is hard to conclude that lutein consumption had a benefit on attention performance. However, there is still a possibility that lutein consumption would improve attention performance because individuals with increased MPOD levels showed improved attention performance compared to non-increasers in young adults [29]. This suggests that attention performance would be improved in the young adults if the dose of lutein were increased. In addition, there is no study investigating the effect of lutein consumption on attention performance in middle-aged adults. In the future, it should be important to investigate the effect of lutein on cognitive function using a higher dose of lutein or middle-aged participants.
Several studies reported the significant correlation between cognitive functions (such as MP and SL) and carotenoid concentration. Previous cohort studies demonstrated the significant correlation between the MP and SL and a wide range of cognitive functions including visual episodic memory, inhibition, and attention in middle-aged and older adults [63,64,65]. The current results of this SR are consistent with these cohort studies. For example, the change of MP had a significant correlation with the improvement of memory performances, inhibition, attention in middle-aged and older adults [20,21]. One study reported that the change of MP and SL had a significant association with the decrease of memory errors in middle-aged adults [20]. These results directly indicated that the enhancement of carotenoid concentration by lutein consumption has a critical role in improvements of cognitive functions in middle-aged and older adults.
It is important to consider a biological mechanism of cognitive improvement by lutein consumption. Lutein and its isomers have roles of antioxidant and anti-inflammatory functions in the body [66]. A recent human study reported that lutein consumption for 6 months increased total antioxidant capacity (AOC) and brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in young adults [67]. The level of BDNF is a biomarker of anti-inflammation because inflammation would affect the expression of BDNF [68]. In addition, the change of the BDNF and that of AOC were significantly correlated with cognitive functions, especially memory performance [67]. These results indicated that the antioxidant and anti-inflammatory functions of lutein and its isomers play a large role in improvements in cognitive functions.
Lutein and its isomers can cross the blood-brain barrier (BBB) [69]. Therefore, their anti-oxidant and anti-inflammatory functions would directly act in the human brain. Notably, lutein is selectively distributed in the frontal cortex, visual cortex, and hippocampus [70,71]. The concentration level of lutein and its isomers in the prefrontal cortex was higher than that in other regions [70]. The concentration of lutein was positively correlated with regional gray matter volume in the parahippocampal gyrus [72]. A cross-sectional brain imaging study using fMRI demonstrated that the concentration level of lutein was associated with brain activations in the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and the visual cortex [73]. A recent longitudinal fMRI study reported that one-year lutein consumption led to an increase of brain activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and hippocampus [32]. Previous neuroimaging studies reported that DLPFC, ACC, and inferior frontal gyrus are important to the inhibition process [74,75]. The medial temporal lobe, which included the hippocampus and parahippocampus, has an important role for memory functions [76,77]. These results suggest that lutein and its isomer would affect the memory and inhibition related brain regions.
For the benefits of astaxanthin on cognitive functions, only two studies were included in this SR [30,31]. The participants of the included studies were the middle-aged adults; however, different methodologies were used in the studies such as cognitive measurement, the intervention period (12 and 8 weeks), and the dose of astaxanthin (12 mg (6 mg) and 8 mg). One of the two studies using 8 mg astaxanthin reported significant improvement of verbal episodic memory performance after 8 weeks in middle-aged adults (less than 55 years old) [31]. Due to the small number of studies, it is difficult to conclude whether astaxanthin would have positive effects on cognitive functions. However, there is still a possibility that astaxanthin consumption has a positive effect on cognitive functions. Another study included in this SR reported that only a high dose astaxanthin group (12 mg) showed the significant pre-post improvement of working memory performance (reaction time) and delay visual episodic memory performance (accuracy) [30]. The low dose (6 mg) and placebo (0 mg) groups did not show any significant pre–post changes in working memory and delayed visual episodic memory performance [30]. This suggests that astaxanthin may be beneficial to memory function in middle-aged adults.
It is clear that the positive effects of these carotenoids on cognitive functions were determined on the basis of supplementation rather than normal daily consumptions. The previous studies demonstrated that the average adults consumed about 2 mg lutein per day [78,79]. It was lower than the dose of the supplementation (10 mg lutein). To take 10 mg lutein from vegetables and fruits, people need about 200 g every day [80]. For astaxanthin, if people take 4 mg astaxanthin in daily consumption, they need to eat about 600 to 2000 g salmon or seafood every day [81,82]. Two RCTs using astaxanthin were conducted in Japan. The National Health and Nutrition Survey in Japan in 2017 reported that average Japanese adults eat about 65 g seafood per day (https://www.mhlw.go.jp/bunya/kenkou/kenkou_eiyou_chousa.html). It was also lower than the dose of the supplementation (8 or 12 mg astaxanthin). These results indicate that it is not easy to take enough lutein and astaxanthin to affect cognitive functions from daily consumption. The supplementation of lutein or astaxanthin led to improvements of cognitive functions in the RCT.
The limitations of this SR are the inclusion of only seven RCT studies and the use of a wide range of cognitive functional measures among included studies (Table 2). It would be difficult to summarize the cognitive domain among studies. Due to the small number of studies and different cognitive functional measurements, it is hard to make a clear conclusion about the benefits of carotenoid intakes on cognitive functions. More RCT studies with lutein and astaxanthin using similar and common cognitive functional tests are needed to make a clear conclusion.
Seven RCT studies in the SR did not report adverse events related to lutein and astaxanthin. A previous meta-analysis reported that overdose of antioxidant increases the risk of a health problem [83]. It must be noted that supplementation with antioxidants should be required delicate handling because of the risk of overdosing poses.

5. Conclusions

We firstly conducted the SR for RCTs to investigate the benefits of carotenoids intakes on cognitive functions. From seven RCT studies (five studies using lutein and two studies using astaxanthin), three studies using lutein reported significant improvements of several cognitive functions (visual episodic memory, verbal episodic memory, inhibition, attention). The SR revealed that lutein selectively leads to improvement of visual episodic memory in young and middle-aged adults and inhibition in the middle-aged and older adults. For astaxanthin, one study reported significant improvement of verbal episodic memory performance in middle-aged adults. The SR indicates that carotenoid (lutein and astaxanthin) would have positive impacts on cognitive functions among different age groups.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/2072-6643/12/3/617/s1, Table S1: PRISMA 2009 Checklist, Table S2: Search terms and strategy.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, R.N., T.S. and E.K.; Methodology, R.N., T.S. and E.K.; Validation, R.N., T.S. and E.K.; Formal Analysis, T.S. and E.K.; Investigation, T.S. and E.K.; Writing—original draft preparation, R.N., T.S., E.K. and H.T.; Writing—review and editing, R.N., T.S., E.K., H.T., M.M., A.U., M.A., and R.K.; Supervision, R.K.; Project Administration, R.N.; Funding Acquisition, R.N. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study is also supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number 16KT0002, 19H01760 (Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (B) and 19H05003 (Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas (Research in a proposed research area)). None of the funding sources had any involvement in the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data, or the writing of the paper.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to John Nolan and Rebecca Power at Waterford Institute of Technology, Ireland. They kindly provided unpublished data. They had no involvement in the study design, data collection, analysis, or reporting of the results. We also thank to Tomoji Ono he supports to write the manuscript draft.

Conflicts of Interest

This study was a corroborative study between Institute of Development, Aging, and Cancer (IDAC), Tohoku University and Lion Corporation. supported by Lion Corporation. R.N. and R.K. received research grants from Lion Corporation. T.S., E.K., H.T., M.M., A.U., and M.A. are full-time employees of Lion Corporation. The company policy was not involved in the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data, or writing of the manuscript. The authors declare that they have no other competing interests.

References

  1. Park, D.C.; Lautenschlager, G.; Hedden, T.; Davidson, N.S.; Smith, A.D.; Smith, P.K. Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychol. Aging 2002, 17, 299–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Spencer, S.J.; Korosi, A.; Layé, S.; Shukitt-Hale, B.; Barrientos, R.M. Food for thought: How nutrition impacts cognition and emotion. NPJ Sci. Food 2017, 1, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Taki, Y.; Hashizume, H.; Sassa, Y.; Takeuchi, H.; Asano, M.; Asano, K.; Kawashima, R. Breakfast staple types affect brain gray matter volume and cognitive function in healthy children. PLoS ONE 2010, 5, e15213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Shimizu, K.; Ihira, H.; Mizumoto, A.; Makino, K.; Ishida, T.; Shimada, H.; Furuna, T. Relationship between dietary habits and cognitive function among community-dwelling elderly adults. Physiotherapy 2015, 101, e1390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Petersson, S.D.; Philippou, E. Mediterranean Diet, Cognitive Function, and Dementia: A Systematic Review of the Evidence. Adv. Nutr. 2016, 7, 889–904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Péneau, S.; Galan, P.; Jeandel, C.; Ferry, M.; Andreeva, V.; Hercberg, S.; Kesse-Guyot, E.; Vogt, L.; Escande, M.; Sérot, J.M.; et al. Fruit and vegetable intake and cognitive function in the SU.VI.MAX 2 prospective study. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2011, 94, 1295–1303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Mottaghi, T.; Amirabdollahian, F.; Haghighatdoost, F. Fruit and Vegetable Intake and Cognitive Impairment: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Observational Studies; Nature Publishing Group: Berlin, Germany, 2018; Volume 72, pp. 1336–1344. [Google Scholar]
  8. Beydoun, M.A.; Fanelli-Kuczmarski, M.T.; Kitner-Triolo, M.H.; Beydoun, H.A.; Kaufman, J.S.; Mason, M.A.; Evans, M.K.; Zonderman, A.B. Dietary antioxidant intake and its association with cognitive function in an ethnically diverse sample of US adults. Psychosom. Med. 2015, 77, 68–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Devore, E.E.; Grodstein, F.; van Rooij, F.J.A.; Hofman, A.; Stampfer, M.J.; Witteman, J.C.M.; Breteler, M.M.B. Dietary Antioxidants and Long-term Risk of Dementia. Arch. Neurol. 2010, 67, 819–825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Alharbi, M.H.; Lamport, D.J.; Dodd, G.F.; Saunders, C.; Harkness, L.; Butler, L.T.; Spencer, J.P.E. Flavonoid-rich orange juice is associated with acute improvements in cognitive function in healthy middle-aged males. Eur. J. Nutr. 2016, 55, 2021–2029. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Ceravolo, S.A.; Hammond, B.R.; Oliver, W.; Clementz, B.; Miller, L.S.; Renzi-Hammond, L.M. Dietary Carotenoids Lutein and Zeaxanthin Change Brain Activation in Older Adult Participants: A Randomized, Double-Masked, Placebo-Controlled Trial. Mol. Nutr. Food Res. 2019, 63, 1801051. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Lamport, D.J.; Dye, L.; Wightman, J.L.D.; Lawton, C.L. The effects of flavonoid and other polyphenol consumption on cognitive performance: A systematic research review of human experimental and epidemiological studies. Nutr. Aging 2012, 1, 5–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Ma, Q.P.; Huang, C.; Cui, Q.Y.; Yang, D.J.; Sun, K.; Chen, X.; Li, X.H. Meta-Analysis of the Association between Tea Intake and the Risk of Cognitive Disorders. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0165861. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Yabuzaki, J. Carotenoids Database: Structures, chemical fingerprints and distribution among organisms. Database 2017, 2017, bax004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Vershinin, A. Biological functions of carotenoids--diversity and evolution. Biofactors 1999, 10, 99–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Yeum, K.J.; Aldini, G.; Russell, R.M.; Krinsky, N.I. Antioxidant/pro-oxidant actions of carotenoids. In Carotenoids; Britton, G., Liaaen-Jensen, S., Pfander, H., Eds.; Birkhäuser Verlag: Basel, Switzerland, 2009; pp. 235–268. ISBN 978-3-7643-7500-3. [Google Scholar]
  17. Khachik, F. Distribution and metabolism of dietary carotenoids in humans as a criterion for development of nutritional supplements. Pure Appl. Chem. 2006, 78, 1551–1557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Eggersdorfer, M.; Wyss, A. Carotenoids in human nutrition and health. Arch. Biochem. Biophys. 2018, 652, 18–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Johnson, E.J. Role of lutein and zeaxanthin in visual and cognitive function throughout the lifespan. Nutr. Rev. 2014, 72, 605–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Power, R.; Coen, R.F.; Beatty, S.; Mulcahy, R.; Moran, R.; Stack, J.; Howard, A.N.; Nolan, J.M. Supplemental Retinal Carotenoids Enhance Memory in Healthy Individuals with Low Levels of Macular Pigment in A Randomized, Double-Blind, Placebo-Controlled Clinical Trial. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2018, 61, 947–961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Hammond, B.R.; Stephen Miller, L.; Bello, M.O.; Lindbergh, C.A.; Mewborn, C.; Renzi-Hammond, L.M. Effects of lutein/zeaxanthin supplementation on the cognitive function of community dwelling older adults: A randomized, double-masked, placebo-controlled trial. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2017, 9, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Moher, D.; Liberati, A.; Tetzlaff, J.; Altman, D.G.; Group, P. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses: The PRISMA Statement. Ann. Intern. Med. 2009, 151, 264–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Kotake-Nara, E.; Nagao, A. Absorption and metabolism of xanthophylls. Mar. Drugs 2011, 9, 1024–1037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Lefebvre, C.; Glanville, J.; Briscoe, S.; Littlewood, A.; Marshall, C.; Metzendorf, M.I.; Noel-Storr, A.; Rader, T.; Shokraneh, F.; Thomas, J.; et al. No Title. In Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions; Higgins, J.P.T., Thomas, J., Chandler, J., Cumpston, M., Li, T., Page, M.J., Eds.; Cochrane: London, UK, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  25. Nouchi, R.; Kawashima, R. Improving Cognitive Function from Children to Old Age: A Systematic Review of Recent Smart Ageing Intervention Studies. Adv. Neurosci. 2014, 2014, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Verhagen, A.P.; De Vet, H.C.W.; De Bie, R.A.; Kessels, A.G.H.; Boers, M.; Bouter, L.M.; Knipschild, P.G. The Delphi List: A Criteria List for Quality Assessment of Randomized Clinical Trials for Conducting Systematic Reviews Developed by Delphi Consensus. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 1998, 51, 1235–1241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Antes, G. The new CONSORT statement. BMJ 2010, 340, 666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Johnson, E.J.; McDonald, K.; Caldarella, S.M.; Chung, H.Y.; Troen, A.M.; Snodderly, D.M. Cognitive findings of an exploratory trial of docosahexaenoic acid and lutein supplementation in older women. Nutr. Neurosci. 2008, 11, 75–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Renzi-Hammond, L.M.; Bovier, E.R.; Fletcher, L.M.; Miller, L.S.; Mewborn, C.M.; Lindbergh, C.A.; Baxter, J.H.; Hammond, B.R. Effects of a lutein and zeaxanthin intervention on cognitive function: A randomized, double-masked, placebo-controlled trial of younger healthy adults. Nutrients 2017, 9, 1246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Katagiri, M.; Satoh, A.; Tsuji, S.; Shirasawa, T. Effects of astaxanthin-rich Haematococcus pluvialis extract on cognitive function: A randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. J. Clin. Biochem. Nutr. 2012, 51, 102–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Hayashi, M.; Ishibashi, T.; Maoka, T. Effect of astaxanthinnrich extract derived from Paracoccus carotinifaciens on cognitive function in middleeaged and older individuals. J. Clin. Biochem. Nutr. 2018, 62, 195–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Lindbergh, C.A.; Renzi-Hammond, L.M.; Hammond, B.R.; Terry, D.P.; Mewborn, C.M.; Puente, A.N.; Miller, L.S. Lutein and Zeaxanthin Influence Brain Function in Older Adults: A Randomized Controlled Trial. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2018, 24, 77–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Ajana, S.; Weber, D.; Helmer, C.; Merle, B.M.; Stuetz, W.; Dartigues, J.F.; Rougier, M.B.; Korobelnik, J.F.; Grune, T.; Delcourt, C.; et al. Plasma concentrations of lutein and zeaxanthin, macular pigment optical density, and their associations with cognitive performances among older adults. Investig. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2018, 59, 1828–1835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Feeney, J.; O’Leary, N.; Moran, R.; O’Halloran, A.M.; Nolan, J.M.; Beatty, S.; Young, I.S.; Kenny, R.A. Plasma Lutein and Zeaxanthin Are Associated With Better Cognitive Function Across Multiple Domains in a Large Population-Based Sample of Older Adults: Findings from The Irish Longitudinal Study on Aging. J. Gerontol. Ser. A 2017, 72, 1431–1436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Bohn, T.; Desmarchelier, C.; Dragsted, L.O.; Nielsen, C.S.; Stahl, W.; Rühl, R.; Keijer, J.; Borel, P. Host-related factors explaining interindividual variability of carotenoid bioavailability and tissue concentrations in humans. Mol. Nutr. Food Res. 2017, 61, 1600685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Giordano, E.; Quadro, L. Lutein, zeaxanthin and mammalian development: Metabolism, functions and implications for health. Arch. Biochem. Biophys. 2018, 647, 33–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Green-Gomez, M.; Bernstein, P.S.; Curcio, C.A.; Moran, R.; Roche, W.; Nolan, J.M. Standardizing the assessment of macular pigment using a dual-wavelength autofluorescence technique. Transl. Vis. Sci. Technol. 2019, 8, 41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Zygouris, S.; Tsolaki, M. Computerized cognitive testing for older adults: A review. Am. J. Alzheimers. Dis. Other Demen. 2015, 30, 13–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. GUALTIERI, C.; JOHNSON, L. Reliability and validity of a computerized neurocognitive test battery, CNS Vital Signs. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2006, 21, 623–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  40. Johansson, B. The MIR–Memory-in-Reality Test; Psykologiforlaget AB: Stockholm, Sweden, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  41. Wechsler. The Wechsler Memory Scale, 4th ed.; Pearson Assessments: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  42. McCarthy, M.; Ferris, S.H.; Clark, E.; Crook, T. Acquistion and retention of categorized material in normal aging and senile dementia. Exp. Aging Res. 1981, 7, 127–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Moms, J.C.; Heyman, A.; Mohs, R.C.; Hughes, J.P.; van Belle, G.; Fillenbaum, G.; Mellits, E.D.; Clark, C. The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assesment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1989, 39, 1159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Wechsler DA Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised; Psychological Corporation: New York, NY, USA, 1981.
  45. Stroop, J.R. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 1935, 18, 643–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Shao, Z.; Janse, E.; Visser, K.; Meyer, A.S. What do verbal fluency tasks measure? Predictors of verbal fluency performance in older adults. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Borkowski, J.G.; Benton, A.L.; Spreen, O. Word fluency and brain damage. Neuropsychologia 1967, 5, 135–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Hakoda, Y.; Watanabe, M. Manual for New Stroop Test II; Toyo Physical: Fukuoka, Japan, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  49. Matsumoto, A.; Hakoda, Y.; Watanabe, M. Life-span development of Stroop and reverse-Stroop interference measured using matching responses. Jpn. J. Psychol. 2012, 83, 337–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  50. Houx, P.J.; Jolles, J.; Vreeling, F.W. Stroop interference: Aging effects assessed with the stroop color-word test. Exp. Aging Res. 1993, 19, 209–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Macleod, C.M. Half a Century of Research on the Stroop Effect-An Integrative Review the Production Effect View project. Psychol. Bull. 1991, 109, 163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Scott, T.M.; Rasmussen, H.M.; Chen, O.; Johnson, E.J. Avocado Consumption Increases Macular Pigment Density in Older Adults: A Randomized, Controlled Trial. Nutrients 2017, 9, 919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Ito, N.; Saito, H.; Seki, S.; Ueda, F.; Asada, T. Effects of Composite Supplement Containing Astaxanthin and Sesamin on Cognitive Functions in People with Mild Cognitive Impairment: A Randomized, Double-Blind, Placebo-Controlled Trial. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2018, 62, 1767–1775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Smith, A.P.; Clark, R.E.; Nutt, D.J.; Haller, J.; Hayward, S.G.; Perry, K. Vitamin C, Mood and Cognitive Functioning in the Elderly. Nutr. Neurosci. 1999, 2, 249–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Yaffe, K.; Clemons, T.E.; McBee, W.L.; Lindblad, A.S. Impact of antioxidants, zinc, and copper on cognition in the elderly: A randomized, controlled trial. Neurology 2004, 63, 1705–1707. [Google Scholar]
  56. Wolters, M.; Hickstein, M.; Flintermann, A.; Tewes, U.; Hahn, A. Cognitive performance in relation to vitamin status in healthy elderly German women-The effect of 6-month multivitamin supplementation. Prev. Med. Balt. 2005, 41, 253–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Grodstein, F.; Kang, J.H.; Glynn, R.J.; Cook, N.R.; Gaziano, J.M. A randomized trial of beta carotene supplementation and cognitive function in men: The physicians’ health study II. Arch. Intern. Med. 2007, 167, 2184–2190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Kang, J.H.; Cook, N.R.; Manson, J.E.; Buring, J.E.; Albert, C.M.; Grodstein, F. Vitamin E, Vitamin C, Beta carotene, and cognitive function among women with or at risk of cardiovascular disease: The women’s antioxidant and cardiovascular study. Circulation 2009, 119, 2772–2780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Yagi, A.; Fujimoto, K.; Michihiro, K.; Goh, B.; Tsi, D.; Nagai, H. The effect of lutein supplementation on visual fatigue: A psychophysiological analysis. Appl. Erg. 2009, 40, 1047–1054. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Kesse-Guyot, E.; Fezeu, L.; Jeandel, C.; Ferry, M.; Andreeva, V.; Amieva, H.; Hercberg, S.; Galan, P. French adults’ cognitive performance after daily supplementation with antioxidant vitamins and minerals at nutritional doses: A post hoc analysis of the supplementation in vitamins and mineral antioxidants (SU.VI.MAX) trial. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2011, 94, 892–899. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Bovier, E.R.; Hammond, B.R. A randomized placebo-controlled study on the effects of lutein and zeaxanthin on visual processing speed in young healthy subjects. Arch. Biochem. Biophys. 2015, 572, 54–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  62. Bovier, E.R.; Renzi, L.M.; Hammond, B.R. A Double-Blind, Placebo-Controlled Study on the Effects of Lutein and Zeaxanthin on Neural Processing Speed and Efficiency. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e108178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Kelly, D.; Coen, R.F.; Akuffo, K.O.; Beatty, S.; Dennison, J.; Moran, R.; Stack, J.; Howard, A.N.; Mulcahy, R.; Nolan, J.M. Cognitive function and its relationship with macular pigment optical density and serum concentrations of its constituent carotenoids. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2015, 48, 261–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Feeney, J.; O’Sullivan, M.; Kenny, R.A.; Robertson, I.H. Change in perceived stress and 2-year change in cognitive function among older adults: The Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing. Stress Heal. 2018, 34, 403–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Feeney, J.; Finucane, C.; Savva, G.M.; Cronin, H.; Beatty, S.; Nolan, J.M.; Kenny, R.A. Low macular pigment optical density is associated with lower cognitive performance in a large, population-based sample of older adults. Neurobiol. Aging 2013, 34, 2449–2456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Ozawa, Y.; Sasaki, M.; Takahashi, N.; Kamoshita, M.; Miyake, S.; Tsubota, K. Neuroprotective Effects of Lutein in the Retina. Curr. Pharm. Des. 2012, 18, 51–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Stringham, N.T.; Holmes, P.V.; Stringham, J.M. Effects of macular xanthophyll supplementation on brain-derived neurotrophic factor, pro-inflammatory cytokines, and cognitive performance. Physiol. Behav. 2019, 211, 112650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Calabrese, F.; Rossetti, A.C.; Racagni, G.; Gass, P.; Riva, M.A.; Molteni, R. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor: A bridge between inflammation and neuroplasticity. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Stringham, J.M.; Johnson, E.J.; Hammond, B.R. Lutein across the Lifespan: From Childhood Cognitive Performance to the Aging Eye and Brain. Curr. Dev. Nutr. 2019, 3, nzz066. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  70. Craft, N.E.; Haitema, T.B.; Garnett, K.M.; Fitch, K.A.; Dorey, C.K. Carotenoid, tocopherol, and retinol concentrations in elderly human brain. J. Nutr. Heal. Aging 2004, 8, 156–162. [Google Scholar]
  71. Vishwanathan, R.; Kuchan, M.J.; Sen, S.; Johnson, E.J. Lutein and preterm infants with decreased concentrations of brain carotenoids. J. Pediatr. Gastroenterol. Nutr. 2014, 59, 659–665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Zamroziewicz, M.K.; Paul, E.J.; Zwilling, C.E.; Johnson, E.J.; Kuchan, M.J.; Cohen, N.J.; Barbey, A.K. Parahippocampal Cortex Mediates the Relationship between Lutein and Crystallized Intelligence in Healthy, Older Adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2016, 8, 297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  73. Lindbergh, C.A.; Mewborn, C.M.; Hammond, B.R.; Renzi-Hammond, L.M.; Curran-Celentano, J.M.; Miller, L.S. Relationship of Lutein and Zeaxanthin Levels to Neurocognitive Functioning: An fMRI Study of Older Adults. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2017, 23, 11–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  74. Laird, A.R.; McMillan, K.M.; Lancaster, J.L.; Kochunov, P.; Turkeltaub, P.E.; Pardo, J.V.; Fox, P.T. A comparison of label-based review and ALE meta-analysis in the Stroop task. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2005, 25, 6–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Simmonds, D.J.; Pekar, J.J.; Mostofsky, S.H. Meta-analysis of Go/No-go tasks demonstrating that fMRI activation associated with response inhibition is task-dependent. Neuropsychologia 2008, 46, 224–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  76. Jeong, W.; Chung, C.K.; Kim, J.S. Episodic Memory in Aspects of Large-Scale Brain Networks; Frontiers Media SA: Lausanne, Switzerland, 2015; Volume 9, p. 454. [Google Scholar]
  77. Squire, L.R.; Wixted, J.T. The Cognitive Neuroscience of Human Memory since H.M. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2011, 34, 259–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  78. Alvarado-Ramos, K.E.; De Leon, L.; Fontes, F.; Rios-Castillo, I. Dietary Consumption of Lutein and Zeaxanthin in Panama: A Cross-Sectional Study. Curr. Dev. Nutr. 2018, 2, nzy064. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Institute of Medicine. Dietary Reference Intakes for Vitamin C, Vitamin E, Selenium, and Carotenoids; National Academies Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2000; ISBN 978-0-309-06935-9. [Google Scholar]
  80. Aizawa, K.; Inakuma, T. Quantitation of Carotenoids in Commonly Consumed Vegetables in Japan. Food Sci. Technol. Res. 2007, 13, 247–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  81. Bjerkeng, B.; Peisker, M.; von Schwartzenberg, K.; Ytrestøyl, T.; Åsgård, T. Digestibility and muscle retention of astaxanthin in Atlantic salmon, Salmo salar, fed diets with the red yeast Phaffia rhodozyma in comparison with synthetic formulated astaxanthin. Aquaculture 2007, 269, 476–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Turujman, S.A.; Wamer, W.G.; Wei, R.R.; Albert, R.H. Rapid Liquid Chromatographic Method to Distinguish Wild Salmon from Aquacultured Salmon Fed Synthetic Astaxanthin. J. Aoac Int. 1997, 80, 622–632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  83. Bjelakovic, G.; Nikolova, D.; Gluud, L.L.; Simonetti, R.G.; Gluud, C. Mortality in Randomized Trials of Antioxidant Supplements for Primary and Secondary Prevention. JAMA 2007, 297, 842. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Study selection flow chart.
Figure 1. Study selection flow chart.
Nutrients 12 00617 g001
Table 1. Characteristics of included studies.
Table 1. Characteristics of included studies.
Lead Author; Year;
Country
Study Design,
Duration
Sample size
(Female)
Age
(mean ± SD)
Health StatusCognitive StatusIntervention
(Timing or Method)
Control
(Contents)
Power; 2018; IrelandA parallel-group, double-blind, placebo-controlled, block-randomized clinical trial,
12 months
91 (48%)
P: 46
A: 45
P: 46.43 ± 13.21
A: 44.38 ± 11.57
Low MP volume without the retinal disease, no consumption of L and/or Z and/or MZ supplementNo impairmentL: 10 mg/d
MZ: 10 mg/d
Z: 2 mg/d
(with a meal)
Placebo
(capsule containing sunflower oil)
Lindberg; 2017; USAA single-site, double-blind RCT,
12 months
44 (59%)
P: 14
A: 30
P: 70.43 ± 5.43
A: 72.43 ± 6.48
Community-dwelling older adults, good overall health, no consumption of xanthophyll supplementn/RL: 10 mg/d
Z: 2 mg/d
(n/R)
Placebo
(n/R)
Renzi-Hammond; 2017; USAA randomized, double-masked, placebo-controlled trial,
12 months
51 (43%)
P: 14
A: 37
P: 20.5 ± 1.91
A: 21.5 ± 2.69
Healthy young college students, no consumption of the supplementNo impairmentL: 10 mg/d
Z: 2 mg/d
(with the highest fat meal)
Placebo
(n/R)
Hammond; 2017; USAThe double-masked, randomized, placebo-controlled trial,
12 months
51 (59%)
P: 15
A: 36
P: 70.93 ± 5.70
A: 72.51 ± 6.24
Healthy community-dwelling older adults, no consumption of L&Z supplementNo impairmentL: 10 mg/d
Z: 2 mg/d
(with the highest fat meal)
Placebo
(n/R)
Johnson; 2008; USARandomized, double-blind, intervention trial,
4 months
49 (100%)
P: 10
D: 14
L: 11
D+L: 14
P: 68.0 ± 1.2
D: 68.5 ± 1.3
L: 66.7 ± 1.9
D+L: 68.6 ± 1.3
Healthy, non-smoking older women, no consumption of carotenoids supplementNo impairmentL: 12 mg/d
(with nutritional energy drink)
Placebo
(n/R)
Katagiri; 2012; JapanThe randomized double-blind placebo-controlled study,
12 weeks
89 (n/R)
HAx: 29
LAx: 29
P: 31
HAx: 51.5 ± 5.7
LAx: 51.1 ± 5.9
P: 51.6 ± 5.3
Healthy men and womenComplaints of age-related forgetfulnessHAx: A 12 mg/d
LAx: A 6 mg/d
(after breakfast or lunch or supper)
Placebo
(a pill containing corn oil)
Hayashi; 2018; JapanRandomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel inter-group comparison
8 weeks
54 (54%)
P: 26
Ax: 28
P: 54.4 ± 6.0
Ax: 56.0 ± 5.2
Healthy men and womenNo impairmentAx 8 mg/d
(after breakfast and dinner)
Placebo
(jelly without Ax)
Note: RCT: Randomized control trial. P: Placebo control group. A: Active intervention group. D: DHA supplementation group. L: Lutein supplementation group. D+L: DHA + Lutein supplementation group. HAx: High astaxanthin supplementation group. LAx: Low astaxanthin supplementation group. MP: Macular pigment. L: Lutein. Z: Zeaxanthin. MZ: Meso-zeaxanthin. Ax: Astaxanthin. n/R: Not reported.
Table 2. Description of measurements, analysis, and intervention effects of cognitive function.
Table 2. Description of measurements, analysis, and intervention effects of cognitive function.
Lead author; Year; CountryTest BatteryTasks for Calculating the Index ScoreDomain of Cognitive FunctionsMeasurement Indices Statistical Analysis MethodsResults
Analytical
Objects
Intervention Effects
Power; 2018; IrelandCANTABMOTProcessing speedMean latencyrANOVA


(Only time effects and time-group interaction effects are shown)
Valuen/R
Mean total correctValuen/R
Mean total errorsValuen/R
ASTInhibitionAST correct latencyValue0
AST percent correctValue0
AST congruency costValue+
VRMEpisodic memory
(verbal stimuli)
VRM Trial 1 immediate free recallValuen/R
VRM Trial 2 immediate free recallValuen/R
VRM Trial 3 immediate free recallValuen/R
VRM Total immediate free recallValuen/R
VRM Trial 1 intrusion errorsValuen/R
VRM Trial 2 intrusion errorsValuen/R
VRM Trial 3 intrusion errorsValuen/R
VRM Learning slopeValuen/R
VRM delayed free recallValuen/R
VRM delayed intrusion errorsValue+
VRM delayed recognition totalValuen/R
VRM recognition false positivesValuen/R
PALEpisodic memory
(visual stimuli)
PAL memory scoreValue+
PAL total errorsValuen/R
PAL total errors stage 6Value+
“FAS” and “Animal” testFAS testShiftingSum of the number of words beginning with the letters F, A, and S generated with a 1-min time limit per letterValue0
Animal testShiftingThe number of animal names generated with a 1-min time limitValuen/R
Lindberg; 2018; USAWTARWTARGlobal cognitionNumber of words correctly pronounced2 × 2 mixed-design ANOVAValuen/R
fMRI taskWechsler memory scale paired associates learning testEpisodic memory (verbal stimuli)Number of cued recallt-testValue0
Renzi-Hammond; 2017; USACNSVSVBMEpisodic memory (verbal stimuli)Verbal memory score
(VBM correct hits (immediate) + VBM correct – passes (immediate) + VBM correct hits (delay) + VBM correct − passes (delay))
RCIDeltan/R
VIMEpisodic memory (visual stimuli)Visual memory score
(VIM correct hits (immediate) + VIM correct − passes (immediate) + VIM correct hits (delay) + VIM correct − passes (delay))
Delta+
NVRTReasoning Reasoning score
(NVRT correct responses − NVRT errors)
Delta+*(“MPOD increaser”)
SATShiftingExecutive function score
(SAT correct responses − SAT errors)
Deltan/R
FTT, SDCProcessing speedPsychomotor speed score
(FTT right response + FTT left response + SDC corrects)
Deltan/R
ST, SAT, CPTAttentionComplex Attention score
(ST errors + SAT errors + CPT errors + CPT correct − passes)
Delta+*(“MPOD increaser”)
ST, SATInhibitionCognitive Flexibility score
(SAT correct responses − SAT error − ST error)
Deltan/R
Hammond; 2017; USACNSVSVBMEpisodic memory (verbal stimuli)Verbal memory score
(VBM correct hits (immediate) + VBM correct − passes (immediate) + VBM correct hits (delay) + VBM correct − passes (delay))
RCIDeltan/R
VIMEpisodic memory (visual stimuli)Visual memory score
(VIM correct hits (immediate) + VIM correct − passes (immediate) + VIM correct hits (delay) + VIM correct − passes (delay))
Deltan/R
NVRTReasoningReasoning score
(NVRT correct responses − NVRT errors)
Deltan/R
SATShiftingExecutive function score
(SAT correct responses − SAT errors)
Delta+‡
FTT, SDCProcessing speed Psychomotor speed score
(FTT right response + FTT left response + SDC corrects)
Deltan/R
ST, SAT, CPTAttentionComplex Attention score
(ST errors + SAT errors + CPT errors + CPT correct-passes)
Delta+
ST, SATInhibitionCognitive Flexibility score
(SAT correct responses − SAT error − ST error)
Delta+
VBM, VIM, FTT, SDC, ST, SAT, CPTGlobal cognitionNeurocognitive Index (NCI)
(Average of following five scores; Composite Memory, Psychomotor Speed, Reaction Time, Complex Attention, and Cognitive Flexibility)
Deltan/R
Johnson; 2008; USAOriginal test batteryVerbal FluencyShiftingNumber of words from a category during a 1-minStudent’s paired t-testValue+†(L)
Digit Span Forward and BackwardShort-term memory/working memoryForward digit span lengthValue0
Forward digit span totalValue0
Backward digit span lengthValue0
Backward digit span totalValue0
Shopping List TaskEpisodic memory (verbal stimuli)Trial 1 items recalled (max. 10)Value0
Trials to learn list (max. 4)Value0
Delayed recall (max. 10)Value0
Word List Memory Test (computer version)Episodic memory (verbal stimuli)Trial 1 items recalled (max. 10)Value0
Trials to learn list (max. 6)Value0
Delayed recall (max. 10)Value0
Memory in Reality (MIR) Apartment TestEpisodic memory (visual stimuli)Delayed recall (max. 10)Value0
Location recall (max. 10)Value0
NES2 Pattern Comparison TestProcessing speedNumber correct (max.15)Value0
Mean response time-correct (s)Value-
Stroop TestProcessing speedMean RT, read words-black (ms) Value0
Processing speedMean RT, read words-color (ms) Value0
Processing speedMean RT, name colors (ms) Value0
InhibitionMean RT, name color-words (ms) Value0
InhibitionTotal RT, interference (NC-C) (s) Value0
Katagiri; 2012; JapanCogHealthSimple reaction testProcessing speedResponse time (ms) Two-way factorial ANOVA, adjusted for age and sex (between-group)

One-way repeated measure ANOVA, adjusted for age and sex (inter-group)

Bonferroni correction (multiple comparisons)
Value0
Choice reaction testResponse time (ms) Value0
Working memory testShort-term memory/working memoryResponse time (ms) Value+†(HAx)
Accuracy (%)Value0
Delayed recall testEpisodic memory (visual stimuli)Response time (ms) Value0
Accuracy (%)Value+†(HAx)
Divided attention testAttentionResponse time (ms) Value0
GMLTGMLT testShort-term memory/working memory Total duration (s)Value+†(HAx, LAx, P)
Total errorsValue+†(HAx, LAx, P)
Hayashi; 2018; JapanOriginal test batteryWord memory testShort-term memory/working memoryImmediate recallTwo-way repeated measures ANOVADelta0
Immediate recall + cued recallDelta0
Episodic memory (verbal stimuli)Recall after 5 minDelta+*(age < 55)
Recall after 5 min + cued recallDelta0
Verbal fluency testShiftingNames of vegetablesDelta0
Words that begin with “a”Delta0
Animal wordsDelta0
Stroop testProcessing speedStroop test Step 1Delta0
InhibitionStroop test Step 2Delta0
Processing speedStroop test Step 3Delta0
InhibitionStroop test Step 4Delta0
Note: CANTAB: Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery. WTAR: Wechsler Test of Adult Reading. CNSVS: CNS Vital Signs testing platform. GMLT: Groton Maze Leaning Test. MOT: Motor screening task. AST: Attention switching task. VRM: Verbal recognition memory. PAL: Paired associated learning. VBM: Verbal memory test. VIM: Visual memory test. NVRT: Non-verbal reasoning test. SAT: Shifting attention test. FTT: Finger tapping test. SDC: Symbol-digit coding test. ST: Stroop test CNSVS ver. CPT: Continuous performance task. rANOVA: Repeated measures analyses of variance. RCI: Reliable Change Index. ANOVA: analyses of variance. Value: Measured values were used to detect intervention effects. Delta: Change values between baseline and various measurement points were used to detect intervention effects. HA: High Astaxanthin group. LA: Low Astaxanthin group. P: Placebo group. n/R: Not Reported. +: there is a significant effect in comparison between groups. +*: there is a significant effect in subgroup analysis. +†: there is a significant effect in comparison between baseline and after intervention measure. +‡: there is a trend in between groups. 0: No change between placebo group and intervention group. −: the control group improved cognitive functions compared to the target intervention group.
Table 3. Correlation between serum lutein (SL) or macular pigment (MP) and cognitive function.
Table 3. Correlation between serum lutein (SL) or macular pigment (MP) and cognitive function.
Cognitive FunctionLead Author; Year; CountryMeasurement Indices (Task Name)Correlation with Task
(p-Value)
Task Score, Mean ± SD
(Pre) [Post]{Change}
Observed EffectSMD [95%CI]
(Bigger Number Shows Active Favors)
SLMPActivePlaceboStatistical Method (p-Value)P pre vs. A pre
(p-Value)
P Post vs. A Post
(p-Value)
P Change vs. A Change
(p-Value)
Episodic memory
(visual stimuli)
Power; 2018; IrelandPAL memory score
(PAL, CANTAB)
r = 0.159
(p = 0.226)
r = 0.219
(p = 0.078)
(18.91 ± 4.96)
[20.77 ± 4.57]
{1.86 ± 4.72}
(21.26 ± 3.52)
[20.32 ± 4.57]
{−0.94 ± 3.48}
rANOVA
(time effect p = 0.376, time × group effect p = 0.009)
0.55
[0.05, 1.04]
(p = 0.03)
0.10
[−0.39, 0.58]
(p = 0.69)
0.66
[1.16, 0.16]
(p = 0.009)
PAL total errors
(PAL, CANTAB)
n/Rn/R(21.50 ± 28.20)
[n/R]
{n/R}
(17.22 ± 16.93)
[n/R]
{n/R}
rANOVA
(n.S.)
−0.18
[−0.59, 0.23]
(p = 0.38)
--
PAL errors adjusted for stage 6
(PAL, CANTAB)
r = −0.346
(p = 0.006)
r = −0.342
(p = 0.005)
(6.78 ± 7.10)
[3.17 ± 4.52]
{−3.61 ± 7.01}
(4.19 ± 3.82)
[4.48 ± 4.89]
{0.29 ± 5.79}
rANOVA
(time effect p = 0.040, time × group effect p = 0.017)
−0.44
[−0.93, 0.05]
(p = 0.08)
0.28
[−0.21, 0.76]
(p = 0.28)
0.60
[0.10, 1.09]
(p = 0.02)
Renzi-Hammond; 2017; USAVisual memory (VIM, CNSVS)n/Rn/R(n/R)
[n/R]
{9.43 ± n/R}
(n/R)
[n/R]
{4.93 ± n/R}
RCI
(RCI active = 6.77 vs. RCI placebo = 1.88, p < 0.04)
---
Hammond; 2017; USAVisual memory (VIM, CNSVS)n/Rr = 0.24
(p = 0.09, in A only)
(41.03 ± 6.68)
[n/R]
{n/R}
(43.87 ± 5.18)
[n/R]
{n/R}
RCI
(n.S.)
---
Johnson; 2008; USADelayed recall (MIR apartment test)−0.16
(n.S.)
n/R(8.3 ± 1.6)
[8.6 ± 2.1]
{n/R}
(9.3 ± 0.8)
[9.4 ± 0.7]
{n/R}
Student’s paired t-test
(n.S.)
0.75
[−0.15, 1.64]
(p = 0.10)
0.48
[−0.39, 1.35]
(p = 0.28)
-
Location recall (MIR apartment test)n/Rn/R(9.5 ± 1.0)
[9.5 ± 0.8]
{n/R}
(9.7 ± 0.7)
[9.7 ± 0.7]
{n/R}
0.22
[−0.64, 1.08]
(p = 0.62)
0.25
[−0.61, 1.12]
(p = 0.56)
-
Episodic memory
(verbal stimuli)
Power; 2018; IrelandVRM Trial 1 immediate free recall (VRM, CANTAB)n/Rn/R(8.17 ± 1.75)
[8.69 ± 1.67]
{0.52 ± 1.68}
(8.57 ± 1.59)
[8.73 ± 1.84]
{0.17 ± 1.58}
rANOVA
(n.S.)
−0.24
[−0.72, 0.25]
(p = 0.34)
−0.02
[−0.51, 0.46]
(p = 0.93)
0.21
[−0.27, 0.70]
(p = 0.39)
VRM Trial 2 immediate free recall (VRM, CANTAB)n/Rn/R(9.83 ± 1.48)
[10.11 ± 2.30]
{0.28 ± 1.67}
(10.10 ± 1.24)
[10.60 ± 1.57]
{0.50 ± 1.80}
−0.19
[−0.68, 0.29]
(p = 0.43)
−0.24
[−0.73, 0.24]
(p = 0.33)
−0.13
[−0.61, 0.36]
(p = 0.61)
VRM Trial 3 immediate free recall (VRM, CANTAB)n/Rn/R(10.71 ± 1.72)
[10.51 ± 2.13]
{−0.20 ± 1.35}
(10.80 ± 1.10)
[11.03 ± 1.13]
{0.23 ± 1.17}
−0.06
[−0.55, 0.43]
(p = 0.81)
−0.29
[−0.79, 0.20]
(p = 0.24)
−0.33
[−0.83, 0.16]
(p = 0.18)
VRM Total immediate free recall (VRM, CANTAB)n/Rn/R(28.67 ± 4.51)
[29.03 ± 5.52]
{0.36 ± 3.22}
(29.47 ± 3.17)
[30.37 ± 3.77]
{0.90 ± 3.25}
−0.2
[−0.69, 0.29]
(p = 0.42)
−0.28
[−0.76, 0.21]
(p = 0.27)
−0.17
[−0.65, 0.32]
(p = 0.51)
VRM Trial 1 intrusion errors (VRM, CANTAB)n/Rn/R(0.11 ± 0.32)
[0.11 ± 0.32]
{0 ± 0.48}
(0.17 ± 0.46)
[0.20 ± 0.55]
{0.03 ± 0.49}
−0.15
[−0.64, 0.33]
(p = 0.54)
−0.20
[−0.69, 0.28]
(p = 0.54)
−0.06
[−0.55, 0.42]
(p = 0.80)
VRM Trial 2 intrusion errors (VRM, CANTAB)n/Rn/R(0.08 ± 0.28)
[0.06 ± 0.23]
{−0.02 ± 0.38}
(0.03 ± 0.18)
[0.20 ± 0.48]
{0.17 ± 0.46}
0.21
[−0.28, 0.69]
(p = 0.41)
−0.38
[−0.87, 0.11]
(p = 0.13)
−0.45
[−0.94, 0.04]
(p = 0.07)
VRM Trial 3 intrusion errors (VRM, CANTAB)n/Rn/R(0.06 ± 0.23)
[0 ± 0]
{−0.06 ± 0.23}
(0.07 ± 0.36)
[0.13 ± 0.50]
{0.06 ± 0.63}
−0.03
[−0.52, 0.45]
(p = 0.89)
-−0.26
[−0.75, 0.23]
(p = 0.30)
VRM Learning slope (VRM, CANTAB)n/Rn/R(2.54 ± 2.09)
[1.89 ± 1.84]
{−0.65 ± 1.89}
(2.20 ± 1.32)
[2.30 ± 1.64]
{0.10 ± 1.63}
0.20
[−0.29, 0.68]
(p = 0.43)
−0.23
[−0.72, 0.26]
(p = 0.35)
−0.42
[−0.92, 0.07]
(p = 0.09)
VRM delayed free recall (VRM, CANTAB)n/Rn/R(9.07 ± 3.25)
[9.92 ± 1.72]
{0.85 ± 3.22}
(10.04 ± 1.43)
[10.08 ± 2.73]
{0.04 ± 2.18}
−0.38
[−0.94, 0.19]
(p = 0.19)
−0.07
[−0.62, 0.49]
(p = 0.81)
0.29
[−0.27, 0.85]
(p = 0.31)
VRM delayed intrusion errors (VRM, CANTAB)r = −0.189
(p = 0.220)
r = −0.306
(p = 0.033)
(0.12 ± 0.33)
[0.04 ± 0.20]
{−0.08 ± 0.39}
(0.00 ± 0.00)
[0.21 ± 0.51]
{0.21 ± 0.51}
rANOVA
(time effect p = 0.309, time × group effect p = 0.030)
−0.5
[−1.06, 0.07]
(p = 0.08)
0.44
[−0.12, 1.00]
(p = 0.13)
0.63
[0.06, 1.20]
(p = 0.03)
VRM delayed recognition total (VRM, CANTAB)n/Rn/R(23.85 ± 0.36)
[23.82 ± 0.52]
{−0.03 ± 0.52}
(23.83 ± 0.38)
[23.87 ± 0.43]
{0.04 ± 0.49}
rANOVA
(n.S.)
0.05
[−0.44, 0.54]
(p = 0.83)
−0.1
[−0.59, 0.39]
(p = 0.68)
−0.14
[−0.63, 0.35]
(p = 0.59)
VRM recognition false positives (VRM, CANTAB)n/Rn/R(0.03 ± 0.17)
[0.09 ± 0.38]
{0.06 ± 0.34}
(0.10 ± 0.31)
[0.07 ± 0.37]
{−0.03 ± 0.32}
0.27
[−0.22, 0.76]
(p = 0.29)
0.05
[−0.44, 0.54]
(p = 0.83)
−0.28
[−0.78, 0.21]
(p = 0.26)
Lindberg; 2018; USAWechsler memory scale paired associates learning testn/Rn/R(8.87 ± 1.50)
[8.80 ± 2.16]
{n/R}
(9.36 ± 0.75)
[8.21 ± 2.29]
{n/R}
t-test, 2 × 2 mixed design ANOVA (n.S.)−0.37
[−1.01, 0.27]
(p = 0.26)
0.26
[−0.37,0.90]
(p = 0.42)
-
Renzi-Hammond; 2017; USAVerbal memory (VEM, CNSVS)n/Rn/R(n/R)
[n/R]
{n/R}
(n/R)
[n/R]
{n/R}
RCI
(n.S.)
---
Hammond; 2017; USAVerbal memory (VEM, CNSVS)n/Rr = 0.31
(p = 0.07, “MPOD increaser”)
(49.91 ± 5.66)
[n/R]
{n/R}
(52.67 ± 5.29)
[n/R]
{n/R}
RCI
(n.S.)
−0.49
[−1.10, 0.12]
(p = 0.12)
--
Johnson; 2008; USATrial 1 items recalled (max. 10) (Shopping List Task)n/Rn/R(6.9 ± 1.8)
[6.5 ± 2.1]
{n/R}
(6.5 ± 1.2)
[7.7 ± 1.5]
{n/R}
Student’s paired t-test
(n.S.)
0.25
[−0.61, 1.11]
(p = 0.57)
−0.63
[−1.51, 0.26]
(p = 0.16)
-
Trials to learn list (max. 6) (Shopping List Task)0.30
(p < 0.05)
n/R(4.2 ± 1.5)
[3.9 ± 1.4]
{n/R}
(3.0 ± 0.8)
[2.8 ± 0.9]
{n/R}
−0.94
[−1.86, −0.03]
(p = 0.04)
−0.89
[−1.79, 0.02]
(p = 0.06)
-
Delayed recall (max. 10) (Shopping List Task)n/Rn/R(8.3 ± 1.9)
[7.6 ± 3.0]
{n/R}
(9.5 ± 0.9)
[9.5 ± 0.7]
{n/R}
0.25
[−0.61, 1.11]
(p = 0.09)
−0.82
[−1.72, 0.08]
(p = 0.07)
-
Trial 1 items recalled (max. 10) (Word List Memory Test)n/Rn/R(5.8 ± 1.8)
[5.8 ± 1.8]
{n/R}
(6.2 ± 1.3)
[6.6 ± 1.8]
{n/R}
−0.24
[−1.10, 0.62]
(p = 0.58)
−0.43
[−1.29, 0.44]
(p = 0.34)
-
Trials to learn list (max. 4) (Word List Memory Test)−0.04
(n.S.)
n/R(3.4 ± 0.7)
[3.5 ± 0.8]
{n/R}
(3.1 ± 0.9)
[2.8 ± 0.9]
{n/R}
−0.36
[−1.22, 0.51]
(p = 0.42)
−0.79
[−1.69, 0.10]
(p = 0.08)
-
Delayed recall (max. 10) (Word List Memory Test)n/Rn/R(6.8 ± 2.9)
[7.6 ± 2.4]
{n/R}
(8.1 ± 1.1)
[8.3 ± 1.8]
{n/R}
−0.56
[−1.43, 0.32]
(p = 0.21)
−0.31
[−1.18, 0.55]
(p = 0.48)
-
Short-term memory/working memoryJohnson; 2008; USAForward digit span lengthn/Rn/R(6.6 ± 1.2)
[7.0 ± 1.5]
{n/R}
(7.2 ± 1.2)
[7.2 ± 1.4]
{n/R}
Student’s paired t-test
(n.S.)
−0.48
[−1.35, 0.39]
(p = 0.28)
−0.13
[−0.99, 0.73]
(p = 0.76)
-
Forward digit span totaln/Rn/R(8.1 ± 2.3)
[8.7 ± 2.5]
{n/R}
(9.7 ± 2.5)
[9.0 ± 2.4]
{n/R}
−0.64
[−1.52, 0.24]
(p = 0.28)
−0.12
[−0.97, 0.74]
(p = 0.79)
-
Backward digit span lengthn/Rn/R(5.1 ± 1.6)
[4.7 ± 1.4]
{n/R}
(5.9 ± 1.4)
[5.8 ± 1.7]
{n/R}
−0.51
[−1.38, 0.36]
(p = 0.25)
−0.68
[−1.57, 0.20]
(p = 0.13)
-
Backward digit span totaln/Rn/R(7.5 ± 3.1)
[6.9 ± 2.7]
{n/R}
(8.2 ± 2.7)
[8.4 ± 3.3]
{n/R}
−0.23
[−1.09, 0.63]
(p = 0.60)
−0.48
[−1.35, 0.39]
(p = 0.28)
-
ReasoningRenzi-Hammond; 2017; USAReasoning (NVRT, CNSVS)n/Rn/R(n/R)
[n/R]
{n/R}
(n/R)
[n/R]
{n/R}
RCI
(RCI “MPOD increase” = 1.94 vs. RCI “no change” = 0.18, p < 0.05*)
---
Hammond; 2017; USAReasoning (NVRT, CNSVS)n/Rr = 0.45
(p = 0.04)
(2.97 ± 3.95) [n/R]
{n/R}
(2.47 ± 3.83) [n/R]
{n/R}
RCI
(n.S.)
0.13
[−0.48, 0.73]
(p = 0.68)
--
AttentionRenzi-Hammond; 2017; USAComplex attention (ST & SAT & CPT, CNSVS)n/Rn/R(n/R)
[n/R]
{n/R}
(n/R)
[n/R]
{n/R}
RCI
(RCI “MPOD increase” = 2.02 vs. RCI “no change” = 0.00, p < 0.04)
---
Hammond; 2017; USAComplex attention (ST & SAT & CPT, CNSVS)n/Rr = −0.31
(p = 0.04, in A only)
(13.12 ± 11.12)
[n/R]
{n/R}
(10.21 ± 7.21)
[n/R]
{n/R}
RCI
(RCI active = 3.71 vs. RCI placebo = 0.34, p < 0.02)
0.28
[−0.32, 0.89]
(p = 0.36)
--
InhibitionPower; 2018; IrelandAST correct latency (AST, CANTAB)n/Rn/R(832 ± 191.86)
[751.63 ± 191.70]
{−80.37 ± 132.98}
(841.41 ± 158.95)
[775.38 ± 217.58]
{−66.03 ± 167.79}
rANOVA
(time effect p < 0.001, time × group effect p = 0.695)
0.05
[−0.42,0.53]
(p = 0.83)
0.12
[−0.36,0.59]
(p = 0.83)
−0.09
[−0.57, 0.38]
(p = 0.70)
AST percent correct (AST, CANTAB)n/Rn/R(93.71 ± 8.14)
[96.84 ± 3.49]
{3.13 ± 6.94}
(93.89 ± 7.57)
[95.02 ± 7.61]
{1.13 ± 4.10}
rANOVA
(time effect p = 0.004, time × group effect p = 0.164)
−0.02
[−0.50, 0.45]
(p = 0.93)
0.31
[−0.17, 0.79]
(p = 0.20)
0.34
[−0.14, 0.82]
(p = 0.17)
AST congruency cost (AST, CANTAB)n/Rn/R(98.46 ± 103.1)
[94.43 ± 70.01]
{−4.03 ± 93.04}
(128.1 ± 99.07)
[72.74 ± 86.65]
{−55.36 ± 110.56}
rANOVA
(time effect p = 0.019, time × group effect p = 0.041)
−0.29
[−0.77, 0.19]
(p = 0.24)
0.27
[−0.20, 0.75]
(p = 0.26)
0.5
[0.02, 0.99]
(p = 0.104)
Renzi-Hammond; 2017; USACognitive flexibility (ST & SAT, CNSVS)n/Rn/R(n/R)
[n/R]
{n/R}
(n/R)
[n/R]
{n/R}
RCI
(n.S.)
---
Hammond; 2017; USACognitive flexibility (ST & SAT, CNSVS)n/Rr = 0.20
(p = 0.10)
(30.32 ± 19.09)
[n/R]
{n/R}
(35.5 ± 16.01)
[n/R]
{n/R}
RCI
(RCI active = 6.31 vs. RCI placebo = 0.84, p < 0.04)
−0.28
[−0.88, 0.33]
(p = 0.37)
--
Johnson; 2008; USAMean RT, name colors – words
(Stroop test)
n/Rn/R(1492 ± 329)
[1462 ± 221]
{n/R}
(1419 ± 308)
[1413 ± 508]
{n/R}
Student’s paired t-test
(n.S.)
−0.22
[−1.08, 0.64]
(p = 0.62)
−0.12
[−0.98, 0.74]
(p = 0.78)
-
Total RT, interference (NC-C)
(Stroop test)
n/Rn/R(24.2 ± 10.9)
[22.4 ± 7.1]
{n/R}
(25.0 ± 14.8)
[23.1 ± 22.0]
{n/R}
0.06
[−0.80, 0.92]
(p = 0.89)
0.04
[−0.81, 0.90]
(p = 0.92)
-
ShiftingPower; 2018; IrelandPhonemic (letter) fluency (FAS test)n/Rn/R(44.44 ± 15.49)
[50.11 ± 15.55]
{5.67 ± 7.85}
(40.23 ± 11.84)
[45.93 ± 10.91]
{5.70 ± 7.88}
rANOVA
(time effect p < 0.001, time × group effect p = 0.986)
0.30
[−0.19, 0.79]
(p = 0.23)
0.30
[−0.18, 0.79]
(p = 0.22)
0.00
[−0.49, 0.48]
(p = 0.99)
Semantic (category) fluency (Animal test)n/Rn/R(23.72 ± 6.96)
[24.25 ± 6.97]
{0.53 ± 5.20}
(19.87 ± 3.86)
[21.30 ± 3.66]
{1.43 ± 3.57}
rANOVA
(n.S.)
0.7
[0.28, 1.13]
(p = 0.001)
0.51
[0.02, 1.00]
(p = 0.04)
−0.2
[−0.68, 0.29]
(p = 0.43)
Renzi-Hammond; 2017; USAExecutive function (SAT, CNSVS)n/Rn/R(n/R)
[n/R]
{n/R}
(n/R)
[n/R]
{n/R}
RCI
(n.S.)
---
Hammond; 2017; USAExecutive function (SAT, CNSVS)n/Rn/R(32.33 ± 18.33)
[n/R]
{n/R}
(35.87 ± 15.90)
[n/R]
{n/R}
RCI
(RCI active = 5.64 vs. RCI placebo = 1.27, p = 0.07)
−0.2
[−0.80, 0.41]
(p = 0.52)
--
Johnson; 2008; USAVerbal Fluency (Verbal Fluency)0.03
(n.S.)
n/R(11.3 ± 5.1)
[15.5 ± 5.5]
{n/R}
(12.9 ± 6.2)
[13.8 ± 3.5]
{n/R}
Student’s paired t-test
(p < 0.05)
−0.27
[−1.13, 0.59]
(p = 0.54)
0.35
[−0.51,1.21]
(p = 0.43)
-
Processing speedPower; 2018; IrelandMean latency
(CANTAB, MOT)
n/Rn/R(n/R)
[n/R]
{n/R}
(n/R)
[n/R]
{n/R}
rANOVA
(n.S.)
---
Mean total correct
(CANTAB, MOT)
n/Rn/R(n/R)
[n/R]
{n/R}
(n/R)
[n/R]
{n/R}
---
Mean total errors
(CANTAB, MOT)
n/Rn/R(n/R)
[n/R]
{n/R}
(n/R)
[n/R]
{n/R}
---
Renzi-Hammond; 2017; USAPsychomotor speed score
(CNSVS, FTT+SDC)
n/Rn/R(n/R)
[n/R]
{n/R}
(n/R)
[n/R]
{n/R}
RCI
(n.S.)
---
Hammond; 2017; USAPsychomotor speed score
(CNSVS, FTT+SDC)
n/Rn/R(145.5 ± 19.37)
[n/R]
{n/R}
(140.2 ± 20.11)
[n/R]
{n/R}
RCI
(n.S.)
0.27
[−0.34, 0.87]
(p = 0.39)
--
Johnson; 2008; USANumber correct (max. 15)
(NES2 Pattern Comparison Test)
n/Rn/R(14.5 ± 0.9)
[14.3 ± 1.8]
{n/R}
(14.5 ± 0.7)
[14.9 ± 0.3]
{n/R}
Student’s paired t-test
(n.S.)
0.00
[−0.86, 0.86]
(p = 1.00)
−0.44
[−1.30, 0.43]
(p = 0.33)
-
Mean response time-correct (s)
(NES2 Pattern Comparison Test)
n/Rn/R(6.1 ± 2.3)
[6.4 ± 2.3]
{n/R}
(6.8 ± 3.0)
[5.9 ± 2.3]
{n/R}
−0.25
[−1.11, 0.61]
(p = 0.33)
0.21
[−0.65, 1.07]
(p = 0.63)
-
Stroop test Mean RT, read words-black (ms) n/Rn/R(844 ± 239)
[945 ± 185]
{n/R}
(1040 ± 380)
[891 ± 222]
{n/R}
−0.6
[−1.48, 0.28]
(p = 0.33)
0.25
[−0.61, 1.12]
(p = 0.33)
-
Stroop test Mean RT, read words-color (ms)n/Rn/R(753 ± 210)
[883 ± 213]
{n/R}
(788 ± 200)
[804 ± 202]
{n/R}
−0.16
[−1.02, 0.69]
(p = 0.33)
0.36
[−0.50, 1.23]
(p = 0.33)
-
Stroop test Mean RT, name colors (ms)n/Rn/R(1008 ± 217)
[1014 ± 193]
{n/R}
(919 ± 173)
[951 ± 220]
{n/R}
0.43
[−0.44, 1.30]
(p = 0.33)
0.29
[−0.57, 1.16]
(p = 0.33)
-
Note: CANTAB: Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery. WTAR: Wechsler Test of Adult Reading. CNSVS: CNS Vital Signs testing platform, GMLT: Groton Maze Learning Test. MOT: Motor screening task. AST: Attention switching task. VRM: Verbal recognition memory. PAL: Paired associated learning. VBM: Verbal memory test. VIM: Visual memory test. NVRT: Non-verbal reasoning test. SAT: Shifting attention test. FTT: Finger tapping test. SDC: Symbol-digit coding test. ST: Stroop test. CNSVS ver. CPT: Continuous performance task. rANOVA: Repeated measures analyses of variance. RCI: Reliable Change Index. ANOVA: analyses of variance. P: Placebo control group. A: Active intervention group. HA: High Astaxanthin group. LA: Low Astaxanthin group. P: Placebo group. n/R: Not reported. n.S.: Not significant (this abbreviation was used if a significantly low p-value was not shown in the paper).
Table 4. Quality assessment scores of included studies using modified Delphi list.
Table 4. Quality assessment scores of included studies using modified Delphi list.
Lead Author; Year; CountryQ1Q2Q3Q4Q5Q6Q7Q8Q9Q10Q11Q12Q13Q14Total score
(Max. = 14)
Power; 2018; IrelandYYNYYYYY?YYY?N10
Lindberg; 2018; USAYY?YYYY??YNYYN9
Renzi-Hammond; 2017; USAYY?YYYY?NYNYYN9
Hammond; 2017; USAYYYYYYY?NYNYYN10
Johnson; 2008; USAYYYYYYYY?NNNYN9
Katagiri; 2012; JapanYYYYYYYYNNYYYN11
Hayashi; 2018; JapanY?YYYYYYNNYYYN10
Total score across studies76477774043660
Note: Q1: Random Allocation. Q2: Treatment Allocation concealed. Q3: Groups/Subjects similar at baseline regarding important prognostic values. Q4: Eligibility Criteria specified. Q5: Blinded outcome assessor. Q6: Care Provider blinded. Q7: Patient blinded. Q8: Point Estimates and Measures of Variability presented for the primary outcome measures. Q9. Intention-to-treat Analysis. Q10: details of Random Allocation methods. Q11: Adequate Description of the Control/Comparison group. Q12: Between-group statistical comparison. Q13: reporting Dropouts. Q14: reporting CONSORT statement. Y: Yes, the study met the criteria of the question. N: No; the study did not meet the criteria of the question? No information or the study was not the case with the question.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Nouchi, R.; Suiko, T.; Kimura, E.; Takenaka, H.; Murakoshi, M.; Uchiyama, A.; Aono, M.; Kawashima, R. Effects of Lutein and Astaxanthin Intake on the Improvement of Cognitive Functions among Healthy Adults: A Systematic Review of Randomized Controlled Trials. Nutrients 2020, 12, 617. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12030617

AMA Style

Nouchi R, Suiko T, Kimura E, Takenaka H, Murakoshi M, Uchiyama A, Aono M, Kawashima R. Effects of Lutein and Astaxanthin Intake on the Improvement of Cognitive Functions among Healthy Adults: A Systematic Review of Randomized Controlled Trials. Nutrients. 2020; 12(3):617. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12030617

Chicago/Turabian Style

Nouchi, Rui, Takahiko Suiko, Eriko Kimura, Hiroki Takenaka, Michiaki Murakoshi, Akira Uchiyama, Megumi Aono, and Ryuta Kawashima. 2020. "Effects of Lutein and Astaxanthin Intake on the Improvement of Cognitive Functions among Healthy Adults: A Systematic Review of Randomized Controlled Trials" Nutrients 12, no. 3: 617. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12030617

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop