Next Article in Journal
Physical and Sociocultural Community-Level Influences on Cigar Smoking among Black Young Adults: An In-Depth Interview Investigation
Previous Article in Journal
Impacts of Extreme Climate Events on Future Rice Yields in Global Major Rice-Producing Regions
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Associations between Caregivers’ Emotional and Instrumental Feeding, Children’s Emotional Eating, and Children’s Consumption of Ultra-Processed Foods in China

1
Department of Maternal and Child Health, School of Public Health, Peking University, Beijing 100191, China
2
Loudi Town Health Centre, Luancheng District, Shijiazhuang 051430, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(8), 4439; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19084439
Submission received: 17 March 2022 / Revised: 2 April 2022 / Accepted: 4 April 2022 / Published: 7 April 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Health Behavior, Chronic Disease and Health Promotion)

Abstract

:
High consumption of ultra-processed foods (UPF) increases the risks of non-communicable diseases and all-cause mortality in adulthood, and the risk of early childhood caries. Limited evidence about factors influencing children’s consumption of UPF exists. This study was conducted to assess the prevalence of UPF consumption among children less than three years of age, and identify its associations with caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding, and children’s emotional eating. A cross-sectional study was conducted in Shijiazhuang, Hebei Province, China. Caregivers caring for children aged 6–36 months (n = 408) were recruited. Caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding practices and children’s emotional eating were assessed by the Parents’ Feeding Practices Scale for Infant and Young Child and Children’s Eating Behaviour Questionnaire, respectively. Children’s UPF consumption was assessed by a validated Food Frequency Questionnaire. Of the children, 86.8% consumed UPF. The highest percentage of UPF consumed was pastries (63.5%), followed by solid or semi-solid dairy products (58.8%), and reconstituted meat products (56.4%). Caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding was positively associated with children’s consumption of UPF (OR = 1.59, 95%CI: 1.01, 2.49), a higher frequency of UPF consumption weekly (OR = 1.80, 95%CI: 1.35, 2.39), and a larger amount of UPF consumption weekly (OR = 1.85, 95% CI: 1.38, 2.49). Children’s higher frequency of emotional undereating was associated with their UPF consumption (OR = 1.61, 95%CI: 1.07, 2.42) and a higher frequency of UPF consumption weekly (OR = 1.33, 95%CI: 1.03, 1.73). Children’s emotional undereating significantly mediated the associations between caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding and children’s consumption of reconstituted meat products. Caregivers should be educated to avoid emotional and instrumental feeding practices, and cultivate children’s good eating habits to improve children’s diet quality.

1. Introduction

Ultra-processed foods (UPF), defined within the NOVA food classification system [1], are foods made through a series of industrial processes with complex equipment and technologies to process substances, seasonings, and additives into delicately packaged foods [2]. Sugar-sweetened beverages, packaged snacks, and candies are examples of UPF [2]. UPF is palatable, convenient, profitable, and has a long shelf life [2]. UPF is high in energy density, carbohydrates, saturated fatty acids, trans fatty acids, and sodium, and low in fibre, protein, and micronutrients [3,4]. Higher UPF consumption was associated with the risks of metabolic syndrome and mortality among adults [5], higher blood lipid levels among children/adolescents [6], and dental caries among younger children [7]. Eating habits formed in early childhood can persist into adulthood [8,9]. A longitudinal study demonstrated the percentage of energy intake from UPF among children at 24 months was related to that among children at 60 months [10]. A reduction of UPF consumption in early childhood is beneficial for one’s health in the whole life course.
The percentage of UPF consumption was high among young children. In Brazil, 74.3% of children under two years old consumed UPF [11]. In Indonesia, 81.6% of children under three years old consumed packaged snacks, and 40.0% consumed sugar-sweetened beverages [12]. In the US, 85% of children aged two-to-three years old consumed sugar-sweetened beverages, desserts, sweets, or salty snacks daily [13]. In China, according to United Nations International Children’s Emergency Fund, the percentage of sales of dried baby foods and prepared baby foods per capita among children under five years old increased from 2014 to 2019 [14]. However, research on UPF consumption among children under three years of age is still lacking in China.
Caregivers’ feeding practices influence children’s dietary intake. Emotional feeding indicates using foods to calm or soothe, or for boredom or fussiness, and instrumental feeding indicates using foods to reward or punish [15,16]. Caregivers usually provide energy-dense, palatable, and nutrient-poor foods, such as candies and cookies, when they adopt emotional and instrumental feeding [16]. Studies in the Netherlands demonstrated parents’ emotional and instrumental feeding was positively related to children’s energy-dense snack consumption at six-to-nine years old [17,18,19]. In the UK, children whose mothers frequently used emotional feeding strategies ate more cookies and chocolates from three to five years old than children whose mothers did not tend to use emotional feeding [20]. Fathers’ emotional and instrumental feeding was positively associated with sugar-sweetened beverage consumption among Hispanic children aged two-to-five years old in the US [21]. In addition to snacks and sugar-sweetened beverages, the associations between caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding practices and children’s consumption of other UPF food items (e.g., reconstituted meat products) need to be further explored.
Children’s eating behaviours are also related to their dietary intake. Emotional eating refers to eating in response to negative emotions such as worry, anger, stress, and depression [22]. Emotions influence differently on appetite, with some experiencing an increased desire to eat (i.e., emotional overeating, EOE), and some experiencing a decreased desire to eat (i.e., emotional undereating, EUE) [23,24]. A study conducted in 12 countries demonstrated emotional eating among children aged nine-to-eleven years old was positively associated with an unhealthy dietary pattern characterised by high loadings for fast foods, ice cream, fried food, cakes, and sugar-sweetened sodas [25]. There is a lack of studies investigating the association between emotional eating and UPF consumption among children under three years old, a critical period of the development of one’s dietary habits. Caregivers’ feeding practices might influence children’s eating behaviours [26], and further play an indirect role in children’s dietary intake [21]. However, evidence examining the mediation effect of children’s eating behaviours on the association between caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding and children’s UPF consumption is still lacking.
The aim of this study was to assess the prevalence of UPF consumption among children under three years old in China, and to identify its association with caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding, and children’s emotional eating. The mediation effect of children’s emotional eating was also examined.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design

This study was a cross-sectional survey to examine caregivers’ feeding practices and their children’s dietary intake and behaviour in China.

2.2. Participants

The caregiver was the primary person responsible for child-feeding daily. The inclusion criteria were: caregivers had children aged 6-to-36 months old, of normal birth weight (i.e., 2500 g ≤ birth weight < 4000 g), of term delivery (i.e., 37 weeks ≤ gestational age < 42 weeks), living in Shijiazhuang, Hebei Province; and caregivers had good literacy and comprehension. The exclusion criteria were: caregivers had serious diseases (e.g., cancer, liver disease, kidney disease, AIDS, or depression), or had children with congenital diseases, infectious diseases, inherited metabolic diseases, dysmorphia, fever, or diarrhea. This study complied with the guidelines in the Declaration of Helsinki, and was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the Peking University (IRB00001052-20047). Written informed consent was obtained from all caregivers.

2.3. Data Collection

Data were collected in a town health centre and village clinics located in Shijiazhuang city, Hebei Province, China, between August and October 2020. A convenience sampling approach was used to recruit the participants. In the town health centre, caregivers who took their children for health check-ups or vaccination were approached and assessed for eligibility. In the village clinic, the village doctor contacted the targeted caregivers by telephone calls, and required them to come to the clinic together with their children if they were willing to participate in the survey. The eligibility of caregivers was assessed according to the inclusion criteria by trained research staff. The eligible caregivers were required to complete a survey through face-to-face interviews by trained research staff. The interview lasted approximately 20 min. After finishing the survey, the completeness of the data was checked, and a toy (approximately 5 CNY) was given to the participant.

2.4. Instruments

The questionnaire was used and included questions about the sociodemographic information of caregivers and their children, caregivers’ feeding practices, children’s eating behaviours, and children’s food frequency questionnaire (FFQ).

2.4.1. Sociodemographic Information

Sociodemographic information included caregiver’s education level, employment status, monthly household income, self-reported height and weight, caregiver’s relationship with the child, child’s age, and gender. Caregiver body mass index (BMI) was calculated. According to the criteria of adults’ weight status in China, caregivers’ BMI were classified as underweight, normal weight, overweight, and obese, respectively [27].

2.4.2. Caregivers’ Emotional and Instrumental Feeding Practices

Caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding was assessed by the subscale of Parents’ Feeding Practices Scale for Infant and Young Child (PFPSIYC). The PFPSIYC was developed to measure the feeding behaviours of Chinese caregivers with children aged 6-to-24 months, with acceptable validity and reliability (content validity index: 0.910, construct validity: χ2/df = 2.00, comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.915, non-normed fit index (NNFI) = 0.904, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = 0.046; Cronbach’s α = 0.783) [28]. The subscale contained five items (Table S1). Participants responded on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = never, 2 = rarely, 3 = sometimes, 4 = often, 5 = always). The average score of the subscale was calculated, and a higher score indicated a higher frequency of using the feeding practices. All items in the PFPSIYC were included into exploratory factor analysis conducted in the current sample. There were seven factors with eigenvalues more than one, which explained 55.5% of variances. The first factor explained 10.6% of variance, and contained five items with loading more than 0.4. These five items were same as the items in the emotional and instrumental feeding subscale of the original PFPSIYC. The subscale showed good internal reliability among the current sample (Cronbach’s α: 0.768).

2.4.3. Children’s Emotional Eating Behaviours

Children’s emotional eating behaviours were measured by the EOE subscale and EUE subscale of the Children’s Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (CEBQ) (Table S1). The CEBQ is a validated and world-widely-used tool reported by caregivers to measure their children’s eating behaviour using a 5-point Likert scale (1 = never, 2 = rarely, 3 = sometimes, 4 = often, 5 = always) [29]. The average scores of the EOE and EUE subscales were calculated, respectively. A higher score indicated a higher frequency of engaging in the eating behaviour. The Chinese translated version the CEBQ had good internal and test–retest reliability among caregivers of primary school children [30], and had good internal reliability (Cronbach’s α: 0.52–0.80) among caregivers with children aged 12–18 months old [31]. In the current study, the Cronbach’s α of the EOE and EUE subscales was 0.698 and 0.733, respectively.

2.4.4. Children’s UPF Consumption

Children’s food consumption in the last week was measured by the FFQ (29 items), which had been used in the Chinese Nutrition and Health Surveillance of children aged 0–2 years old in 2013 [32]. Caregivers reported whether their children consumed each food item, and times of consumption over the last week, and the average amount of food consumed each time. A food atlas including portion sizes of familiar foods eaten by local children was provided to facilitate the estimation of the amount of food consumed. The formula of times of food consumption in the last week multiplied by the average amount of each consumption was used to calculate the amount of food consumed in the last week by children.
All food items in the FFQ were categorised as UPF and non-UPF based on the NOVA food classification system [2] by two researchers (M.A. and X.L.) independently. The consistency of categorization was 90% (26/29). Any inconsistency was discussed with a third researcher (Q.Z.) to reach an agreement. In the present study, the UPF contained (1) sugar-sweetened beverages (e.g., carbonated drinks, fruit juice beverages, tea drinks, milk beverages, flavoured water drinks), (2) solid or semi-solid dairy products (e.g., cheeses, thick yogurts), (3) pastries (e.g., cakes, cookies, caramel treats, chocolate pies with egg yolk, waffles), (4) savoury packaged snacks (e.g., rice cakes, bugles), (5) confectioneries (e.g., candies, chocolates, jelly, hawthorn strips, preserved fruits), and (6) reconstituted meat products (e.g., hams, sausages). The consumption of UPF was defined as the consumption of at least one of the above UPF food items. The times of UPF consumption were defined as the summary of times of the above UPF food items that were consumed. The amount of UPF consumption in the last week was defined as the summary of the amount of the above UPF food items that were consumed. For the sugar-sweetened beverages, the amount in mL was transformed into g according to the estimated density of 1 g/mL.

2.5. Sample Size Estimation

Based on a correlation coefficient of 0.21 [33] in the relationship between emotional feeding and the frequency of pastry consumption among children aged one-to-three years old, and considering a power of 80% and a type 1 error of 5%, the sample size requirement for this study was 175. With a consideration of a 10% nonresponse rate, the sample size in this study was estimated to be 184.

2.6. Statistical Analysis

In the descriptive analysis, means and standard deviations (SD) were used to describe the normally distributed variables, and frequencies and percentages were used to illustrate the categorical variables. The frequencies and amount of UPF consumption per week were displayed as the median and interquartile range, respectively. The median values were further used to categorise the times and amount of UPF consumption into two groups. The associations between children’s UPF consumption and participants’ sociodemographic characteristics were assessed by chi-squared test. Variables with a p < 0.2 were adjusted as potential confounders. The independent effects of caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding and children’s emotional eating on children’s UPF consumption were calculated by multivariable logistic regression analyses. The relationships between caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding and children’s emotional eating were assessed by Pearson correlation analysis. The mediation effect of children’s emotional eating on the relationships between caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding and children’s UPF consumption was explored by the mediation package in R. All statistical analyses were performed by SPSS version 22.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA) and R version 3.6.2. Statistical significance was set at a p < 0.05.

3. Results

3.1. General Characteristics of the Participants

A total of 444 caregivers agreed to participate in this study, and 428 of them completed the questionnaire. Twenty caregiver–child pairs were excluded further. In total, data from 408 caregiver–child pairs were analysed (Figure 1). All caregivers included 309 parents (97.1% of those were mothers) and 99 nonparents (94.9% of those were grandmothers). The mean age was 30.2 (SD 3.9) years old for parents and 55.6 (SD 6.0) years old for nonparents (data not shown). The majority of the caregivers were unemployed (52.9%), had received senior middle school education or below (68.9%), and had a monthly household income of over 5000 CNY (60.5%). The mean age of children was 22.4 months old. Approximately half of the children were 25-to-36 months old (53.2%) and boys (52.2%). A total of 81.4% of children were introduced to complementary foods at 6–8 months old. The mean scores of caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding, children’s EOE, and EUE were 2.7, 2.1, and 2.9, respectively (Table 1).

3.2. Children’s Consumption of UPF

Among the children, 86.8% consumed UPF. The most frequently consumed UPF were pastries (63.5%), followed by solid or semi-solid dairy products (58.8%), and reconstituted meat products (56.4%) (Table 2). The percentage of UPF consumption was 73.8% for children aged 6–24 months old, and 98.2% for children aged 25–36 months old. With an increase in children’s age, an increasing trend of total UPF consumption and each UPF item were observed (p < 0.05) (Figure 2). The median times of the consumption of total UPF, solid or semi-solid dairy products, pastries, and reconstituted meat products per week were 9, 1, 2, and 1, respectively. The median amounts consumed per week were 365 g, 100 g, 30 g, and 19 g, respectively (Table 2).

3.3. Associations between Caregivers’ Emotional and Instrumental Feeding and Children’s Consumption of UPF

The univariate associations between participants’ sociodemographic characteristics and children’s consumption of UPF, and the frequencies and the amount of UPF consumption in a week are presented in Tables S2–S4. After controlling for confounders, a higher score of caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding was associated with higher odds of children’s consumption of UPF (OR = 1.59, 95% CI: 1.01, 2.49), sugar-sweetened beverages, solid or semi-solid dairy products, pastries, and savoury packaged snacks. Higher scores of children’s EUE were associated with children’s consumption of UPF (OR = 1.61, 95% CI: 1.07, 2.42) and reconstituted meat products. Children’s EOE and their consumption of UPF or each UPF item was not related (Table 3). Higher scores of caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding were associated with higher odds of children’s consumption of UPF ≥ nine times/week (OR = 1.80, 95% CI: 1.35, 2.39) and pastries ≥ two times/week. Higher scores of children’s EUE were associated with children’s consumption of UPF ≥ nine times/week (OR = 1.33, 95% CI: 1.03, 1.73) (Table 4). Higher scores of caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding were associated with higher odds of children’s consumption of UPF ≥ 365 g/week (OR = 1.85, 95%CI: 1.38, 2.49), solid or semi-solid dairy products ≥ 100 g/week, pastries ≥ 30 g/week, and reconstituted meat products ≥ 19 g/week. Higher scores of children’s EUE were associated with children’s consumption of reconstituted meat products ≥ 19 g/week (Table 5). Children’s EOE and the frequency or amount of UPF consumption in a week was not related (Table 4 and Table 5).

3.4. The Effect of Children’s Emotional Eating on the Relationship between Caregiver’s Emotional and Instrumental Feeding and Children’s Consumption of UPF Item

Caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding was positively associated with children’s EOE (r = 0.333, p < 0.001) and EUE (r = 0.354, p < 0.001) (data not shown). Children’s EUE significantly mediated the associations between caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding and children’s consumption of reconstituted meat products (effect: 0.026, 95%CI: 0.005, 0.050) (Table 6).

4. Discussion

This study reported a high percentage of UPF consumption (86.8%) among children aged 6-to-36 months old in China. The median times and amount of UPF consumption per week were nine times and 365 g, respectively. To our knowledge, this was the first study investigating the associations between caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding, children’s emotional eating, and the consumption of UPF among children aged 6-to-36 months old. Caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding was positively associated with children’s consumption behaviours of the total UPF, solid and semi-solid dairy products, and pastries. Children’s EUE was positively associated with the consumption and weekly consumption frequency of total UPF, as well as the consumption behaviours of reconstituted meat products. Moreover, children’s EUE mediated the associations between caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding and children’s consumption of reconstituted meat products.
The percentage of UPF consumption in our study was high and similar to the results from Brazilian and American cross-sectional studies among infants and toddlers [11,13,34]. The percentages of savoury packaged snack consumption and sugar-sweetened beverage consumption in our study were lower than those in Indonesia (81.6% and 40.0%) [12]. The median times of pastry and reconstituted meat product consumption among our sample were similar to those among children aged two-to-five years old in Hong Kong, China [35]. The median times of pastry consumption among our sample were less than those among children aged one-to-three years old in the Netherlands [33]. It appears that there were geographical and cultural variations in UPF consumption. The mean scores of emotional and instrumental feeding among our study participants were similar to those of parents in three cities (Harbin City, Xiamen City, and Xi’an City, 2.8 ± 0.7) in mainland China [28], and parents in Hong Kong [35].
Similar to studies among older children in the Netherlands, the UK, and the US [17,18,19,20,21], and studies among younger children in the Netherlands and Hong Kong, China [33,35], our study demonstrated that caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding was positively associated with children’s UPF consumption and the frequency and amount of weekly consumption. In the context of emotional and instrumental feeding practices, caregivers preferred to offer UPF, such as confectioneries, pastries, and savoury packaged snacks, to comfort children’s distress, and to reward or punish children’s behaviours among children aged 4-to-12 years old in the Netherlands [16]. More studies investigating food types provided in the context of emotional and instrumental feeding in younger children under three years old are needed. Emotional and instrumental feeding might not change children’s preference for rewarded/punished behaviours [36]. However, children might be misled to consume foods owing to their performance and their bad emotions, rather than their own hunger/satiety cues [37,38]. Frequent use of emotional and instrumental feeding might increase children’s intake of UPF. A cross-sectional study in the US showed that children aged three-to-seven years old preferred high fat and sugar foods if parents used emotional or instrumental feeding [39]. Another study among children 6–12 years old demonstrated that caregivers using food as a reinforcer to control children’s behaviour was associated with an increased consumption of that reinforcing food [40]. Caregivers should be advised to avoid using emotional and instrumental feeding strategies.
In our study, children with higher scores of EUE were positively related to UPF consumption and the frequency of UPF consumption. Likewise, children with higher scores of EUE were positively related to reconstituted meat product consumption and their amount of consumption weekly. EUE was children’s coping strategy for negative emotions. When experiencing negative emotions, children were likely to eat less and consume specific UPF, such as reconstituted meat products, to meet their satiety needs. However, a study in the UK showed that in negative mood conditions, children characterised as experiencing EUE consumed less energy from crisps or potato chips [41]. The consumption of chips or crisps appeared to be more associated with good mood and leisurable conditions, rather than negative moods. Our results, together with the above the UK findings, might imply that EUE only affects the intake of specific foods (such as reconstituted meat products), but not the total dietary intake. In the future, the dietary energy or the food type consumed by children in the context of emotional undereating should be investigated to examine this hypothesis.
Our study was consistent with two studies in China showing that caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding were positively associated with children’s EUE [42,43]. The reason might be that those feeding practices increase children’s expression of bad moods by avoiding foods [43]. In the context of emotional and instrumental feeding, food serves as a response to negative emotions, as opposed to physiological needs. In the long run, caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding practices might impair children’s capability of emotion regulation, and increase children’s emotional eating behaviours [44]. Furthermore, our study was novel in demonstrating a mediation effect of children’s EUE on the association between caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding and children’s consumption of reconstituted meat products. It is likely that when exposed to emotional and instrumental feeding, children were feeling negative emotions and tended to eat less. Therefore, they chose reconstituted meat products that have a fast satiety effect to eat less. EUE was more likely to be associated with satisfying satiety rather than enjoying the meal occasion. Further studies to examine the above mechanism are warranted.
Finally, we failed to find a significant association between EOE and UPF consumption, which is similar to a study in the UK that found EOE among children aged 34–59 months old was not related to their snack food intake, regardless of their mood conditions [41]. However, our results contradicted findings among older children aged 9-to-11 years old in other countries [25]. This might be because children’s EOE increased with age. Between the ages of four and ten years old, children’s EOE increased, whereas EUE decreased [23]. The low prevalence of EOE among younger children might contribute to its insignificant association with UPF consumption.
Our findings added to the literature that caregivers’ nonresponsive feeding practices and children’s emotional eating increased children’s UPF consumption. There are some limitations. First, the cross-sectional design made it difficult to draw a causal conclusion. Second, the self-reported feeding and eating behaviours may elicit recall bias. When investigated, caregivers found it difficult to fill in the questionnaire independently due to looking after their children (e.g., soothing the child after vaccination, holding the child, and paying attention to the child’s safety), and the decreased vision or lower education level of elderly caregivers. Therefore, the method of face-to-face interview by trained research staff was used. Although trained research staff reminded the caregiver to feel free and respond as honestly as possible before the interview, the face-to-face interview may result in social desirability bias and reporting bias. Third, our findings might not be generalizable to children in other areas owing to the convenience sampling method.

5. Conclusions

The percentage of UPF consumption was high among children under three years of age in the current sample. Caregivers frequently adopting emotional and instrumental feeding practices were positively associated with children’s consumption of UPF, as well as most UPF subgroups. Children’s EUE was positively related to their consumption of reconstituted meat product, as well as a higher weekly amount of reconstituted meat product consumption. Children’s EUE mediated the associations between caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding and children’s consumption of reconstituted meat products. Longitudinal studies are warranted to confirm the findings. In the prevention of noncommunicable diseases in childhood and adulthood, caregivers should be educated to avoid emotional and instrumental feeding, and to help children develop healthy eating behaviours and dietary patterns.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ijerph19084439/s1: Table S1: Scores of scale of items of emotional and instrumental feeding and children’s emotional eating (n = 408); Table S2: UPF consumption across different sociodemographic characteristics, by chi-squared test (n = 408); Table S3: Frequency of UPF consumption per week across different sociodemographic characteristics, by chi-squared test (n = 408); Table S4: Amount of UPF consumption in a week across different sociodemographic characteristics, by chi-squared test (n = 408).

Author Contributions

Conceptualisation, Q.Z.; methodology, Q.Z. and M.A.; validation, Q.Z. and X.L.; formal analysis, M.A.; investigation, M.A., H.G. and X.L.; resources, H.G.; data curation, M.A. and Q.Z.; writing—original draft preparation, M.A.; writing—review and editing, Q.Z. and X.L.; visualization, M.A. and X.L.; supervision, Q.Z.; project administration, Q.Z.; funding acquisition, Q.Z. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Peking University Research Initiation Fund, grant number BMU2018YJ005.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the Peking University (IRB00001052-20047).

Informed Consent Statement

Written informed consent was obtained from all caregivers.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge the participation of all the study participants.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Monteiro, C.; Cannon, G.; Levy, R.; Moubarac, J.C.; Jaime, P.; Al, A. NOVA. The star shines bright. World Nutr. 2016, 7, 28–38. [Google Scholar]
  2. Monteiro, C.A.; Cannon, G.; Levy, R.B.; Moubarac, J.C.; Louzada, M.L.; Rauber, F.; Khandpur, N.; Cediel, G.; Neri, D.; Martinez-Steele, E.; et al. Ultra-processed foods: What they are and how to identify them. Public Health Nutr. 2019, 22, 936–941. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Spaniol, A.M.; da Costa, T.H.M.; Souza, A.M.; Gubert, M.B. Early consumption of ultra-processed foods among children under 2 years old in Brazil. Public Health Nutr. 2021, 24, 3341–3351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Batalha, M.A.; França, A.; Conceição, S.; Santos, A.M.D.; Silva, F.S.; Padilha, L.L.; Silva, A. Processed and ultra-processed food consumption among children aged 13 to 35 months and associated factors. Cad. De Saude Publica 2017, 33, e00152016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Pagliai, G.; Dinu, M.; Madarena, M.P.; Bonaccio, M.; Iacoviello, L.; Sofi, F. Consumption of ultra-processed foods and health status: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Br. J. Nutr. 2021, 125, 308–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Leffa, P.S.; Hoffman, D.J.; Rauber, F.; Sangalli, C.N.; Valmórbida, J.L.; Vitolo, M.R. Longitudinal associations between ultra-processed foods and blood lipids in childhood. Br. J. Nutr. 2020, 124, 341–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. de Souza, M.S.; Vaz, J.D.S.; Martins-Silva, T.; Bomfim, R.A.; Cascaes, A.M. Ultra-processed foods and early childhood caries in 0–3-year-olds enrolled at Primary Healthcare Centers in Southern Brazil. Public Health Nutr. 2021, 24, 3322–3330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Powell, F.; Farrow, C.; Meyer, C.; Haycraft, E. The stability and continuity of maternally reported and observed child eating behaviours and feeding practices across early childhood. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 1017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Christoph, M.J.; Larson, N.I.; Winkler, M.R.; Wall, M.M.; Neumark-Sztainer, D. Longitudinal trajectories and prevalence of meeting dietary guidelines during the transition from adolescence to young adulthood. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2019, 109, 656–664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Fangupo, L.J.; Haszard, J.J.; Taylor, B.J.; Gray, A.R.; Lawrence, J.A.; Taylor, R.W. Ultra-processed food intake and associations with demographic factors in young New Zealand children. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet 2021, 121, 305–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Lopes, W.C.; Pinho, L.; Caldeira, A.P.; Lessa, A.D.C. Consumption of ultra-processed foods by children under 24 months of age and associated factors. Rev. Paul. Pediatr. 2020, 38, e2018277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Green, M.; Hadihardjono, D.N.; Pries, A.M.; Izwardy, D.; Zehner, E.; Huffman, S.L. High proportions of children under 3 years of age consume commercially produced snack foods and sugar-sweetened beverages in Bandung City, Indonesia. Matern. Child Nutr. 2019, 15 (Suppl. S4), e12764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Fox, M.K.; Condon, E.; Briefel, R.R.; Reidy, K.C.; Deming, D.M. Food consumption patterns of young preschoolers: Are they starting off on the right path? J. Am. Diet Assoc. 2010, 110, S52–S59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. United Nations International Children’s Emergency Fund. The Market for Highly Processed Food and Drink: Driving Children’s Diets. 2020. Available online: https://www.unicef.cn/en/reports/market-highly-processed-food-and-drink-driving-childrens-diets-briefing (accessed on 24 February 2021).
  15. Heller, R.L.; Mobley, A.R. Instruments assessing parental responsive feeding in children ages birth to 5 years: A systematic review. Appetite 2019, 138, 23–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Raaijmakers, L.G.; Gevers, D.W.; Teuscher, D.; Kremers, S.P.; van Assema, P. Emotional and instrumental feeding practices of Dutch mothers regarding foods eaten between main meals. BMC Public Health 2014, 14, 171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Rodenburg, G.; Kremers, S.P.; Oenema, A.; van de Mheen, D. Associations of parental feeding styles with child snacking behaviour and weight in the context of general parenting. Public Health Nutr. 2014, 17, 960–969. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Sleddens, E.F.; Kremers, S.P.; Stafleu, A.; Dagnelie, P.C.; De Vries, N.K.; Thijs, C. Food parenting practices and child dietary behavior. Prospective relations and the moderating role of general parenting. Appetite 2014, 79, 42–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Sleddens, E.F.; Kremers, S.P.; De Vries, N.K.; Thijs, C. Relationship between parental feeding styles and eating behaviours of Dutch children aged 6–7. Appetite 2010, 54, 30–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Blissett, J.; Haycraft, E.; Farrow, C. Inducing preschool children’s emotional eating: Relations with parental feeding practices. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2010, 92, 359–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Lora, K.R.; Hubbs-Tait, L.; Ferris, A.M.; Wakefield, D. African-American and Hispanic children’s beverage intake: Differences in associations with desire to drink, fathers’ feeding practices, and weight concerns. Appetite 2016, 107, 558–567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Faith, M.S.; Allison, D.B.; Geliebter, A. Emotional eating and obesity: Theoretical considerations and practical recommendations. In Overweight and Weight Management: The Health Professional’s Guide to Understanding and Practice; Aspen Publishers: Gaithersburg, MD, USA, 1997; pp. 439–465. [Google Scholar]
  23. Ashcroft, J.; Semmler, C.; Carnell, S.; van Jaarsveld, C.H.; Wardle, J. Continuity and stability of eating behaviour traits in children. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2008, 62, 985–990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Herle, M.; Fildes, A.; Llewellyn, C.H. Emotional eating is learned not inherited in children, regardless of obesity risk. Pediatr. Obes. 2018, 13, 628–631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Jalo, E.; Konttinen, H.; Vepsäläinen, H.; Chaput, J.P.; Hu, G.; Maher, C.; Maia, J.; Sarmiento, O.L.; Standage, M.; Tudor-Locke, C.; et al. Emotional eating, health behaviours, and obesity in children: A 12-country cross-sectional study. Nutrients 2019, 11, 351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. Rodgers, R.F.; Paxton, S.J.; Massey, R.; Campbell, K.J.; Wertheim, E.H.; Skouteris, H.; Gibbons, K. Maternal feeding practices predict weight gain and obesogenic eating behaviors in young children: A prospective study. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2013, 10, 24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. National Health Commission of the People’s Republic of China. Criteria of Weight for Adults. 2013. Available online: http://www.moh.gov.cn/zwgkzt/yingyang/201308/a233d450fdbc47c5ad4f08b7e394d1e8.shtml (accessed on 8 February 2021).
  28. Xie, L. Development and Evaluation of Parents’ Feeding Practice Scale for Infant and Young Child. Master’s Thesis, Capital Institute of Pediatrics, Beijing, China, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  29. Wardle, J.; Guthrie, C.A.; Sanderson, S.; Rapoport, L. Development of the Children’s Eating Behaviour Questionnaire. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2001, 42, 963–970. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  30. Xue, K. Research on the Relationship and Risk Factors of Childhood Obesity and Appetite. Ph.D. Thesis, Fudan University, Shanghai, China, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  31. Cao, Y.T.; Svensson, V.; Marcus, C.; Zhang, J.; Zhang, J.D.; Sobko, T. Eating behaviour patterns in Chinese children aged 12–18 months and association with relative weight--factorial validation of the Children’s Eating Behaviour Questionnaire. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2012, 9, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Ding, X. Research on the Growth and Development Status of Infants and Young Children within 2 Years of Age in China in 2013 and Its Relationship with Feeding Behaviors. Master’s Thesis, Chinese Center for Disease Control and Prevention, Beijing, China, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  33. Gubbels, J.S.; Gerards, S.M.; Kremers, S.P. The association of parenting practices with toddlers’ dietary intake and BMI, and the moderating role of general parenting and child temperament. Public Health Nutr. 2020, 23, 2521–2529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Marçal, G.M.; Mendes, M.M.E.; Fragoso, M.; Florêncio, T.; Bueno, N.B.; Clemente, A.P.G. Association between the consumption of ultra-processed foods and the practice of breast-feeding in children under 2 years of age who are beneficiaries of the conditional cash transfer programme, Bolsa Família. Public Health Nutr. 2021, 24, 3313–3321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Lo, K.; Cheung, C.; Lee, A.; Tam, W.W.; Keung, V. Associations between parental feeding styles and childhood eating habits: A survey of Hong Kong pre-school children. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0124753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Birch, L.L.; Birch, D.; Marlin, D.W.; Kramer, L. Effects of instrumental consumption on children’s food preference. Appetite 1982, 3, 125–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Mallan, K.; Miller, N. Effect of parental feeding practices (i.e., responsive feeding) on children’s eating behavior. Nestle Nutr. Inst. Workshop Ser. 2019, 91, 21–30. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  38. Stifter, C.A.; Moding, K.J. Understanding and measuring parent use of food to soothe infant and toddler distress: A longitudinal study from 6 to 18 months of age. Appetite 2015, 95, 188–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  39. Vollmer, R.L.; Baietto, J. Practices and preferences: Exploring the relationships between food-related parenting practices and child food preferences for high fat and/or sugar foods, fruits, and vegetables. Appetite 2017, 113, 134–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Lu, J.; Xiong, S.; Arora, N.; Dubé, L. Using food as reinforcer to shape children’s non-food behavior: The adverse nutritional effect doubly moderated by reward sensitivity and gender. Eat Behav. 2015, 19, 94–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Blissett, J.; Farrow, C.; Haycraft, E. Relationships between observations and parental reports of 3–5 year old children’s emotional eating using the Children’s Eating Behaviour Questionnaire. Appetite 2019, 141, 104323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Guo, S.; Xie, S.; Xiong, Y.; Zhang, J. Study on the correlation between dietary behaviours of children aged two years old and gender and maternal feeding styles. Matern. Child Health Care China 2017, 32, 85–88. [Google Scholar]
  43. Liu, H.; Chen, J. Effect of parental feeding behaviour on eating behaviour of children aged 1–3 years. Chin. J. Contemp. Pediatr. 2014, 16, 643–647. [Google Scholar]
  44. Christensen, K.A. Emotional feeding as interpersonal emotion regulation: A developmental risk factor for binge-eating behaviors. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2019, 52, 515–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Flowchart of the participants.
Figure 1. Flowchart of the participants.
Ijerph 19 04439 g001
Figure 2. The consumption of UPF by children aged 6–24 months (n = 191) and children aged 25–36 months (n = 217). UPF: ultra-processed foods. The percentages of consumption of UPF, as well as each UPF item, were different between children aged 6–24 months and children aged 25–36 months (p < 0.05).
Figure 2. The consumption of UPF by children aged 6–24 months (n = 191) and children aged 25–36 months (n = 217). UPF: ultra-processed foods. The percentages of consumption of UPF, as well as each UPF item, were different between children aged 6–24 months and children aged 25–36 months (p < 0.05).
Ijerph 19 04439 g002
Table 1. Sociodemographic characteristics of the participants, caregivers’ feeding practices, and children’s emotional eating behaviours (n = 408).
Table 1. Sociodemographic characteristics of the participants, caregivers’ feeding practices, and children’s emotional eating behaviours (n = 408).
Characteristicsn (%)/Mean (SD)
Caregiver’s relationship with the child
Parent309 (75.7)
Nonparent99 (24.3)
Caregiver’s education level
Senior middle school or below281 (68.9)
College or above127 (31.1)
Caregiver’s employment status
Unemployed216 (52.9)
Employed192 (47.1)
Caregiver’s weight status †
Underweight18 (4.5)
Normal206 (51.7)
Overweight116 (29.0)
Obesity59 (14.8)
Monthly household income
≤5000 CNY161 (39.5)
>5000 CNY247 (60.5)
Child’s age
6–24 months191 (46.8)
25–36 months217 (53.2)
Child’s gender
Boy213 (52.2)
Girl195 (47.8)
Timing of complementary feeding †
<6 months62 (15.2)
6–8 months332 (81.4)
>8 months13 (3.2)
Caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding ‡2.7 (0.8)
Children’s emotional overeating ‡2.1 (0.6)
Children’s emotional undereating ‡2.9 (0.8)
† Variable with missing values. ‡ Assessed by a 5-point Likert scale, ranged from 1 to 5. A higher score indicated a higher frequency of the practice.
Table 2. Children’s consumption of UPF (n = 408).
Table 2. Children’s consumption of UPF (n = 408).
UPF ItemsPercentage of Consumption
n (%)
Times of Consumption in a WeekAmount of Consumption in a Week (in g)
Median (IQR)≥Median
n (%)
Median (IQR)≥Median
n (%)
UPF354 (86.8)9 (4–14)210 (51.5)365.0 (62.0–813.8)204 (50.0)
Sugar-sweetened beverages82 (20.1)0 (0–0)-0.0 (0.0–0.0)-
Solid or semi-solid dairy products240 (58.8)1 (0–5)-100.0 (0.0–500.0)220 (53.9)
Pastries259 (63.5)2 (0–5)217 (53.2)30.0 (0.0–136.3)205 (50.2)
Savoury packaged snacks146 (35.8)0 (0–2)-0.0 (0.0–40.0)-
Confectioneries156 (38.2)0 (0–2)-0.0 (0.0–20.0)-
Reconstituted meat products230 (56.4)1 (0–3)-19.0 (0.0–70.0)204 (50.0)
UPF: ultra-processed foods; IQR: interquartile range. The estimated density of sugar-sweetened beverages is 1 g/mL. -: Not categorised.
Table 3. The associations between children’s UPF consumption and caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding, as well as children’s emotional eating (n = 408).
Table 3. The associations between children’s UPF consumption and caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding, as well as children’s emotional eating (n = 408).
VariablesUPF aSugar-Sweetened Beverages bSolid or Semi-Solid Dairy Products cPastries d
Emotional and instrumental feeding
Crude OR (95%CI)1.37 (0.95,1.98)1.68 (1.20,2.36) **1.54 (1.18,2.00) **1.37 (1.06,1.79) *
Adjusted OR (95%CI)1.59 (1.01,2.49) *1.64 (1.17,2.31) **1.61 (1.21,2.16) **1.49 (1.12,1.99) **
Emotional overeating
Crude OR (95%CI)0.90 (0.58,1.40)1.05 (0.72,1.53)0.88 (0.65,1.19)1.08 (0.79,1.48)
Adjusted OR (95%CI)1.25 (0.76,2.04)1.10 (0.74,1.63)1.04 (0.75,1.46)1.29 (0.92,1.81)
Emotional undereating
Crude OR (95%CI)1.45 (1.03,2.04) *1.26 (0.92,1.70)1.21 (0.95,1.54)1.19 (0.93,1.52)
Adjusted OR (95%CI)1.61 (1.07,2.42) *1.24 (0.91,1.69)1.25 (0.97,1.63)1.20 (0.93,1.55)
Savoury Packaged Snacks eConfectioneries fReconstituted Meat Products g
Emotional and instrumental feeding
Crude OR (95%CI)1.60 (1.21,2.11) **1.27 (0.98,1.66)1.22 (0.95,1.58)
Adjusted OR (95%CI)1.58 (1.20,2.09) **1.28 (0.96,1.70)1.31 (0.99,1.74)
Emotional overeating
Crude OR (95%CI)1.12 (0.82,1.53)0.89 (0.65,1.21)1.04 (0.77,1.40)
Adjusted OR (95%CI)1.20 (0.87,1.66)1.12 (0.79,1.59)1.28 (0.91,1.79)
Emotional undereating
Crude OR (95%CI)1.12 (0.87,1.43)1.14 (0.89,1.46)1.40 (1.10,1.79) **
Adjusted OR (95%CI)1.12 (0.87,1.43)1.15 (0.88,1.51)1.46 (1.12,1.90) **
UPF: ultra-processed foods; OR: odds ratio; CI: confidence interval. a In the adjusted model (presented by adjusted OR), child’s age, caregiver’s relationship with the child, caregiver’s employment status, and monthly household income were controlled as potential confounders. b In the adjusted model (presented by adjusted OR), child’s age, caregiver’s weight status, and monthly household income were controlled as potential confounders. c In the adjusted model (presented by adjusted OR), child’s age, timing of complementary feeding, caregiver’s relationship with the child, and monthly household income were controlled as potential confounders. d In the adjusted model (presented by adjusted OR), child’s age, child’s gender, timing of complementary feeding, caregiver’s relationship with the child, and monthly household income were controlled as potential confounders. e In the adjusted model (presented by adjusted OR), child’s age was controlled as the potential confounder. f In the adjusted model (presented by adjusted OR), child’s age and caregiver’s weight status were controlled as potential confounders. g In the adjusted model (presented by adjusted OR), child’s age, caregiver’s relationship with the child, caregiver’s education level, employment status, and weight status were controlled as potential confounders. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Table 4. The associations between children’s frequencies of UPF consumption and caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding, as well as children’s emotional eating (n = 408).
Table 4. The associations between children’s frequencies of UPF consumption and caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding, as well as children’s emotional eating (n = 408).
VariablesUPF (≥9 times/week) aPastries (≥2 times/week) b
Emotional and instrumental feeding
Crude OR (95%CI)1.71 (1.31,2.23) ***1.29 (1.00,1.67) *
Adjusted OR (95%CI)1.80 (1.35,2.39) ***1.36 (1.03,1.78) *
Emotional overeating
Crude OR (95%CI)1.05 (0.78,1.41)1.04 (0.77,1.40)
Adjusted OR (95%CI)1.30 (0.93,1.81)1.20 (0.87,1.65)
Emotional undereating
Crude OR (95%CI)1.29 (1.02,1.64) *1.03 (0.81,1.31)
Adjusted OR (95%CI)1.33 (1.03,1.73) *1.04 (0.81,1.33)
UPF: ultra-processed foods; OR: odds ratio; CI: confidence interval. a In the adjusted model (presented by adjusted OR), child’s age, caregiver employment status, weight status, and monthly household income were controlled for as potential confounders. b In the adjusted model (presented by adjusted OR), child’s age, gender, caregiver relationship with the child, weight status, and household monthly income were controlled for as potential confounders. * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001.
Table 5. The associations between children’s amount of UPF consumption and caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding, as well as children’s emotional eating (n = 408).
Table 5. The associations between children’s amount of UPF consumption and caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding, as well as children’s emotional eating (n = 408).
VariablesUPF (≥365 g/week) aSolid or Semi-Solid Dairy Products (≥100 g/week) bPastries (≥30 g/week) cReconstituted Meat Products (≥19 g/week) d
Emotional and instrumental feeding
Crude OR (95%CI)1.70 (1.30,2.21) ***1.50 (1.16,1.95) **1.47 (1.14,1.91) **1.30 (1.01,1.68)
Adjusted OR (95%CI)1.85 (1.38,2.49) ***1.55 (1.16,2.07) **1.54 (1.16,2.02) **1.38 (1.04,1.81) *
Emotional overeating
Crude OR (95%CI)0.77 (0.57,1.05)0.89 (0.66,1.20)0.92 (0.68,1.25)0.98 (0.72,1.32)
Adjusted OR (95%CI)0.90 (0.64,1.26)1.06 (0.76,1.48)1.11 (0.80,1.53)1.17 (0.84,1.63)
Emotional undereating
Crude OR (95%CI)1.20 (0.94,1.52)1.19 (0.94,1.51)1.08 (0.85,1.37)1.35 (1.06,1.72) *
Adjusted OR (95%CI)1.25 (0.96,1.63)1.23 (0.95,1.59)1.07 (0.84,1.38)1.37 (1.06,1.78) *
UPF: ultra-processed foods; OR: odds ratio; CI: confidence interval. a In the adjusted model (presented by adjusted OR), child’s age, caregiver’s relationship with the child, education level, and employment status were controlled as potential confounders. b In the adjusted model (presented by adjusted OR), child’s age, caregiver’s relationship with the child, and monthly household income were controlled as potential confounders. c In the adjusted model (presented by adjusted OR), child’s age, caregiver’s relationship with the child, and weight status were controlled as potential confounders. d In the adjusted model (presented by adjusted OR), child’s age, caregiver’s relationship with the child, education level, employment status, and weight status were controlled as potential confounders. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table 6. The mediation effect of children’s emotional undereating on the associations between caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding and children’s consumption of reconstituted meat products (n = 408).
Table 6. The mediation effect of children’s emotional undereating on the associations between caregivers’ emotional and instrumental feeding and children’s consumption of reconstituted meat products (n = 408).
PathIndirect Effect EstimateBootstrappingBias Corrected 95%CI
Emotional and instrumental feeding → Emotional undereating → Reconstituted meat products a 0.026 *0.005, 0.050
Emotional and instrumental feeding → Emotional undereating → Reconstituted meat products (≥19 g/week) b0.020−0.002, 0.040
The calculation of indirect effect estimate used the mediation package in R, and is based on the mediator model and outcome model. In the mediator model, a linear regression model was conducted with emotional undereating as outcome variable, and emotional and instrumental feeding as independent variable. In the outcome model, a logistic regression model was conducted with the consumption of reconstituted meat products as outcome variable, and emotional and instrumental feeding, and emotional undereating as independent variables. CI: confidence interval. a child’s age, caregiver’s relationship with the child, caregiver’s education level, employment status, and weight status were adjusted in both the mediator and outcome models. b child’s age, caregiver’s relationship with the child, caregiver’s education level, employment status, and weight status were adjusted in both the mediator and outcome models. * p < 0.05.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

An, M.; Liu, X.; Guo, H.; Zhou, Q. The Associations between Caregivers’ Emotional and Instrumental Feeding, Children’s Emotional Eating, and Children’s Consumption of Ultra-Processed Foods in China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 4439. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19084439

AMA Style

An M, Liu X, Guo H, Zhou Q. The Associations between Caregivers’ Emotional and Instrumental Feeding, Children’s Emotional Eating, and Children’s Consumption of Ultra-Processed Foods in China. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(8):4439. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19084439

Chicago/Turabian Style

An, Meijing, Xiyao Liu, Hao Guo, and Qianling Zhou. 2022. "The Associations between Caregivers’ Emotional and Instrumental Feeding, Children’s Emotional Eating, and Children’s Consumption of Ultra-Processed Foods in China" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 8: 4439. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19084439

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop