How Reputation Systems Change the Psychological Antecedents of Fairness in Virtual Environments
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Personal and Contextual Influences on Fairness and Adhesion to the Norm
1.2. Aims of the Study and Hypothesis
2. Material and Methods
2.1. Participants and Design
2.2. The Software
2.3. Psychological Measures
- Socio-demographic form. It is composed of five questions about gender, age, education, household size, and parents’ marital status.
- Five-Factor Adjective Short Test (5-FasT) [59]. It is a short version of the Five-Factor Model and investigates the traits of Neuroticism, Extraversion, Agreeableness, Closeness, and Consciousness using 26 adjectives. For each adjective, participants indicate how much they describe their personality on a five-point Likert scale (from 1 = “not at all”, to 5 = “very much”). All the subscales are characterized by good reliability (Neuroticism ; Extraversion ; Agreeableness ; Closeness ; Consciousness ).
- General Self-Efficacy scale [60]. General self-efficacy is the belief in one’s competence to cope with a broad range of situations and demands and reflects a generalization across various domains of functioning in which people judge how efficacious they are [61]. This scale investigates the Self-Efficacy perception of the participants and is made of ten items on which participants have to express their level of agreement on a four-point Likert scale (from 1 = “not true at all”, to 4 = “totally true”). According to the review by Luszczynska and colleagues [62], the internal consistency coefficients of the measure ranges from to .
- Classroom Community scale [63]. This 20-item scale investigates the participants’ classroom Sense of Community. Two subscales, ten items each, comprise the instrument and consider the classroom both as a social and a learning community. With respect to the first subscale, classroom as a social community, some items examples are: “I feel that students in this course care about each other”; “I feel connected to others in this course”; “I do not feel a spirit of community.” As regards the second subscale, classroom as a learning community, some items examples are: “I feel that I am encouraged to ask questions”; “I feel that it is hard to get help when I have a question”; “I feel that I receive timely feedback.” For each assertion, participants should express their level of agreement relying on a five-point Likert scale (from 1 = “strongly disagree”, to 5 = “strongly agree”). The overall scale reliability is , while for the social community is , and for the learning community [64].
2.4. Procedures
2.5. Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Descriptive Statistics
3.2. Effects of Socio-Demographic and Psychological Characteristics on the Offer
3.3. Effects of Socio-Demographic and Psychological Characteristics on the Request
3.4. Effects of Socio-Demographic and Psychological Characteristics on Fairness
4. Discussion
4.1. Offer and Request Behaviors
4.2. Checking Hypotheses on Fairness
4.3. Study Limitations and Future Perspectives
4.4. Application Areas
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
References
- McCrae, R.R.; Costa, P.T., Jr. Personality trait structure as a human universal. Am. Psychol. 1997, 52, 509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Barry, B.; Stewart, G.L. Composition, process, and performance in self-managed groups: The role of personality. J. Appl. Psychol. 1997, 82, 62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sagiv, L.; Sverdlik, N.; Schwarz, N. To compete or to cooperate? Values’ impact on perception and action in social dilemma games. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 41, 64–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Luse, A.; McElroy, J.C.; Townsend, A.M.; Demarie, S. Personality and cognitive style as predictors of preference for working in virtual teams. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2013, 29, 1825–1832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Collodi, S.; Panerati, S.; Imbimbo, E.; Stefanelli, F.; Duradoni, M.; Guazzini, A. Personality and Reputation: A Complex Relationship in Virtual Environments. Future Internet 2018, 10, 120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Resnick, P.; Kuwabara, K.; Zeckhauser, R.; Friedman, E. Reputation systems. Commun. ACM 2000, 43, 45–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Henrich, J.; Boyd, R.; Bowles, S.; Camerer, C.; Fehr, E.; Gintis, H.; McElreath, R. In search of homo economicus: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Am. Econ. Rev. 2001, 91, 73–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kahneman, D.; Tversky, A. Prospect Theory of Decisions Under Risk. Econometrica 1979, 47, 1156–1167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Merrick, K.; Hardhienata, M.; Shafi, K.; Hu, J. A survey of game theoretic approaches to modelling decision-making in information warfare scenarios. Future Internet 2016, 8, 34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kimbrough, E.O.; Vostroknutov, A. Norms make preferences social. J. Eur. Econ. Assoc. 2016, 14, 608–638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roberts, G. Competitive altruism: From reciprocity to the handicap principle. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. Biol. Sci. 1998, 265, 427–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fehr, E.; Fischbacher, U. The nature of human altruism. Nature 2003, 425, 785–791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sanfey, A.G. Social decision-making: Insights from game theory and neuroscience. Science 2007, 318, 598–602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Caliendo, M.; Fossen, F.; Kritikos, A. Trust, positive reciprocity, and negative reciprocity: Do these traits impact entrepreneurial dynamics? J. Econ. Psychol. 2012, 33, 394–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gunnthorsdottir, A.; McCabe, K.; Smith, V. Using the Machiavellianism instrument to predict trustworthiness in a bargaining game. J. Econ. Psychol. 2002, 23, 49–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Semmann, D.; Krambeck, H.J.; Milinski, M. Reputation is valuable within and outside one’s own social group. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2005, 57, 611–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Milinski, M.; Semmann, D.; Krambeck, H.J.; Marotzke, J. Stabilizing the Earth’s climate is not a losing game: Supporting evidence from public goods experiments. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2006, 103, 3994–3998. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rockenbach, B.; Milinski, M. The efficient interaction of indirect reciprocity and costly punishment. Nature 2006, 444, 718–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Semmann, D.; Krambeck, H.J.; Milinski, M. Strategic investment in reputation. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2004, 56, 248–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sauermann, J. Reputation Formation in Bargaining. Partners vs. Strangers in the Ultimatum Game. In Jahrbuch für Handlungs-und Entscheidungstheorie; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2019; pp. 179–189. [Google Scholar]
- Duradoni, M.; Paolucci, M.; Bagnoli, F.; Guazzini, A. Fairness and Trust in Virtual Environments: The Effects of Reputation. Future Internet 2018, 10, 50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nowak, M.A.; Page, K.M.; Sigmund, K. Fairness versus reason in the ultimatum game. Science 2000, 289, 1773–1775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bicchieri, C.; Chavez, A. Behaving as expected: Public information and fairness norms. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2010, 23, 161–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Webster, M.M.; Ward, A.J. Personality and social context. Biol. Rev. 2011, 86, 759–773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Postmes, T.; Spears, R.; Sakhel, K.; De Groot, D. Social influence in computer-mediated communication: The effects of anonymity on group behavior. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2001, 27, 1243–1254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spears, R.; Postmes, T.; Lea, M.; Wolbert, A. When are net effects gross products? Communication. J. Soc. Issues 2002, 58, 91–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baumeister, R.F.; Zhang, L.; Vohs, K.D. Gossip as cultural learning. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2004, 8, 111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Okada, I. A Review of Theoretical Studies on Indirect Reciprocity. Games 2020, 11, 27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hilbig, B.E.; Zettler, I.; Heydasch, T. Personality, punishment and public goods: Strategic shifts towards cooperation as a matter of dispositional honesty–humility. Eur. J. Personal. 2012, 26, 245–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eckel, C.C.; Grossman, P.J. Altruism in anonymous dictator games. Games Econ. Behav. 1996, 16, 181–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Goldberg, L.R. The structure of phenotypic personality traits. Am. Psychol. 1993, 48, 26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koole, S.L.; Jager, W.; van den Berg, A.E.; Vlek, C.A.; Hofstee, W.K. On the social nature of personality: Effects of extraversion, agreeableness, and feedback about collective resource use on cooperation in a resource dilemma. PErsonality Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2001, 27, 289–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Volk, S.; Thöni, C.; Ruigrok, W. Personality, personal values and cooperation preferences in public goods games: A longitudinal study. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2011, 50, 810–815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- DeYoung, C.G.; Peterson, J.B.; Higgins, D.M. Higher-order factors of the Big Five predict conformity: Are there neuroses of health? Personal. Individ. Differ. 2002, 33, 533–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Proto, E.; Rustichini, A. A reassessment of the relationship between GDP and life satisfaction. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e79358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Graziano, W.G.; Tobin, R.M. Agreeableness: Dimension of personality or social desirability artifact? J. Personal. 2002, 70, 695–728. [Google Scholar]
- Jensen-Campbell, L.A.; Rosselli, M.; Workman, K.A.; Santisi, M.; Rios, J.D.; Bojan, D. Agreeableness, conscientiousness, and effortful control processes. J. Res. Personal. 2002, 36, 476–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paulhus, D.L.; John, O.P. Egoistic and moralistic biases in self-perception: The interplay of self-deceptive styles with basic traits and motives. J. Personal. 1998, 66, 1025–1060. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eagly, A.H.; Crowley, M. Gender and helping behavior: A meta-analytic review of the social psychological literature. Psychol. Bull. 1986, 100, 283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Simpson, B. Sex, fear, and greed: A social dilemma analysis of gender and cooperation. Soc. Forces 2003, 82, 35–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bonacich, P. Norms and cohesion as adaptive responses to potential conflict: An experimental study. Sociometry 1972, 35, 357–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dawes, R.M.; McTavish, J.; Shaklee, H. Behavior, communication, and assumptions about other people’s behavior in a commons dilemma situation. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1977, 35, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown-Kruse, J.; Hummels, D. Gender effects in laboratory public goods contribution: Do individuals put their money where their mouth is? J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 1993, 22, 255–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sell, J.; Wilson, R.K. Levels of information and contributions to public goods. Soc. Forces 1991, 70, 107–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vugt, M.v.; Cremer, D.D.; Janssen, D.P. Gender differences in cooperation and competition: The male-warrior hypothesis. Psychol. Sci. 2007, 18, 19–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Caldwell, M.D. Communication and sex effects in a five-person Prisoner’s Dilemma Game. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1976, 33, 273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goehring, D.J.; Kahan, J.P. The uniform n-person prisoner’s dilemma game: Construction and test of an index of cooperation. J. Confl. Resolut. 1976, 20, 111–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sell, J. Gender, strategies, and contributions to public goods. Soc. Psychol. Q. 1997, 60, 252–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lerner, B.S.; Locke, E.A. The effects of goal setting, self-efficacy, competition, and personal traits on the performance of an endurance task. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1995, 17, 138–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piazza, J.; Bering, J.M. Concerns about reputation via gossip promote generous allocations in an economic game. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2008, 29, 172–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sommerfeld, R.D.; Krambeck, H.J.; Milinski, M. Multiple gossip statements and their effect on reputation and trustworthiness. Proc. R. Soc. Biol. Sci. 2008, 275, 2529–2536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- De Cremer, D. Respect and cooperation in social dilemmas: The importance of feeling included. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2002, 28, 1335–1341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wit, A.P.; Wilke, H.A. The effect of social categorization on cooperation in three types of social dilemmas. J. Econ. Psychol. 1992, 13, 135–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feinberg, M.; Willer, R.; Stellar, J.; Keltner, D. The virtues of gossip: Reputational information sharing as prosocial behavior. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2012, 102, 1015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Lampe, C.; Zube, P.; Lee, J.; Park, C.H.; Johnston, E. Crowdsourcing civility: A natural experiment examining the effects of distributed moderation in online forums. Gov. Inf. Q. 2014, 31, 317–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Seo, K.K. Utilizing peer moderating in online discussions: Addressing the controversy between teacher moderation and nonmoderation. Am. J. Distance Educ. 2007, 21, 21–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, Y.; Xie, J. Third-party product review and firm marketing strategy. Mark. Sci. 2005, 24, 218–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ma, Y.; Li, F. Detecting review spam: Challenges and opportunities. In Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Collaborative Computing: Networking, Applications and Worksharing (CollaborateCom), Pittsburgh, PA, USA, 14–17 October 2012; pp. 651–654. [Google Scholar]
- Giannini, M.; Pannocchia, L.; Grotto, L.; Gori, A. A measure for counseling: The five factor adjective short test (5-fast). Couns. Ital. Ric. Appl. 2012, 3, 384. [Google Scholar]
- Jerusalem, M.; Schwarzer, R. Self-efficacy as a resource factor in stress appraisal processes. In Self-Efficacy: Thought Control of Action; Taylor & Francis: Park Avenue/Abingdon, UK, 1992; Volume 195213. [Google Scholar]
- Luszczynska, A.; Scholz, U.; Schwarzer, R. The general self-efficacy scale: Multicultural validation studies. J. Psychol. 2005, 139, 439–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Scholz, U.; Doña, B.G.; Sud, S.; Schwarzer, R. Is general self-efficacy a universal construct? Psychometric findings from 25 countries. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2002, 18, 242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rovai, A.P.; Wighting, M.J.; Lucking, R. The classroom and school community inventory: Development, refinement, and validation of a self-report measure for educational research. Internet High. Educ. 2004, 7, 263–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rovai, A.P. Development of an instrument to measure classroom community. Internet High. Educ. 2002, 5, 197–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCulloch, C.E.; Searle, S.R. Generalized, Linear, and Mixed Models (Wiley Series in Probability and Statistics); Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Reicher, S.D.; Spears, R.; Postmes, T. A social identity model of deindividuation phenomena. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 1995, 6, 161–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duradoni, M.; Gronchi, G.; Bocchi, L.; Guazzini, A. Reputation matters the most: The reputation inertia effect. In Human Behavior and Emerging Technologies; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Stigler, G.J. Free riders and collective action: An appendix to theories of economic regulation. Bell J. Econ. Manag. Sci. 1974, 5, 359–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Karau, S.J.; Williams, K.D. Social loafing: A meta-analytic review and theoretical integration. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1993, 65, 681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Raju, A. Can reviewer reputation and webcare content affect perceived fairness? J. Res. Interact. Mark. 2019, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Condor, S. Towards a social psychology of citizenship? Introduction to the special issue. J. Community Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 21, 193–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berardi, U. Sustainability assessment of urban communities through rating systems. Environ. Dev. Sustain. 2013, 15, 1573–1591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spagnolli, A.; Corradi, N.; Gamberini, L.; Hoggan, E.; Jacucci, G.; Katzeff, C.; Broms, L.; Jonsson, L. Eco-feedback on the go: Motivating energy awareness. Computer 2011, 44, 38–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Too, L.; Bajracharya, B. Sustainable campus: Engaging the community in sustainability. Int. J. Sustain. High. Educ. 2015, 16, 57–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duradoni, M.; Innocenti, F.; Guazzini, A. Well-Being and Social Media: A Systematic Review of Bergen Addiction Scales. Future Internet 2020, 12, 24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Guazzini, A.; Duradoni, M.; Capelli, A.; Meringolo, P. An explorative model to assess individuals’ phubbing risk. Future Internet 2019, 11, 21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Experimental Hypothesis | ||
---|---|---|
Variable | Reputation OFF | Reputation ON |
Fairness | Age (+) Self-Efficacy (−) Sense of Community (+) | Extraversion (−) Neuroticism (−) Openness (−) Agreeableness (+) Conscientiousness (+) Age (+) Self-Efficacy (0) Sense of Community (+) |
Experimental Design and Participants | |||
---|---|---|---|
Males | Females | Total | |
Reputation OFF | |||
Reputation ON | |||
Total |
Descriptive Statistics of the Study’s Variables | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Rep OFF | Rep ON | |||
Variable | M | s.d. | M | s.d. |
Age | ||||
Offer | ||||
Request | ||||
Fairness | ||||
Neuroticism | ||||
Extraversion | ||||
Agreeableness | ||||
Closeness | 4 | |||
Conscientiousness | ||||
Self-Efficacy | ||||
Sense of Community |
Predictors of the Offer | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Best Model | Akaike | F | df-1 (2) | |||
Offer Rep OFF | 4027.520 | 10.482 *** | 3 (1516) | |||
Offer Rep ON | 4148.733 | 8.333 *** | 6 (1527) | |||
Fixed coefficients for Rep OFF | ||||||
Factors | Coefficient () | Student t | p. | |||
Extraversion | 0.027 | 3.50 | 0.001 | |||
Age | 0.011 | 2.19 | 0.029 | |||
Gender (M) | 0.152 | 3.20 | 0.001 | |||
Fixed coefficients for Rep ON | ||||||
Factors | Coefficient () | Student t | p. | |||
Extraversion | −0.029 | −3.46 | 0.001 | |||
Agreeableness | −0.028 | −3.87 | 0.001 | |||
Conscientiousness | 0.021 | 3.31 | 0.001 | |||
Neuroticism | 0.019 | 2.92 | 0.004 | |||
Sense of Community | 0.017 | 3.47 | 0.001 | |||
Self-Efficacy | 0.026 | 3.61 | 0.001 |
Predictors of the Request | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Best Model | Akaike | F | df-1 (2) | |||
Request Rep OFF | 3239.906 | 7528 *** | 3 (1286) | |||
Request Rep ON | 3331.380 | 18,604 *** | 6 (1309) | |||
Fixed coefficients for Rep OFF | ||||||
Factors | Coefficient () | Student t | p. | |||
Extraversion | 0.022 | 2.73 | 0.006 | |||
Agreeableness | −0.017 | −2.76 | 0.006 | |||
Closeness | 0.024 | 3.42 | 0.001 | |||
Fixed coefficients for Rep ON | ||||||
Factors | Coefficient () | Student t | p. | |||
Extraversion | −0.032 | −3.98 | 0.001 | |||
Agreeableness | −0.017 | −2.38 | 0.017 | |||
Closeness | −0.020 | −2.87 | 0.004 | |||
Neuroticism | 0.033 | 4.60 | 0.001 | |||
Self-Efficacy | 0.029 | 3.97 | 0.001 | |||
Age | −0.079 | −8.68 | 0.001 |
Predictors of Fairness | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Best Model | Akaike | F | df-1 (2) | |||
Fairness Rep OFF | 10953.659 | 11.968 *** | 3 (1557) | |||
Fairness Rep ON | 11252.259 | 11.501 *** | 4 (1595) | |||
Fixed coefficients for Rep OFF | ||||||
Factors | Coefficient () | Student t | p. | |||
Sense of Community | 0.196 | 4.33 | 0.001 | |||
Self-Efficacy | −0.143 | −2.62 | 0.009 | |||
Age | 0.169 | 3.74 | 0.001 | |||
Fixed coefficients for Rep ON | ||||||
Factors | Coefficient () | Student t | p. | |||
Sense of Community | 0.084 | 1.99 | 0.047 | |||
Neuroticism | −0.135 | −2.16 | 0.031 | |||
Closeness | 0.130 | 2.27 | 0.023 | |||
Age | 0.374 | 4.59 | 0.001 |
Predictors of Fairness | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Best Model | Akaike | F | df-1 (2) | |||
Fairness Final Model | 22176.750 | 11.163 *** | 8 (3152) | |||
Fixed coefficients | ||||||
Factors | Coefficient () | Student t | p. | |||
Reputation Condition (Rep. ON) | 1.42 | 2.79 | 0.005 | |||
Sense of Community | 1.93 | 6.35 | 0.001 | |||
Self-Efficacy | −1.08 | −3.55 | 0.001 | |||
Age | 1.39 | 3.51 | 0.001 | |||
Closeness X Rep ON | 1.30 | 3.14 | 0.002 | |||
Neuroticism X Rep ON | −0.64 | −1.54 | 0.048 |
Summary of Fairness Antecedents | ||
---|---|---|
Variable | Reputation OFF | Reputation ON |
Offer | Extraversion (+) Age (+) Gender (M) (+) | Extraversion (−) Agreeableness (−) Conscientiousness (+) Neuroticism (+) Sense of Community (+) Self-Efficacy (+) |
Request | Extraversion (+) Agreeableness (−) Closeness (+) | Extraversion (−) Agreeableness (−) Closeness (−) Neuroticism (+) Self-Efficacy (+) Age (−) |
Fairness | Sense of Community (+) Self-Efficacy (−) Age (+) | Sense of Community (+) Self-Efficacy (−) Neuroticism (−) Closeness (+) Age (+) |
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Collodi, S.; Fiorenza, M.; Guazzini, A.; Duradoni, M. How Reputation Systems Change the Psychological Antecedents of Fairness in Virtual Environments. Future Internet 2020, 12, 132. https://doi.org/10.3390/fi12080132
Collodi S, Fiorenza M, Guazzini A, Duradoni M. How Reputation Systems Change the Psychological Antecedents of Fairness in Virtual Environments. Future Internet. 2020; 12(8):132. https://doi.org/10.3390/fi12080132
Chicago/Turabian StyleCollodi, Stefania, Maria Fiorenza, Andrea Guazzini, and Mirko Duradoni. 2020. "How Reputation Systems Change the Psychological Antecedents of Fairness in Virtual Environments" Future Internet 12, no. 8: 132. https://doi.org/10.3390/fi12080132
APA StyleCollodi, S., Fiorenza, M., Guazzini, A., & Duradoni, M. (2020). How Reputation Systems Change the Psychological Antecedents of Fairness in Virtual Environments. Future Internet, 12(8), 132. https://doi.org/10.3390/fi12080132