Untangling the Additive and Multiplicative Relations between Natural Scenery Exposure and Human–Animal Interaction on Affective Well-Being: Evidence from Daily Diary Studies
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Human–Animal Interaction as Part of Nature
1.2. Current Study
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Sample
2.2. Measures
2.3. Analytic Plan
Natural Scenery Exposure | |
Level 1 | (Daily well-being)di = B0i + B1i(daily natural scenery exposure)di + εdi |
Level 2 | B0i = γ00 + γ01(average natural scenery exposure)i + μ0i B1i = γ10 + μ1i |
Human–Animal Interaction | |
Level 1 | (Daily well-being)di = B0i + B1i(daily human–animal interaction)di + εdi |
Level 2 | B0i = γ00 + γ01(average human–animal interaction)i + μ0i B1i = γ10 + μ1i |
Additive Model | |
Level 1 | (Daily well-being)di = B0i + B1i(daily natural scenery exposure)di + B2i(daily human–animal interaction)di + εdi |
Level 2 | B0i = γ00 + γ01(average natural scenery exposure)i + γ02(average human–animal interaction)i + μ0i B1i = γ10 + μ1i B2ii = γ20 + μ2i |
Multiplicative Model | |
Level 1 | (Daily well-being)di = B0i + B1i(daily natural scenery exposure)di + B2i(daily human–animal interaction)di + B3i(daily natural scenery exposure × daily human–animal interaction)di + εdi |
Level 2 | B0ii = γ00 + γ01(average natural scenery exposure)i + γ02(average human–animal interaction)i + γ03(average natural scenery exposure × average human–animal interaction)i + μ0i B1ii = γ10 + μ1i B2ii = γ20 + μ2i B3ii = γ30 |
2.4. Transparency and Openness
3. Results
3.1. Natural Scenery and Affective Well-Being
3.2. Human–Animal Interaction and Affective Well-Being
3.3. Additive Associations with Well-Being
3.4. Multiplicative Associations with Well-Being
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Samus, A.; Freeman, C.; van Heezik, Y.; Krumme, K.; Dickinson, K.J. How do urban green spaces increase well-being? The role of perceived wildness and nature connectedness. J. Environ. Psychol. 2022, 82, 101850. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buijs, A.; Jacobs, M. Avoiding negativity bias: Towards a positive psychology of human–wildlife relationships. AMBIO 2020, 50, 281–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bakolis, I.; Hammoud, R.; Smythe, M.; Gibbons, J.; Davidson, N.; Tognin, S.; Mechelli, A. Urban Mind: Using Smartphone Technologies to Investigate the Impact of Nature on Mental Well-Being in Real Time. Bioscience 2018, 68, 134–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- MacKerron, G.; Mourato, S. Happiness is greater in natural environments. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2013, 23, 992–1000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baceviciene, M.; Jankauskiene, R. The Mediating Effect of Nature Restorativeness, Stress Level, and Nature Connectedness in the Association between Nature Exposure and Quality of Life. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 2098. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Martin, L.; Pahl, S.; White, M.P.; May, J. Natural environments and craving: The mediating role of negative affect. Health Place 2019, 58, 102160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- White, M.P.; Alcock, I.; Wheeler, B.W.; Depledge, M.H. Would You Be Happier Living in a Greener Urban Area? A Fixed-Effects Analysis of Panel Data. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 920–928. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anderson, A.R. Enjoying nature, exercise, social interaction, and affect: A daily diary study. J. Health Psychol. 2020, 27, 890–900. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jiang, B.; He, J.; Chen, J.; Larsen, L.; Wang, H. Perceived Green at Speed: A Simulated Driving Experiment Raises New Questions for Attention Restoration Theory and Stress Reduction Theory. Environ. Behav. 2020, 53, 296–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Han, K.-T. Effects of Three Levels of Green Exercise, Physical and Social Environments, Personality Traits, Physical Activity, and Engagement with Nature on Emotions and Attention. Sustainability 2021, 13, 2686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jones, R.J.F.; Littzen, C.O.R. An Analysis of Theoretical Perspectives in Research on Nature-Based Interventions and Pain. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 12740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Memari, S.; Pazhouhanfar, M.; Nourtaghani, A. Relationship between perceived sensory dimensions and stress restoration in care settings. Urban For. Urban Green. 2017, 26, 104–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jiang, B.; Chang, C.-Y.; Sullivan, W.C. A dose of nature: Tree cover, stress reduction, and gender differences. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2014, 132, 26–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berto, R. The Role of Nature in Coping with Psycho-Physiological Stress: A Literature Review on Restorativeness. Behav. Sci. 2014, 4, 394–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ulrich, R.S.; Simons, R.F.; Losito, B.D.; Fiorito, E.; Miles, M.A.; Zelson, M. Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 1991, 11, 201–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beetz, A.; Uvnäs-Moberg, K.; Julius, H.; Kotrschal, K. Psychosocial and Psychophysiological Effects of Human-Animal Interactions: The Possible Role of Oxytocin. Front. Psychol. 2012, 3, 234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nagasawa, M.; Kikusui, T.; Onaka, T.; Ohta, M. Dog’s gaze at its owner increases owner’s urinary oxytocin during social interaction. Horm. Behav. 2008, 55, 434–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Odendaal, J.; Meintjes, R. Neurophysiological Correlates of Affiliative Behaviour between Humans and Dogs. Veter. J. 2003, 165, 296–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wells, D.L. The State of Research on Human–Animal Relations: Implications for Human Health. Anthrozoös 2019, 32, 169–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Odendaal, J.S.J. Animal-assisted therapy––Magic or medicine? J. Psychosom. Res. 2000, 49, 275–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carter, C.S. Oxytocin Pathways and the Evolution of Human Behavior. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014, 65, 17–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wells, D.L.; Clements, M.A.; Elliott, L.J.; Meehan, E.S.; Montgomery, C.J.; Williams, G.A. Quality of the Human–Animal Bond and Mental Wellbeing During a COVID-19 Lockdown. Anthrozoös 2022, 35, 847–866. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kogan, L.; Currin-McCulloch, J.; Bussolari, C.; Packman, W.; Erdman, P. The Psychosocial Influence of Companion Animals on Positive and Negative Affect during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Animals 2021, 11, 2084. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Clements, H.; Valentin, S.; Jenkins, N.; Rankin, J.; Gee, N.; Snellgrove, D.; Sloman, K. Companion Animal Type and Level of Engagement Matter: A Mixed-Methods Study Examining Links between Companion Animal Guardianship, Loneliness and Well-Being during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Animals 2021, 11, 2349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Applebaum, J.W.; Ellison, C.; Struckmeyer, L.; Zsembik, B.A.; McDonald, S.E. The Impact of Pets on Everyday Life for Older Adults During the COVID-19 Pandemic. Front. Public Health 2021, 9, 652610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keniger, L.E.; Gaston, K.J.; Irvine, K.N.; Fuller, R.A. What are the Benefits of Interacting with Nature? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2013, 10, 913–935. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sumner, R.C.; Goodenough, A.E. A walk on the wild side: How interactions with non-companion animals might help reduce human stress. People Nat. 2020, 2, 395–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Franco, L.S.; Shanahan, D.F.; Fuller, R.A. A Review of the Benefits of Nature Experiences: More Than Meets the Eye. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scollon, C.N.; Kim-Prieto, C. Experience Sampling: Promises and Pitfalls, Strengths and Weaknesses. J. Happiness Stud. 2003, 4, 5–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Majeed, N.M.; Tan, J.J.; Tov, W.; Hartanto, A. Dispositional optimism as a buffer against emotional reactivity to daily stressors: A daily diary approach. J. Res. Pers. 2021, 93, 104105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hartanto, A.; Lee, K.Y.X.; Chua, Y.J.; Quek, F.Y.X.; Majeed, N.M. Smartphone use and daily cognitive failures: A critical examination using a daily diary approach with objective smartphone measures. Br. J. Psychol. 2022, 114, 70–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Almeida, D.M. Resilience and Vulnerability to Daily Stressors Assessed via Diary Methods. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2005, 14, 64–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hartanto, A.; Wong, J.; Lua, V.Y.Q.; Tng, G.Y.Q.; Kasturiratna, K.T.A.S.; Majeed, N.M. A Daily Diary Investigation of the Fear of Missing Out and Diminishing Daily Emotional Well-Being: The Moderating Role of Cognitive Reappraisal. Psychol. Rep. 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stone, A.A.; Shiffman, S. Capturing momentary, self-report data: A proposal for reporting guidelines. Ann. Behav. Med. 2002, 24, 236–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ulrich, R.S. Natural Versus Urban Scenes: Some Psychophysiological Effects. Environ. Behav. 1981, 13, 523–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lua, V.Y.Q.; Majeed, N.M.; Leung, A.K.-Y.; Hartanto, A. A daily within-person investigation on the link between social expectancies to be busy and emotional wellbeing: The moderating role of emotional complexity acceptance. Cogn. Emot. 2022, 36, 773–780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ng, M.H.; Lua, V.Y.; Majeed, N.M.; Hartanto, A. Does trait self-esteem serve as a protective factor in maintaining daily affective well-being? Multilevel analyses of daily diary studies in the US and Singapore. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2022, 198, 111804. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Adler, N.E.; Epel, E.S.; Castellazzo, G.; Ickovics, J.R. Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: Preliminary data in healthy, White women. Health Psychol. 2000, 19, 586–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kamitsis, I.; Francis, A.J. Spirituality mediates the relationship between engagement with nature and psychological wellbeing. J. Environ. Psychol. 2013, 36, 136–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brim, O.G.; Featherman, D.L. Surveying midlife development in the United States. 1998; unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
- Robinson, J.P.; Andrews, F.M. Measures of subjective well-being. In Measures of Personality and Social Psychological Attitudes: Measures of Social Psychological Attitudes; Elsevier Science: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2014; pp. 61–76. [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2019; Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 10 December 2022).
- Revelle, W. psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research, R package version 2.2.5; Northwestern University: Evanston, IL, USA, 2022; Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych (accessed on 10 December 2022).
- Rosseel, Y. lavaan: AnRPackage for Structural Equation Modeling. J. Stat. Softw. 2012, 48, 1–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jorgensen, T.D.; Pornprasertmanit, S.; Schoemann, A.M.; Rosseel, Y. semTools: Useful Tools for Structural Equation Modeling, R package version 0.5-6. 2022. Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=semTools (accessed on 10 December 2022).
- Bates, D.; Mächler, M.; Bolker, B.; Walker, S. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 2015, 67, 48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuznetsova, A.; Brockhoff, P.B.; Christensen, R.H.B. lmerTest Package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. J. Stat. Softw. 2017, 82, 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ben-Shachar, M.S.; Lüdecke, D.; Makowski, D. effectsize: Estimation of Effect Size Indices and Standardized Parameters. J. Open Source Softw. 2020, 5, 2815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johansson, M.; Hartig, T.; Staats, H. Psychological Benefits of Walking: Moderation by Company and Outdoor Environment. Appl. Psychol. Health Well-Being 2011, 3, 261–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hartig, T.; Evans, G.W.; Jamner, L.D.; Davis, D.S.; Gärling, T. Tracking restoration in natural and urban field settings. J. Environ. Psychol. 2003, 23, 109–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tan, P.Y.; Wang, J.; Sia, A. Perspectives on five decades of the urban greening of Singapore. Cities 2013, 32, 24–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tan, K.W. A greenway network for singapore. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2006, 76, 45–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Douglas, I. Urban ecology and urban ecosystems: Understanding the links to human health and well-being. Curr. Opin. Environ. Sustain. 2012, 4, 385–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wyles, K.J.; White, M.P.; Hattam, C.; Pahl, S.; King, H.; Austen, M. Are Some Natural Environments More Psychologically Beneficial Than Others? The Importance of Type and Quality on Connectedness to Nature and Psychological Restoration. Environ. Behav. 2019, 51, 111–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yao, W.; Chen, F.; Wang, S.; Zhang, X. Impact of Exposure to Natural and Built Environments on Positive and Negative Affect: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Front. Public Health 2021, 9, 758457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Herzog, H.A. Gender Differences in Human–Animal Interactions: A Review. Anthrozoös 2007, 20, 7–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hartanto, A.; Quek, F.Y.X.; Tng, G.Y.Q.; Yong, J.C. Does Social Media Use Increase Depressive Symptoms? A Reverse Causation Perspective. Front. Psychiatry 2021, 12, 641934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rohrer, J.M. Thinking Clearly About Correlations and Causation: Graphical Causal Models for Observational Data. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci. 2018, 1, 27–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grosz, M.P.; Rohrer, J.M.; Thoemmes, F. The Taboo Against Explicit Causal Inference in Nonexperimental Psychology. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2020, 15, 1243–1255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Variable | M or % | SD | Observed Range | Theoretical Range |
---|---|---|---|---|
Demographics | ||||
Age (years) | 22.24 | 1.68 | 19–30 | |
Sex (% female) | 75.29% | |||
Ethnicity (% Chinese) | 80.16% | |||
Monthly household income a | 3.02 | 1.44 | 1–6 | 1–6 |
Subjective socioeconomic status b | 6.16 | 1.32 | 2–10 | 1–10 |
Daily measures | ||||
Natural scenery exposure | 4.21 | 2.03 | 2–10 | 2–10 |
Human–animal interaction (% interacted) | 15.29% | |||
Affective well-being | ||||
Perceived stress | 3.25 | 2.64 | 0–10 | 0–10 |
Negative affect | 0.56 | 0.64 | 0–4 | 0–4 |
Positive affect | 1.93 | 0.93 | 0–4 | 0–4 |
Stress | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Predictors | ß | 95% CI | Unstd. est. | SE | p |
Fixed effects | |||||
Intercept, γ00 | 3.25 | 0.09 | <0.001 | ||
Level 2 (Between-person) | |||||
Natural scenery exposure, γ01 | −0.03 | [−0.12, 0.06] | −0.03 | 0.05 | 0.548 |
Level 1 (Within-person) | |||||
Natural scenery exposure, γ10 | −0.03 | [−0.07, 0.004] | −0.05 | 0.03 | 0.083 |
Random effects | |||||
Intercept, μ0i | 3.20 | ||||
Slope of natural scenery exposure, μ1i | 0.05 | ||||
Negative Affect | |||||
Predictors | ß | 95% CI | Unstd. est. | SE | p |
Fixed effects | |||||
Intercept, γ00 | 0.56 | 0.02 | <0.001 | ||
Level 2 (Between-person) | |||||
Natural scenery exposure, γ01 | −0.04 | [−0.13, 0.05] | −0.01 | 0.01 | 0.377 |
Level 1 (Within-person) | |||||
Natural scenery exposure, γ10 | −0.03 | [−0.07, 0.004] | −0.01 | 0.007 | 0.082 |
Random effects | |||||
Intercept, μ0i | 0.21 | ||||
Slope of natural scenery exposure, μ1i | 0.004 | ||||
Positive Affect | |||||
Predictors | ß | 95% CI | Unstd. est. | SE | p |
Fixed effects | |||||
Intercept, γ00 | 1.93 | 0.03 | <0.001 | ||
Level 2 (Between-person) | |||||
Natural scenery exposure, γ01 | 0.22 | [0.13, 0.31] | 0.10 | 0.02 | <0.001 |
Level 1 (Within-person) | |||||
Natural scenery exposure, γ10 | 0.20 | [0.16, 0.24] | 0.09 | 0.009 | <0.001 |
Random effects | |||||
Intercept, μ0i | 0.48 | ||||
Slope of natural scenery exposure, μ1i | 0.006 |
Stress | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Predictors | ß | 95% CI | Unstd. est. | SE | p |
Fixed effects | |||||
Intercept, γ00 | 3.25 | 0.09 | <0.001 | ||
Level 2 (Between-person) | |||||
Human–animal interaction, γ01 | −0.05 | [−0.16, 0.06] | −0.32 | 0.34 | 0.345 |
Level 1 (Within-person) | |||||
Human–animal interaction, γ10 | −0.002 | [−0.06, 0.06] | −0.10 | 0.17 | 0.942 |
Random effects | |||||
Intercept, μ0i | 3.22 | ||||
Slope of human–animal interaction, μ1i | 0.55 | ||||
Negative Affect | |||||
Predictors | ß | 95% CI | Unstd. est. | SE | p |
Fixed effects | |||||
Intercept, γ00 | 0.57 | 0.02 | <0.001 | ||
Level 2 (Between-person) | |||||
Human–animal interaction, γ01 | 0.006 | [−0.10, 0.11] | 0.01 | 0.08 | 0.908 |
Level 1 (Within-person) | |||||
Human–animal interaction, γ10 | −0.03 | [−0.09, 0.02] | −0.04 | 0.04 | 0.250 |
Random effects | |||||
Intercept, μ0i | 0.21 | ||||
Slope of human–animal interaction, μ1i | |||||
Positive Affect | |||||
Predictors | ß | 95% CI | Unstd. est. | SE | p |
Fixed effects | |||||
Intercept, γ00 | 1.89 | 0.03 | <0.001 | ||
Level 2 (Between-person) | |||||
Human–animal interaction, γ01 | −0.01 | [−0.11, 0.09] | −0.03 | 0.12 | 0.822 |
Level 1 (Within-person) | |||||
Human–animal interaction, γ10 | 0.14 | [0.08, 0.21] | 0.24 | 0.05 | <0.001 |
Random effects | |||||
Intercept, μ0i | 0.51 | ||||
Slope of human–animal interaction, μ1i | 0.08 |
Stress | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Predictors | ß | 95% CI | Unstd. est. | SE | p |
Fixed effects | |||||
Intercept, γ00 | 3.25 | 0.09 | <0.001 | ||
Level 2 (Between-person) | |||||
Natural scenery exposure, γ01 | −0.02 | [−0.12, 0.07] | −0.02 | 0.05 | 0.648 |
Human−animal interaction, γ02 | −0.05 | [−0.16, 0.05] | −0.33 | 0.33 | 0.327 |
Level 1 (Within-person) | |||||
Natural scenery exposure, γ10 | −0.03 | [−0.07, 0.00] | −0.05 | 0.02 | 0.074 |
Human–animal interaction, γ20 | −0.003 | [−0.06, 0.06] | 0.02 | 0.17 | 0.903 |
Random effects | |||||
Intercept, μ0i | 3.23 | ||||
Slope of natural scenery exposure, μ1i | 0.049 | ||||
Slope of human–animal interaction, μ2i | 0.616 | ||||
Negative Affect | |||||
Predictors | ß | 95% CI | Unstd. est. | SE | p |
Fixed effects | |||||
Intercept, γ00 | 0.57 | 0.02 | <0.001 | ||
Level 2 (Between-person) | |||||
Natural scenery exposure, γ01 | −0.03 | [−0.13, 0.06] | −0.01 | 0.01 | 0.493 |
Human−animal interaction, γ02 | 0.008 | [−0.09, 0.11] | 0.01 | 0.08 | 0.874 |
Level 1 (Within-person) | |||||
Natural scenery exposure, γ10 | −0.03 | [−0.07, 0.005] | −0.01 | 0.007 | 0.089 |
Human–animal interaction, γ20 | −0.03 | [−0.09, 0.03] | −0.04 | 0.04 | 0.308 |
Random effects | |||||
Intercept, μ0i | 0.22 | ||||
Slope of natural scenery exposure, μ1i | 0.004 | ||||
Slope of human–animal interaction, μ2i | 0.007 | ||||
Positive Affect | |||||
Predictors | ß | 95% CI | Unstd. est. | SE | p |
Fixed effects | |||||
Intercept, γ00 | 1.90 | 0.03 | <0.001 | ||
Level 2 (Between-person) | |||||
Natural scenery exposure, γ01 | 0.20 | [0.11, 0.29] | 0.09 | 0.02 | <0.001 |
Human−animal interaction, γ02 | −0.03 | [−0.12, 0.07] | −0.06 | 0.12 | 0.595 |
Level 1 (Within-person) | |||||
Natural scenery exposure, γ10 | 0.19 | [0.15, 0.23] | 0.09 | 0.01 | <0.001 |
Human–animal interaction, γ20 | 0.12 | [0.06, 0.18] | 0.20 | 0.05 | <0.001 |
Random effects | |||||
Intercept, μ0i | 0.49 | ||||
Slope of natural scenery exposure, μ1i | 0.007 | ||||
Slope of human–animal interaction, μ2i | 0.07 |
Stress | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Predictors | ß | 95% CI | Unstd. est. | SE | p |
Fixed effects | |||||
Intercept, γ00 | 3.25 | 0.09 | <0.001 | ||
Level 2 (Between-person) | |||||
Natural scenery exposure, γ01 | −0.03 | [−0.12, 0.07] | −0.03 | 0.06 | 0.597 |
Human–animal interaction, γ02 | −0.05 | [−0.16, 0.06] | −0.30 | 0.35 | 0.385 |
Natural scenery exposure × human–animal interaction, γ03 | −0.008 | [−0.10, 0.08] | −0.03 | 0.20 | 0.868 |
Level 1 (Within-person) | |||||
Natural scenery exposure, γ10 | −0.03 | [−0.07, 0.01] | −0.04 | 0.03 | 0.127 |
Human–animal interaction, γ20 | −0.002 | [−0.06, 0.06] | −0.008 | 0.17 | 0.961 |
Natural scenery exposure × human–animal interaction, γ30 | −0.0008 | [−0.04, 0.04] | −0.003 | 0.07 | 0.968 |
Random effects | |||||
Intercept, μ0i | 3.22 | ||||
Slope of natural scenery exposure, μ1i | |||||
Slope of human–animal interaction, μ2i | 0.56 | ||||
Negative Affect | |||||
Predictors | ß | 95% CI | Unstd. est. | SE | p |
Fixed effects | |||||
Intercept, γ00 | 0.56 | 0.02 | <0.001 | ||
Level 2 (Between-person) | |||||
Natural scenery exposure, γ01 | −0.04 | [−0.13, 0.05] | −0.01 | 0.01 | 0.389 |
Human–animal interaction, γ02 | −0.009 | [−0.12, 0.10] | 0.01 | 0.08 | 0.870 |
Natural scenery exposure × human–animal interaction, γ03 | 0.07 | [−0.03, 0.16] | 0.07 | 0.05 | 0.176 |
Level 1 (Within-person) | |||||
Natural scenery exposure, γ10 | −0.04 | [−0.08, 0.01] | −0.01 | 0.007 | 0.096 |
Human–animal interaction, γ20 | −0.03 | [−0.09, 0.03] | −0.04 | 0.04 | 0.329 |
Natural scenery exposure × human–animal interaction, γ30 | 0.007 | [−0.04, 0.05] | 0.005 | 0.02 | 0.739 |
Random effects | |||||
Intercept, μ0i | 0.21 | ||||
Slope of natural scenery exposure, μ1i | 0.004 | ||||
Slope of human–animal interaction, μ2i | |||||
Positive Affect | |||||
Predictors | ß | 95% CI | Unstd. est. | SE | p |
Fixed effects | |||||
Intercept, γ00 | 1.90 | 0.034 | <0.001 | ||
Level 2 (Between-person) | |||||
Natural scenery exposure, γ01 | 0.21 | [0.12, 0.29] | 0.09 | 0.02 | <0.001 |
Human–animal interaction, γ02 | −0.02 | [−0.12, 0.08] | −0.04 | 0.12 | 0.716 |
Natural scenery exposure × human–animal interaction, γ03 | −0.04 | [−0.14, 0.06] | −0.05 | 0.07 | 0.456 |
Level 1 (Within-person) | |||||
Natural scenery exposure, γ10 | 0.19 | [0.14, 0.23] | 0.09 | 0.01 | <0.001 |
Human–animal interaction, γ20 | 0.12 | [0.06, 0.18] | 0.20 | 0.05 | <0.001 |
Natural scenery exposure × human–animal interaction, γ30 | 0.01 | [−0.03, 0.06] | 0.016 | 0.023 | 0.491 |
Random effects | |||||
Intercept, μ0i | 0.49 | ||||
Slope of natural scenery exposure, μ1i | 0.007 | ||||
Slope of human–animal interaction, μ2i | 0.07 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Goh, A.Y.H.; Chia, S.M.; Majeed, N.M.; Chen, N.R.Y.; Hartanto, A. Untangling the Additive and Multiplicative Relations between Natural Scenery Exposure and Human–Animal Interaction on Affective Well-Being: Evidence from Daily Diary Studies. Sustainability 2023, 15, 2910. https://doi.org/10.3390/su15042910
Goh AYH, Chia SM, Majeed NM, Chen NRY, Hartanto A. Untangling the Additive and Multiplicative Relations between Natural Scenery Exposure and Human–Animal Interaction on Affective Well-Being: Evidence from Daily Diary Studies. Sustainability. 2023; 15(4):2910. https://doi.org/10.3390/su15042910
Chicago/Turabian StyleGoh, Adalia Y. H., Shu Min Chia, Nadyanna M. Majeed, Nicole R. Y. Chen, and Andree Hartanto. 2023. "Untangling the Additive and Multiplicative Relations between Natural Scenery Exposure and Human–Animal Interaction on Affective Well-Being: Evidence from Daily Diary Studies" Sustainability 15, no. 4: 2910. https://doi.org/10.3390/su15042910
APA StyleGoh, A. Y. H., Chia, S. M., Majeed, N. M., Chen, N. R. Y., & Hartanto, A. (2023). Untangling the Additive and Multiplicative Relations between Natural Scenery Exposure and Human–Animal Interaction on Affective Well-Being: Evidence from Daily Diary Studies. Sustainability, 15(4), 2910. https://doi.org/10.3390/su15042910