Next Article in Journal
Using Si, Al and Fe as Tracers for Source Apportionment of Air Pollutants in Lake Baikal Snowpack
Next Article in Special Issue
Virtual Reality Nature Experiences Involving Wolves on YouTube: Presence, Emotions, and Attitudes in Immersive and Nonimmersive Settings
Previous Article in Journal
Environmental Impacts of a Pet Dog: An LCA Case Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
Addressing Sustainable Development: Promoting Active Informed Citizenry through Trans-Contextual Science Education
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Importance of Different Knowledge Types in Health-Related Decisions—The Example of Type 2 Diabetes

by
Julia Caroline Arnold
Centre for Science and Technology Education, School of Education, FHNW University of Applied Sciences and Arts Northwestern Switzerland, 4132 Muttenz, Switzerland
Sustainability 2020, 12(8), 3396; https://doi.org/10.3390/su12083396
Submission received: 20 March 2020 / Revised: 9 April 2020 / Accepted: 16 April 2020 / Published: 22 April 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Teaching Sustainable Development Goals in Science Education)

Abstract

:
Noncommunicable diseases (NCDs, e.g., type 2 diabetes) are a burden to humanity and hence addressed in the Sustainable Development Goals (SDGs) (target 3.4). One way of tackling NCDs is by health education as part of science education. Yet, the role of knowledge for health-promoting actions, and thus, the role of science teaching in health education, is not sufficiently clarified. Therefore, the author proposes to differentiate three knowledge types: System Health Knowledge (SK), Action-related Health Knowledge (AK), and Effectiveness Health Knowledge (EK). Accordingly, we designed a questionnaire that asked students to evaluate different questions about sugar consumption and type 2 diabetes according to their relevance for deciding their future sugar consumption. We found that students considered all questions as rather important (3–4.3, out of 5) with an assigned mean importance for SK with a mean of 3.8, for AK with a mean of 4.0, and for EK with a mean of 3.9. This research indicates that knowledge is important for decision-making and that all three types of knowledge should be recognized in health education.

1. Introduction

1.1. Problem

Chronic or noncommunicable diseases (NCDs, e.g., cardiovascular disease, stroke, cancer, or type 2 diabetes) are “the leading cause of mortality in the world,” and according to the World Health Organization (WHO), 80% of premature deaths attributed to NCDs could be prevented [1]. To meet this challenge, the UN Sustainable Development Goals (SDGs) include target 3.4: “By 2030, reduce by one-third premature mortality from non-communicable diseases through prevention and treatment and promote mental health and well-being” [2]. This target is specified, e.g., in indicator 3.41 “[m]ortality rate attributed to cardiovascular disease, cancer, diabetes, or chronic respiratory disease” [2]. Here, the nutritional status is crucial, since unhealthy diet (e.g., excessive sugar consumption) is a significant risk factor for NCDs [1]. Regarding type 2 diabetes, the number of people affected is increasing, especially in developed countries with western lifestyles [3], and “diabetes was the seventh leading cause of death in 2016” according to recent estimates [4]. Consequently, one of the “Grand Challenges and Opportunities for Science and Technology in the medium term Future” is the improvement of nutrition [5] (p. 8).
NCDs like type 2 diabetes have (besides genetic predispositions) physiological risk factors such as, e.g., overweight, and obesity and raised blood glucose. These physiological problems, in turn, can be favored by various behavioral risk factors, e.g., physical inactivity, and an unhealthy diet [6]. The last one is linked to the disproportional intake of fat, fruits, and vegetables as well as sugar (e.g., in sugar-sweetened beverages [7]) and alcohol. On the one hand, the reasons for these behaviors are conditioned by the environment or setting and socioeconomic status. On the other hand, individual motivational factors (e.g., attitudes, needs, values) and, in many places, the role of knowledge, are discussed as potential causes for (un)healthy behavior [8] (Figure 1).
For preventing premature mortality from NCDs, Cao et al. [9] suggest “[h]igh-level political commitments to effective and equitable national surveillance and prioritized prevention, early detection, and treatment programs tailored to the major NCD types” (p. 1288), especially in lower-resourced settings. In higher-income settings, access through prevention and education—namely health literacy—can also play a significant role. Consequently, it is essential to teach students how to make healthy decisions, especially regarding nutrition. However, how can people be encouraged to change their behavior, e.g., their diet? To find possible answers to this question, this paper focuses on the NCD “type 2 diabetes” and the risk-factor “unhealthy diet,” especially in disproportional intake of sugar, especially free sugars (e.g., in sugar-sweetened beverages [7]). High sugar consumption can influence weight-gain overweight (simply because of the excessive calorie intake), which in turn can lead to insulin resistance. On the other hand, high sugar consumption can lead to high blood glucose levels, which can also lead to insulin resistance [10,11]. Nevertheless, it should be explicitly mentioned that type 2 diabetes is multifactorial and sugar consumption does not directly cause diabetes, as shown in Figure 1.
The WHO defines health promotion as “a process of enabling people to increase control over, and to improve, their health” [12] (p. 17). One of the priorities of health promotion lies in empowering individuals, which “demands more consistent, reliable access to the decision-making process and the skills and knowledge essential to effect change” [13] (p. 17). Accordingly, this paper takes a look at what role knowledge can play for making health-related decisions in this context. Since science and health issues can be of mutual benefit [14,15,16], science education can have a significant impact on preventing NCDs like type 2 diabetes. To figure out possibilities for prevention through (school-) education, this paper looks at the issue from a science education perspective.

1.2. Theoretical Background

Science education aims at enabling learners to make competent behavioral decisions in various science-related issues, e.g., in health issues. Health education, in turn, strives for enabling learners to “make informed decisions about their future lives and health” [17] (p. 99). These decisions can relate to either individual or political behavior [18,19,20]. As a precondition for taking competent behavioral decisions on an individual level, the need for learners to develop an understanding of the concepts and principles within science is imperative [21]. Also, learners need to be able to reflect on the driving forces behind their own behavioral decisions and to recognize the role those decisions may have in the development of the abovementioned future challenges [18,22,23]. Health education has a long tradition in science and biology teaching, as scientific/biological facts, relationships, and principles play a central role in understanding organ functions and the body as a system. However, the role of knowledge for the following health-promoting action or the decision for this, and thus, the role of science and biology teaching in health education, is not yet sufficiently clarified [8].
Health action or behavior can be described as “(a)ny activity undertaken by an individual, regardless of actual or perceived health status, for the purpose of promoting, protecting or maintaining health, whether or not such behavior is objectively effective towards that end.” [24] (p. 355). Hence, health action is deliberate and, accordingly, the action is based on cognitions and decision-making processes that lead to intention and, ultimately, action [25]. Health actions can be further divided into protective behavior (e.g., exercise or diet), risk behavior (e.g., smoking or drug use), or detection behavior (e.g., screening) [26]. In the context of this study, the focus is on protective behavior (including preventive behavior), and exemplarily nutritional behavior is discussed. Recent studies show that, e.g., German adolescents consume too much sugar and salt, too many fats, and high-fat foods, drink too much alcohol, and consume too little fruit and vegetables [27,28]. These behaviors are regarded as causes of NCDs such as obesity, diabetes, cancer, and cardiovascular diseases. Accordingly, health education should find ways to positively influence such behaviors in the long term [29].
It is essential to identify conditional factors for this behavior to promote positive behavior [30]. Research in health education is influenced by two research lines, social-cognitive and behavioral research (Health Behavior Change), which focuses on motivational factors of health action, and health literacy research, which focuses on basic education and knowledge [31]. On the behavioral research side, there are several international models explaining health action. Most of these models are prediction models that include several factors that are suitable for predicting health action. These models are called continuous, because a person can be assigned a certain probability of healthy action along a continuum [32]. The prediction models allow identifying factors that can be individually manipulated to promote healthy actions. Prominent examples of prediction models are the social-cognitive theory of Bandura (social-cognitive theory, SCT; [25,33]), the theory of reasoned action (TRA; [34]), and the theory of planned behavior (TPB; [35,36,37]), the model of health belief (HBM; [38,39]), the protection motivation theory (PMT; [40,41]) and the model of the health action process approach (HAPA; [32,42,43]). Research following this paradigm found that nutritional behavior is determined by attitudes and motivational factors towards nutrition [27,44]. Accordingly, different motivational factors can play a role in decision-making processes [23]; for example, perceived susceptibility and perceived severity of associated diseases (e.g., type 2 diabetes), efficacy expectations (e.g., concerning the reduction of sugar) and the personal evaluation of this action, as unpleasant, expensive, or stressful. Even more, the expectation that a particular action leads to the desired outcome (e.g., the prevention of type 2 diabetes) is not easy to calculate and prone to subjective assessments as well as the value of this outcome, not to forget the social norm, which, e.g., might lead one to eat sweets to be socially recognized.
In addition to motivational aspects, an essential role in the context of health actions is attributed to knowledge [15,45,46,47]. The numerous definitions of health literacy that describe the procurement, understanding, and use of information as the basis of health literacy [48] support the role knowledge has. However, the influence of knowledge on intention formation or directly on behavior, especially in the area of nutrition, has not been clarified yet, and findings about the role of information or knowledge, in decision-making (or intention formation) and action, however, are inconsistent [49,50]. There is a corpus of studies indicating that knowledge about nutrition issues is associated with behavioral intention and performance (e.g., [51,52,53]). Other studies, however, point to the exact opposite (e.g., [54,55]). Reasons for this inconsistency can be found in the subject matter and its complexity, as well as in the process of human decision-making [56].
A lack of correlation can be attributed to a lack of specificity in the operationalization of knowledge and intention or action [34,50]. For example, it is conceivable that the measured correlation in the global issue of “healthy nutrition” is smaller than, for example, in “sugar consumption.” Also, Worsley [50] argues that different types of knowledge (e.g., declarative and procedural knowledge) must be taken into account. Furthermore, he argues that it may not be possible to grasp the connections between knowledge and action because they are not directly present, but are mediated, for example, by motivational factors [50]. However, models that take into account different types of knowledge and integrate both knowledge and motivational factors in a meaningful way, so that mediation correlations can also be taken into account, are still lacking. Thus, an adequate description of the role of knowledge is not possible, and corresponding interventions in science and biology teaching based on knowledge transfer remain explorative. To make appropriate models usable for science/biology teaching and to avoid the danger of training action routines instead of promoting knowledge-based decision-making, Arnold proposed a model [23]. This model identifies different knowledge types and systematically places it in connection with well-established motivational factors. Here, Kaiser and Fuhrer [57] used a threefold division of knowledge underlying ecological behavior, which can be transferred to health behavior like, for example, sugar consumption ([23]; Figure 2):
  • System Health Knowledge (SK), which is the “knowledge about health, the body, and its (mal-) functioning” [23]. It includes knowledge about the use of carbohydrates and how carbohydrates are metabolized in the body, the mechanisms and risk factors that lead to insulin resistance and type 2 diabetes, and the impact of type 2 diabetes on health. This knowledge might especially influence the evaluation of susceptibility and severity of coming down with type 2 diabetes (perceived health threat). Moreover, it influences the following knowledge types.
  • Action-related Health Knowledge (AK) is the “knowledge about possible actions to preserve functioning and prevent malfunctioning of body and health” [23]. It includes knowledge about recommendations about sugar intake, about foods that contain carbohydrates and sugars, and knowledge about actions to reduce the intake of sugar. This knowledge is hypothesized to influence the attitude towards health action.
  • Effectiveness Health Knowledge (EK), which is the knowledge about the relative potential of actions that lead to the desired prevention of diseases [23]. It includes, e.g., the ability to decide on foods that contain less sugar. This knowledge is hypothesized to influence the attitude towards the health outcome.
The proposed three-dimensionality has been tested, and it was shown that health knowledge concerning the reduction of sugar consumption in favor of type 2 diabetes prevention could be treated as three-dimensional, hence consisting of SK, AK, and EK [56]. Since this three-dimensionality of health knowledge was derived from theory and adopted in a model for decision-making [23], it needs validation. The question arises whether learners perceive the three knowledge types as important for their decisions, too. In the presented study, we examined which knowledge students consider being relevant for health decisions, again using type 2 diabetes as an example. Hence, the goal of this study is to identify the ascribed importance of different health knowledge (dimensions) for decision-making processes. If students would not find all types of knowledge important, it would have a corresponding influence on the model, or at least on teaching according to the model. This study is therefore a first step towards validating this model with empirical evidence.

2. Materials and Methods

In the first step, we asked students (N = 56) from vocational school (mean age: 20.1; 91.07% female) via an open-ended questionnaire-item, what knowledge they would need to form a decision regarding their sugar consumption: “Chocolate, gummi bears, cakes, and coke are delicious! However, too much sugar can lead to health problems. For example, it may be a cause of type 2 diabetes. Imagine that you should decide today if you wanted to reduce your sugar intake for one year to prevent type 2 diabetes. What information would you need for your decision? Write at least three questions that need to be answered for your decision.” Of these 56 students, 13 had a special relationship to biology, chemistry, or nutrition, e.g., through a subject or course of study. We chose this older and more-experienced sample, as it has been shown that younger pupils give few answers to these open questions. The answers were categorized inductively and deductively assigned to the three knowledge dimensions. For example, the students asked the following questions: “How is type 2 diabetes triggered?” “I should know what particular foods can cause this disease,” “Where exactly is the problem, is it inherited or a real threat to me from my diet?”, “How does type 2 diabetes get into my body?” or “How do I get type 2 diabetes?”. These questions have been combined into the category “causes of type 2 diabetes” and attributed to the SK dimension.
Finally, the categories were formulated as questions in Likert-scale items, e.g., “What are the causes of type 2 diabetes?”. Additionally, we added six items covering the SK concerning sugar and its processing in the (un)healthy body, because there were no such questions in the students’ questions. Yet still, we wanted to see all the possible knowledge types of the model covered.
In a second step, the resulting quantitative questionnaire was used for the reported study. The questionnaire consisted of 34 items in three scales. This second sample consisted of students from the 10th grade (N = 81) of a rural city in southern Germany (mean age: 15.9; 56.10% female). The sample contained no diabetics, 11 vegetarians, no vegans, 2 persons with lactose intolerance and no persons with fructose or glucose intolerance or allergy. On a scale of 0 to 3, students described themselves as having little dietary experience (“My dietary experience is great”; 0.35), rather nutrition-conscious (“It is important to me to know what I eat and drink”; 1.81), rather health-conscious (“I live very health-consciously”; 1.63) and rather less sugar-consciously (“I take care to eat little sugar”; 1.30). These students were asked to rate the importance of information for their decision-making on a six-point Likert scale (0 = unimportant, 5 = important; see Table 1).
Descriptive analyses were conducted (means and standard deviations for each question) to identify the ascribed importance of single health information for the decision-making process. This allowed for ranking the questions in terms of importance. Furthermore, means and standard deviations for the three knowledge types (scales) were calculated to describe their importance.

3. Results

The descriptive results are displayed in Table 2 and show the 34 questions sorted by their ascribed importance. As can be seen, the range of the attributed importance ranges from an average of 3–4.3 (with a maximum of 5) points. Accordingly, it can be assumed that the students surveyed find all the questions somewhat necessary when it comes to deciding on reducing sugar consumption. The six questions that have been added subsequently, which relate to sugar and its processing in the body, are among the least important questions. Then, we find a mixture of items from all three scales with the questions “What is type 2 diabetes?”, “What can I do to prevent type 2 diabetes?” and “What are the causes of type 2 diabetes?” being the most important questions to be answered to form an intention to reduce one’s sugar intake.
Looking at the descriptive data of the knowledge scales (SK, AK, and EK; Figure 3), it can be seen that all three types of knowledge are assessed as rather relevant, since the mean values with a maximum number of five are each close to four with a standard deviation of less than one making (MSK = 3.7, SD = 0.74; MAK = 4.0, SD = 0.81; MEK = 3.9, SD = 0.90). This means that, on average, the importance of the respective types of knowledge is estimated to be rather high for decision-making.

4. Discussion

This study shows that students think information is important for forming a decision about changing their actions, since all questions are rated above the mean. This fits with the results of other research, where, e.g., students had to decide about vaccination and were rather hesitant because they wanted more information [58]. Hence, even if research has not yet been able to prove uniformly that knowledge influences decisions, the results of this study show that information is (subjectively) highly relevant for the people who make decisions. Furthermore, it could be shown that all three types of knowledge, as assumed in the model (Figure 2), are relevant. Accordingly, the results of this study support the model assumptions.
However, the study’s results are limited to some extent. On the one hand, this concerns the sample, which does not permit generalization but yet gives valuable insight into how students value information for decision-making. On the other hand, the object of investigation is limited because we chose to examine the very close relationship between sugar consumption and type 2 diabetes. This limitation is, on the one hand, necessary to achieve sufficient depth of knowledge, because tests and questionnaires can only have a certain length, and one has to decide whether the topic is covered in depth or breadth. This, of course, means that other factors influencing the onset of type 2 diabetes, such as exercise and diet in general, as well as genetic factors, cannot be adequately assessed. On the other hand, this close connection also follows the law of specificity [34]. According to Fishbein and Ajzen [34], a lack of correlation between knowledge and action is due to a lack of fit between knowledge and the dependent variable [50]. Furthermore, only the significance of knowledge for the decision was inquired. This investigation cannot examine to what extent the knowledge influences the actual action. Additionally, this study takes a look at decision-making from a very individualistic point of view. For reasons of feasibility, it is assumed that the individual decision or action depends solely on the individual and his or her personal assessment. Social, political, and economic reasons are neglected for this purpose. It is assumed that people who decide to take action and notice that external limits are imposed on them are more willing to change such conditions [19,20].

5. Conclusions and Outlook

This study’s results indicate that students want and need a solid understanding of all three knowledge types to form informed decisions. Accordingly, if teachers want to support students in making health-related decisions, they should include all three types of knowledge in their teaching. Accordingly, it is not enough to simply explain the causes of a disease (SK) or even to convey what health-promoting behaviour would be (AK). The data indicate that it makes sense to convey knowledge about the disease and its causes (SK) and possible alternative courses of action (AK), but also how effective different courses of action can ever be (EK). Using type 2 diabetes as an example, this would mean that both diabetes and its development would be discussed, and the role of sugar in this would be addressed (SK). In addition, ways of reducing sugar intake should be shown (AK), but also its effectiveness (possibly in connection with other behaviours) should be discussed (EK).
The next steps in research will be to gather data from a larger sample as well as for other health contexts in order to broaden the message. However, now that we have found hints that, consistent with the model, learners find the three types of knowledge relevant to their decisions, further research can be done in this direction: if these knowledge types are important, are they included in curricula and do students have knowledge in all three dimensions to the same extent and, if so, do they use it in decision-making processes? These are questions that we will ask in following studies. Yet despite all this, it becomes clear that although knowledge seems to be relevant, the respective knowledge for all possible health decisions can neither be learned nor taught. Therefore, the goal of the project is to shed light on the actual role of knowledge and, in a first step, the three dimensions in decision-making and intention formation. Then, implications for school science shall be formulated about how to prepare students to be able to adapt to different situations, e.g., by learning how to gather and reflect information and, thereby, to become responsible decision-makers. The question then is, how can science education equip people to be able to get the necessary information and use it? Or, in connection to the model in Figure 2, what is the scientific literacy underlying that specific knowledge? Here, four “tools” can be suggested [18]: (1) Systems Thinking, because the body can be seen as a system (and yet part of other systems), which can lead to complex interdependencies. Yet, if students have an understanding of systems, they are more likely to understand the system and reflect on the borders of knowledge (e.g., [59,60,61]). This point leads to (2) knowledge and understanding of the Nature of Science. This is important to understand and appreciate, e.g., that scientific knowledge is tentative, provisional, and uncertain and can be influenced by values and bias (e.g., [61,62]). However, not only is science influenced by values, every person has values, and decisions cannot be purely objective, because (3) affectivity plays a role, and subjective judgments (e.g., [15]) and personal values must be taken into account for action decisions as suggested by many health behavior models (e.g., [24,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43] ), as summarized in [23]. Finally, one needs (4) critical thinking to be able to reflect one’s point of view, question information critically, and be able to change perspectives (e.g., [63,64]).

Funding

This research was funded by swissuniversities, Development of scientific competences in subject didactics (2017–2020)—P-9/TP2, and a scholarship of the FHNW University of Applied Sciences and Arts Northwestern Switzerland, School of Education. Swissuniversities funded the APC.

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank Lea Kahl for her valuable help in data collection and analysis. Also, the author would like to thank the participating schools, teachers, and students, without which our research would not be possible.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. WHO/World Health Organization. Global Health Observatory (GHO) Data: Noncommunicable Diseases (NCD). Available online: https://www.who.int/gho/ncd/en/ (accessed on 5 December 2019).
  2. UN/United Nations. Sustainable Development Goal 3: Ensure Healthy Lives and Promote Well-Being for All at All Ages. Available online: https://sustainabledevelopment.un.org/sdg3 (accessed on 5 December 2019).
  3. Forouhi, N.G.; Wareham, N.J. Epidemiology of diabetes. Medicine (Abingdon) 2014, 42, 698–702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. WHO/World Health Organization. Diabetes. Available online: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/diabetes (accessed on 5 December 2019).
  5. Fensham, P.J. Preparing Citizens for a Complex World: The Grand Challenge of Teaching Socio-scientific Issues in Science Education. In Science|Environment|Health. Towards a Renewed Pedagogy for Science Education; Zeyer, A., Kyburz-Graber, R., Eds.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2012; pp. 7–30. [Google Scholar]
  6. WHO/World Health Organization. Global Health Observatory (GHO) Data: Risk Factors. Available online: https://www.who.int/gho/ncd/risk_factors/en/ (accessed on 5 December 2019).
  7. Forouzanfar, M.H.; Afshin, A.; Alexander, L.T.; Anderson, H.R.; Bhutta, Z.A.; Biryukov, S.; Brauer, M.; Burnett, R.; Cercy, K.; Charlson, F.J.; et al. Global, regional, and national comparative risk assessment of 79 behavioural, environmental and occupational, and metabolic risks or clusters of risks, 1990–2015: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2015. Lancet 2016, 388, 1659–1724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Arnold, J.; Dannemann, S.; Gropengießer, I.; Heuckmann, B.; Kahl, L.; Schaal, S.; Schaal, S.; Schlüter, K.; Schwanewedel, J.; Simon, U.; et al. Entwicklung eines Modells zur reflexiven gesundheitsbezogenen Handlungsfähigkeit aus biologiedidaktischer Perspektive [Development of a model for reflexive health-related ability to act from a biology didactic perspective]. Biol. Unserer Zeit 2019, 49, 243–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Cao, B.; Bray, F.; Ilbawi, A.; Soerjomataram, I. Effect on longevity of one-third reduction in premature mortality from non-communicable diseases by 2030: A global analysis of the Sustainable Development Goal health target. Lancet Glob. Health 2018, 6, e1288–e1296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Biesalski, K.; Bischoff, S.C.; Pirlich, M.; Weimann, A. (Eds.) Ernährungsmedizin [Nutritional Medicine]; Georg Thieme: Stuttgart, Germany, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  11. Renz-Polster, H.; Krautzig, S. (Eds.) Basislehrbuch Innere Medizin [Basic Textbook Internal Medicine]; Urban & Fischer: München, Germany, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  12. WHO/World Health Organization. Jakarta Declaration on Leading Health Promotion into the 21st Century. 1997. Available online: https://www.who.int/healthpromotion/milestones_ch4_20090916_en.pdf (accessed on 21 April 2020).
  13. WHO/World Health Organization. Global Health Observatory (GHO) Data: Unhealthy Diet. Available online: https://www.who.int/gho/ncd/risk_factors/unhealthy_diet_text/en/ (accessed on 5 December 2019).
  14. Zeyer, A.; Dillon, J. Science|Environment|Health—The emergence of a new pedagogy of complex living systems. Discip. Interdiscip. Sci. Educ. Res. 2019, 1, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Zeyer, A. A Win-Win Situation for Health and Science Education: Seeing Through the Lens of a New Framework Model of Health Literacy. In Science Environment Health. Towards a Renewed Pedagogy for Science Education; Zeyer, A., Kyburz-Graber, R., Eds.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2012; pp. 147–173. [Google Scholar]
  16. Zeyer, A.; Dillon, J. Science|Environment|Health—Towards a reconceptualization of three critical and inter-linked areas of education. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2014, 36, 1409–1411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Joint Committee on Health Education and Promotion Terminology. Report of the 2000 Joint Committee on Health Education and Promotion Terminology. Am. J. Health Educ. 2001, 32, 97–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Arnold, J.; Bauer, D. The Role of Science Education in Decision Making Concerning Health and Environmental Issues. In Science|Environment|Health—Towards a New Science Pedagogy of Complex Living Systems; Zeyer, A., Kyburz-Graber, R., Eds.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, Accepted.
  19. Malmberg, C.; Urbas, A. Health in school: Stress, individual responsibility and democratic politics. Cult. Stud. Sci. Educ. 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Malmberg, C.; Urbas, A. Health and Sustainable Development Education—A Paradox of Responsibility. In Science|Environment|Health—Towards a New Science Pedagogy of Complex Living Systems; Zeyer, A., Kyburz-Graber, R., Eds.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, Accepted.
  21. Lee, T.D.; Gail Jones, M.; Chesnutt, K. Teaching Systems Thinking in the Context of the Water Cycle. Res. Sci. Educ. 2019, 49, 137–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Bauer, D.; Arnold, J.; Kremer, K. Consumption-Intention Formation in Education for Sustainable Development: An Adapted Model Based on the Theory of Planned Behavior. Sustainability 2018, 10, 3455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Arnold, J.C. An integrated model of decision-making in health contexts: The role of science education in health education. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2018, 40, 519–537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Nutbeam, D. Health Promotion Glossary. Health Promot. Int. 1998, 13, 349–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Bandura, A. Self-efficacy: Toward a Unifying Theory of Behavioral Change. Psychol. Rev. 1977, 84, 191–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Røysamb, E.; Rse, J.; Kraft, P. On the structure and dimensionality of health-related behaviour in adolescents. Psychol. Health 1997, 12, 437–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Gerhards, J.; Roessel, J. Das Ernährungsverhalten Jugendlicher im Kontext ihrer Lebensstile. Eine empirische Studie [The Nutritional Behaviour of Young People in the Context of Their Lifestyles. An Empirical Study]; Bundeszentrale für Gesundheitliche Aufklärung: Köln, Germany, 2003; p. 145. [Google Scholar]
  28. Mensink, G.B.M.; Kleiser, C.; Richter, A. Was essen Kinder und Jugendliche in Deutschland? Ausgewählte Ergebnisse des Kinder- und Jugendgesundheitssurveys (KiGGS) [What do children and young people eat in Germany? Selected results of the Child and Adolescent Health Survey]. Ernährung 2007, 1, 204–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. WHO/FAO/Joint WHO/FAO Expert Consultation on Diet Nutrition and the Prevention of Chronic Diseases. Diet, Nutrition and the Prevention of Chronic Diseases: Report of a Joint WHO/FAO Expert Consultation. 2003. Available online: https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/42665/WHO_TRS_916.pdf;jsessionid=106E0A600548C07A603F24A196039F44?sequence=1 (accessed on 21 April 2020).
  30. Story, M.; Neumark-Sztainer, D.; French, S. Individual and Environmental Influences on Adolescent Eating Behaviors. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2002, 102, S40–S51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Camerini, L.; Schulz, P.J.; Nakamoto, K. Differential effects of health knowledge and health empowerment over patients’ self-management and health outcomes: A cross-sectional evaluation. Patient Educ. Couns. 2012, 89, 337–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Schwarzer, R. Modeling Health Behavior Change: How to Predict and Modify the Adoption and Maintenance of Health Behaviors. Appl. Psychol. 2008, 57, 1–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Bandura, A. Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control; Freeman & Co.: New York, NY, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  34. Fishbein, M.; Ajzen, I. Belief, Attitude, Intention and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research; Addison-Wesley Publishing Company: Reading, UK, 1975. [Google Scholar]
  35. Ajzen, I. The Theory of Planned Behaviour is Alive and Well, and not Ready to Retire: A Commentary on Sniehotta, Presseau, and Araújo-Soares. Health Psychol. Rev. 2014, 9, 131–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Ajzen, I. Perceived Behavioral Control, Self-Efficacy, Locus of Control, an the Theory of Planned Behavior. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 32, 665–683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Ajzen, I. From Intentions to Actions: A Theory of Planned Behavior. In Action Control: From Cognition to Behavior; Kuhl, J., Beckmann, J., Eds.; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 1985; pp. 11–39. [Google Scholar]
  38. Rosenstock, I.M. The Health Belief Model and Preventive Health Behavior. Health Educ. Monogr. 1974, 2, 354–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Rosenstock, I.M. Historical Origins of the Health Belief Model. Health Educ. Monogr. 1974, 2, 328–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Rogers, R.W. Cognitive and physiological processes in fear appeals and attitude change: A revised theory of protection motivation. In Social Psychology: A Source Book; Cacioppo, J.T., Petty, R.E., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1983; pp. 153–176. [Google Scholar]
  41. Rogers, R.W. A Protection Motivation Theory of Fear Appeals and Attitude Change1. J. Psychol. Interdiscip. Appl. 1975, 91, 93–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Schwarzer, R. Self-Efficacy in the Adoption and Maintenance of Health Behaviors: Theoretical Approaches and a new Model. In Self-Efficacy: Thought Control of Action; Schwarzer, R., Ed.; Hemisphere Publishing Corporation: Washington, DC, USA, 1992; pp. 217–243. [Google Scholar]
  43. Sutton, S. Stage Theories of Health Behaviour. In Predicting Health Behaviour; Conner, M., Norman, P., Eds.; Open University Press: London, UK, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  44. The Information Centre. Health Survey for England 2007: Summary of Key Findings; The Information Centre: Leeds, UK, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  45. Gazmararian, J.A.; Williams, M.V.; Peel, J.; Baker, D.W. Health literacy and knowledge of chronic disease. Patient Educ. Couns. 2003, 51, 267–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Hart, P. Creating Spaces for Rethinking School Science: Perstpectives from Subjective and Social-Relational Ways of Knowing. In Science|Environment|Health. Towards a Renewed Pedagogy for Science Education; Zeyer, A., Kyburz-Graber, R., Eds.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2012; pp. 103–126. [Google Scholar]
  47. Keselman, A.; Hundal, S.; Smith, C.A. General and Environmental Health as the Context for Science Education. In Science|Environment|Health. Towards a Renewed Pedagogy for Science Education; Zeyer, A., Kyburz-Graber, R., Eds.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2012; pp. 127–146. [Google Scholar]
  48. Sørensen, K.; Van den Broucke, S.; Fullam, J.; Doyle, G.; Pelikan, J.; Slonska, Z.; Brand, H. Health literacy and public health: A systematic review and integration of definitions and models. BMC Public Health 2012, 12, 80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Nutbeam, D. Health literacy as a public health goal: A challenge for contemporary health education and communication strategies into the 21st century. Health Promot. Int. 2000, 15, 259–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Worsley, A. Nutrition knowledge and food consumption: Can nutrition knowledge change food behaviour? Asia Pac. J. Clin. Nutr. 2002, 11, S579–S585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Hall, E.; Chai, W.; Albrecht, J.A. Relationships between nutrition-related knowledge, self-efficacy, and behavior for fifth grade students attending Title I and non-Title I schools. Appetite 2016, 96, 245–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Kostanjevec, S.; Jerman, J.; Koch, V. Nutrition knowledge in relation to the eating behaviour and attitudes of Slovenian schoolchildren. Nutr. Food Sci. 2013, 43, 564–572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Webb, M.C.; Beckford, S.E. Nutritional Knowledge and Attitudes of Adolescent Swimmers in Trinidad and Tobago. J. Nutr. Metab. 2014, 2014, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Dissen, A.R.; Policastro, P.; Quick, V.; Byrd-Bredbenner, C. Interrelationships among nutrition knowledge, attitudes, behaviors and body satisfaction. Health Educ. 2011, 111, 283–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Spronk, I.; Kullen, C.; Burdon, C.; O’Connor, H. Relationship between nutrition knowledge and dietary intake. Br. J. Nutr. 2014, 1713–1726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  56. Zeyer, A.; Álvaro, N.; Arnold, J.; Benninghaus, J.C.; Hasslöf, H.; Kremer, K.; Lundström, M.; Mayoral, O.; Sjöström, J.; Sprenger, S.; et al. Addressing Complexity in Science|Environment|Health Pedagogy. In Contributions from Science Education Research, Selected Papers from the ESERA 2017 Conference; McLoughlin, E., Finlayson, O., Erduran, S., Childs, P., Eds.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2019; pp. 153–170. [Google Scholar]
  57. Kaiser, F.G.; Fuhrer, U. Ecological Behavior’s Dependency on Different Forms of Knowledge. Appl. Psychol. 2003, 52, 598–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Zeyer, A. Getting Involved with Vaccination. Swiss Student Teachers’ Reactions to a Public Vaccination Debate. Sustainability 2019, 11, 6644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Rieß, W.; Mischo, C. Entwicklung und erste Validierung eines Fragebogens zur Erfassung des systemishcen Denkens in nachhaltigkeitsrelevanten Kontexten [Development and first validation of a questionnaire to capture systemic thinking in sustainability relevant contexts]. In Kompetenzen der Bildung für Nachhaltige Entwicklung: Operationalisierung, Messung, Rahmenbedingungen, Befunde; Bormann, I., de Haan, G., Eds.; VS Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften: Wiesbaden, Germany, 2008; pp. 215–232. [Google Scholar]
  60. Rieckmann, M. Future-oriented higher education: Which key competencies should be fostered through university teaching and learning? Futures 2012, 4, 127–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Erduran, S.; Dagher, Z.R. Reconceptualizing the Nature of Science for Science Education—Scientific Knowledge, Practices and Other Family Categories; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  62. Lederman, N.G.; Abd-El-Khalick, F.; Bell, R.L.; Schwartz, R.S. Views of Nature of Science Questionnaire: Toward Valid and Meaningful Assessment of Learners’ Conceptions of Nature of Science. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2002, 36, 497–521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Facione, P.A. Critical Thinking: A Statement of Expert Consensus for Purposes of Educational Assessment and Instruction—Executive Summary. Insight Assessment. 1990. Available online: http://www.qcc.cuny.edu/SocialSciences/ppecorino/CT-Expert-Report.pdf (accessed on 21 April 2020).
  64. Rafolt, S.; Kapelari, S.; Kremer, K. Kritisches Denken im naturwissenschaftlichen Unterricht—Synergiemodell, Problemlage und Desiderata [Critical thinking in science teaching—synergy model, problems and desiderata]. Z. Didakt. Nat. 2019, 25, 63–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Schematic representation of possible causes of NCDs [6,9,10]. NCD: noncommunicable diseases.
Figure 1. Schematic representation of possible causes of NCDs [6,9,10]. NCD: noncommunicable diseases.
Sustainability 12 03396 g001
Figure 2. Integrated Model of Decision-Making in Health Contexts for Science Education [21] with possible correlations.
Figure 2. Integrated Model of Decision-Making in Health Contexts for Science Education [21] with possible correlations.
Sustainability 12 03396 g002
Figure 3. Students’ answers to the questionnaire about the importance of knowledge for decision-making.
Figure 3. Students’ answers to the questionnaire about the importance of knowledge for decision-making.
Sustainability 12 03396 g003
Table 1. Scales and examples for Likert-scale items (the complete item set is presented in Table 2). Introduction: “Imagine that you should decide today if you want to reduce your sugar intake in the future to prevent type 2 diabetes. How important would the following information be for you?”
Table 1. Scales and examples for Likert-scale items (the complete item set is presented in Table 2). Introduction: “Imagine that you should decide today if you want to reduce your sugar intake in the future to prevent type 2 diabetes. How important would the following information be for you?”
ScaleNo. of ItemsCronbach’s αExamples
System Health Knowledge (SK)150.92What is sugar?
What are the symptoms of type 2 diabetes?
Why does my body need sugar?
Action-related Knowledge (AK)150.94What can I do to prevent type 2 diabetes?
How can I reduce my sugar consumption?
Which foods contain how much sugar?
Effectivity Knowledge (EK)40.86What is the likelihood that my sugar consumption will cause me to develop type 2 diabetes?
What is the probability that I will develop type 2 diabetes if I reduce my sugar consumption?
Table 2. Questions concerning sugar and type 2 diabetes and how important students think they are for forming decisions about reducing sugar intake.
Table 2. Questions concerning sugar and type 2 diabetes and how important students think they are for forming decisions about reducing sugar intake.
QuestionScaleImportance
MeanSD
How is sugar structured?SK3.021.31
What is sugar?SK3.231.23
Why does my body need sugar?SK3.491.19
What types of sugar are there?SK3.521.17
How is sugar processed in a healthy body?SK3.571.18
How effective are individual low-sugar alternatives to prevent the onset of type 2 diabetes?EK3.651.16
Which organs of my body are involved in sugar processing?SK3.691.10
Where can I find information on the sugar content of foods?AK3.781.16
Which types of sugar should I consume preferably?AK3.81.09
Are all sugars equally harmful?AK3.881.13
What influences my blood sugar level?SK3.891.08
How much sugar can I eat every day to stay healthy?AK3.891.08
How is sugar processed in the body if you have type 2 diabetes?SK3.891.08
What is the probability that I will develop type 2 diabetes if I reduce my sugar consumption?EK3.901.08
Which foods contain the least amount of sugar?AK3.911.18
To what extent should I reduce my sugar intake to prevent type 2 diabetes?AK3.941.05
What is the likelihood that I will develop type 2 diabetes if I reduce my sugar intake?EK3.961.16
What are the alternatives to sugar?AK3.961.21
What is the likelihood that my sugar consumption will cause me to develop type 2 diabetes?SK3.961.10
What role does sugar play in the development of type 2 diabetes?SK3.961.03
What are the consequences of type 2 diabetes for me and my well-being?SK4.021.11
Which foods contain how much sugar?AK4.040.97
Which foods contain the most sugar?AK4.061.14
What low-sugar alternatives are there?AK4.061.13
How can I reduce my sugar consumption?AK4.091.10
What are the consequences of reducing my sugar intake for my body?EK4.090.95
What types of sugar should I avoid?AK4.101.02
How high is my current sugar consumption?AK4.121.03
How does type 2 diabetes develop?SK4.220.99
What are the symptoms of type 2 diabetes?SK4.221.07
Which foods should I consume, preferably, and which should I consume in moderation to reduce my sugar intake? AK4.241.05
What is type 2 diabetes?SK4.271.00
What can I do to prevent type 2 diabetes?AK4.270.99
What are the causes of type 2 diabetes?SK4.310.97

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Arnold, J.C. The Importance of Different Knowledge Types in Health-Related Decisions—The Example of Type 2 Diabetes. Sustainability 2020, 12, 3396. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12083396

AMA Style

Arnold JC. The Importance of Different Knowledge Types in Health-Related Decisions—The Example of Type 2 Diabetes. Sustainability. 2020; 12(8):3396. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12083396

Chicago/Turabian Style

Arnold, Julia Caroline. 2020. "The Importance of Different Knowledge Types in Health-Related Decisions—The Example of Type 2 Diabetes" Sustainability 12, no. 8: 3396. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12083396

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop