Next Article in Journal
Deep Dive into the Long Haul: Analysis of Symptom Clusters and Risk Factors for Post-Acute Sequelae of COVID-19 to Inform Clinical Care
Previous Article in Journal
Eating Behaviours in Sportswomen from the Silesian Training in Different Sports Disciplines
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Cognitive Strategies: Moderating the Relationship between Executive Functions and Daily Functioning

Department of Occupational Therapy, Ariel University, Ariel 40700, Israel
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(24), 16845; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192416845
Submission received: 15 November 2022 / Revised: 13 December 2022 / Accepted: 14 December 2022 / Published: 15 December 2022

Abstract

:
Whereas prior studies have addressed relationships between cognitive strategies and learning and achievement, very few dealt with their connection to daily functioning. This study examines the moderation effect of the frequency of compensatory cognitive strategy use within that relationship among university students. A sample of 336 students (18–36 years; 180 women, 156 men) answered the Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX; executive function components), Time Organization and Participation Scale (TOPS; daily functioning), and Compensatory Cognitive Strategies Scale (CCSS; strategy use). The results showed significant correlations between the DEX and TOPS for three CCSS levels (−1.0, −1.0 to 1.0, and 1.0 SD from average); the higher the frequency of cognitive strategy use, the stronger the association between the DEX and TOPS. The findings suggest that more frequently use of cognitive strategies can strengthen efficient daily functioning.

1. Introduction

Successful academic performance requires cognitive strategies, such as setting reminders, minimizing distractions, planning and prioritizing a “to-do” list, notetaking, and developing new study strategies to remember important material. For students with or without cognitive weaknesses or impairments, compensatory cognitive strategy use can help successful academic performance during postsecondary studies [1].
Strategies are a part of typical learning and performance [2] and inherent in everyone’s daily occupational performance [3]. They can be described as “mind tools” that assist in acquiring new information, enhancing understanding, monitoring performance, and coping with cognitively challenging or complex situations [4]. With the help of strategies, people can take in and integrate information more deeply and increase the efficient use and allocation of cognitive resources [4].
According to Toglia’s [3] dynamic interactional model, appropriate strategy selection increases the efficient use of information-processing resources, and such strategic behavior reflects intact executive functions (EF). All strategies enhance information processing and allow tasks to be completed with less effort and more efficiency. Strategy generation occurs when a person faces a complex or novel situation or task. A task is considered complex if it takes place over long periods, requires the choice of a solution from among many possibilities, or occurs in a dynamic, real-world environment where the examiner does not control numerous environmental variables [5]. The suggested (or generated) cognitive strategies can be combined, adjusted, or modified to help cope with cognitive challenges and support effective functioning [6].
Cognitive strategies are a particular class of strategies involved in all activities requiring EF. Executive functions encompass a diverse array of goal-directed functions, such as problem-solving, cognitive flexibility, planning, monitoring, decision-making, working memory, and inhibitory control [7]. The need for EFs to complete the activity depends on the task’s complexity and the degree of familiarity and structure provided [8]. Components of EF can be observed when people carry out daily life activities that require time multitasking, goal formulation, or specific activity sequences for successful completion; face changing rules and must be flexible or change strategies; or must ignore competing stimuli to maintain goal-directed activities [9,10].
Many researchers have noted differences among students in cognition and the application of cognitive strategies. Proctor [11] pointed out a significant relationship of students’ cognitive-strategy ability (especially learning information processing speed, comprehension and understanding power, and working memory) with learning and academic achievement, especially in mathematics. Ganbari-Taleb et al. [12] showed the effectiveness of cognitive strategies in improving academic functioning and positive attitudes toward learning.
One reason for students’ disabilities in the educational process relates to weak awareness and use of cognitive strategies. The evidence has suggested that a learner can improve performance by modifying strategies, and unlike poor learners, good learners spontaneously apply deep processing strategies [2]. The ability to organize and execute daily life tasks within time constitutes an essential component of efficiency and may influence a person’s participation and success in life events [13].
Studies about the need and role of cognitive strategies and their relationships to EF and daily functioning were conducted mainly among populations with learning or attention difficulties [14] and traumatic brain injury [5]. Despite demonstrated associations between EF and daily performance, little of the literature addresses the role of cognitive strategies in moderating these relationships [15,16].
Therefore, this study’s main objectives were to characterize the frequency of cognitive strategies among students and explore the strategies’ contributions to the relationship between EF and daily functioning (Figure 1). The main hypothesis is that a high frequency of cognitive strategy use will strengthen the relationship.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Using G*Power software guidelines, the author determined a minimum sample size of 169 participants considering the medium effect size of 0.15, power = 95, and α = 0.05 [17]. A total of 336 students (180 women, 156 men) aged 18 to 40 years from universities across Israel invited to participated in the study. Inclusion criteria were (1) students who could read and understand the Hebrew language (the questionnaires were presented to the participants in Hebrew), (2) students enrolled in Israeli universities, and (3) between the ages of 18 and 40 years. Students with disabilities (motor, mental, cognitive, communication, neurodevelopmental, etc.) were asked to report them when completing the demographic questionnaire.

2.2. Procedure

Following ethics committee approval, participants were recruited via advertisements to local online students’ groups and social media. Interested participants who met the inclusion criteria signed online consent forms and then were provided links to the online questionnaires (Demographic Questionnaire, Compensatory Cognitive Strategies Scale [CCSS], Dysexecutive Questionnaire [DEX], and Time Organisation and Participation Scale [TOPS]).

2.3. Measures

2.3.1. Demographic Questionnaire

The author developed a short demographic questionnaire for this study. It included characteristics such as age, gender, family status, field of study at the university, grade range in the last year, whether employed while studying, and previous first degree. It also asked about motor (e.g., cerebral palsy, motor clumsiness) or other physical disabilities, mental disabilities (anxiety, depression, sleep disorders, mental disorders such as schizophrenia), cognitive disability (after a stroke or head injury, etc.), communication disability with some language difficulty (lack of fluency, dyspraxia), autistic spectrum disorder, or neurodevelopmental disorder (e.g., learning disability, attention deficit disorder, or motor clumsiness).

2.3.2. Compensatory Cognitive Strategies Scale

The CCSS [18] is a 20-item questionnaire on how frequently participants use compensatory cognitive strategies in their everyday lives. It queries strategies related to processing speed and attention, memory, and EF. Participants are instructed, “Please rate how frequently you use each strategy to complete cognitive tasks (e.g., those involving thinking, attention, memory) in your daily life.” They rate each item’s frequency of use on a 5-point Likert-type scale of 0 (never), 1 (rarely), 2 (sometimes), 3 (often), and 4 (all the time). Higher scores mean the strategy is used with more frequency; lower scores mean less frequency.
For patients with multiple sclerosis, previous research has shown acceptable internal consistency for the CCSS total score (α = 0.90) and evidence of convergent and discriminant validity [18]. With the CCSS author’s permission, the questionnaire was translated into Hebrew for this study. The questionnaire’s internal reliability for this study was 0.81

2.3.3. Dysexecutive Questionnaire

The DEX [19] examines individuals’ functional management levels in coping with day-to-day functioning. The questionnaire is part of the Behavioral Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome assessment but stands on its own. It contains two versions, one for family members and one for self-reports. Only the self-report was used in this study.
The DEX includes 20 statements graded on a 5-point Likert scale from 0 (never) to 4 (very often) and divided into four areas: emotion/personality, motivation, behavior, and cognition. The final score is the mean of all 20 items; higher scores indicate a higher incidence of EF challenges in daily life. The DEX is sensitive and ecologically valid for identifying symptoms that characterize executive dysfunction in healthy adults and in various neurological and psychiatric disorders. The questionnaire has good reliability and validity [20]. Its internal reliability for the current study was 0.89.

2.3.4. Time Organisation and Participation Scale

The TOPS [13] is a standardized self-reported scale to assess respondents’ difficulties in time organization while performing daily tasks. It is divided into four parts (35 items): (A) the pace of daily task performance (20 items) rated from 5 (always) to 1 (never); (B) how activities are organized throughout the day (five items) rated from 5 (excellent) to 1 (very bad); (C) the frequency of emotional responses following disorganization in time (eight items), rated from 5 (never) to 1 (always); and (D) two open-ended questions: (1) Do you have difficulty organizing your daily activities on time when a change occurs in a familiar routine? (2) Are you distracted by various stimuli and hence do not succeed in finishing your tasks on time? Respondents rate the frequency of the two-Part D items as 5 (never), 4 (rarely), 3 (sometimes), 2 (usually), or 1 (always).
The item scores are averaged to calculate the TOPS total score in the Parts A through D domains; higher total scores indicate better daily functioning. The questionnaire has good reliability and validity [14]. Internal reliability for this study was 0.94.

2.4. Data Analysis

The data were analyzed using IBM SPSS Statistics (ver. 26). The sample was normally distributed, and parametric testing was used. First, descriptive statistics were analyzed for each variable, and Pearson correlations were calculated among the variables. Next, the effects of cognitive-strategy use frequencies and moderating effects were examined using hierarchical multiple regression analyses [21]. When the interaction term is used to examine a moderation effect, multicollinearity between the predictors may result in underestimating regression coefficients or overestimating standard error values [22]. The Shapiro–Wilk statistic for dependent variable normality was 0.69 (p = 0.136). Linearity was found between the dependent and independent variables (0.187 < r < 0.554). For multicollinearity, the strongest intercorrelation was between gender and the CCSS (r = −0.165, p = 0.002). The minimum tolerance value was 0.948, the maximum Mahalanobis distance was 3.98, and the maximum Cook’s distance was 0.138. A scatterplot of residuals showed that most residuals were between −2 and +2.
The EF (DEX) and moderator (cognitive strategies) were mean-centered prior to statistical analysis to address this potential problem. The effect on the criterion variable (daily functioning) was examined after the predictor and moderator variables were entered as independent variables in Stage 1. The interaction term between the predictor and moderator variable was entered in Stage 2 as the independent variable.

3. Results

3.1. Data Analysis

A total of 336 students (180 women, 156 men) aged 18 to 40 years from universities across Israel—44 (13.1%) from southern, 62 (18.5%) from northern, and 230 (68.5%) from central regions—completed the questionnaires. Table 1 presents their demographic characteristics.

3.2. Frequency of Cognitive Strategies

The most frequently used compensatory cognitive strategies (according to the CCSS) were prioritizing a “to-do” list to aid time management and organization (65.2% of participants reported using this strategy often to all the time) and using reminder functions on computers or personal electronic devices such as smartphones or tablets (64% reported using this strategy often to all the time). The least frequently used strategies were requesting accommodations as needed, for example, extra time to complete assignments (73.5% reported using this strategy never to rarely) and using a digital recorder to make and later review short reminder memos (73.5% reported using this strategy never to rarely). Table 2 presents the frequency of cognitive strategy results.

3.3. Correlations between Cognitive Strategies, Executive Function, and Daily Functioning

Table 3 presents the descriptive statistics and correlations among study variables. The results indicated a significantly strong correlation between the DEX and TOPS scores (r = 0.51, p = 0.000). The other significant correlations were weak (ranging from −0.18 to −0.16).

3.4. Executive Function Effect on Daily Functioning: Moderating Effect of Cognitive Strategies

A hierarchical regression analysis was conducted to determine whether cognitive strategy use moderated the relationships between EF and daily functioning. The age and gender were entered in the first step and the DEX and TOPS mean scores in the second step. Table 4 presents the results, which indicate that the DEX significantly predicted daily functioning as assessed by the TOPS.
Linear regression predicting the TOPS scores by DEX, CCSS, and DEX * CCSS interaction was used to assess the moderation relationship. Figure 2 demonstrates the moderation effect of cognitive strategy use (as assessed by CCSS) on the relationships between EF (by DEX) and daily functioning (by TOPS). Results showed a significant interaction (B = 0.01, SE B = 0.001, p = 0.07, 95% CI [0.01,0.02]).
To probe that interaction, the relationship between the DEX and TOPS was calculated for three CCSS levels: −1.0 SD, −1.0 to 1.0 SD, and 1.0 SD from average. The results showed that the higher CCSS is, the stronger the association between the DEX and TOPS becomes. Specifically, a relatively low relationship was found between the DEX and TOPS (B = 0.47, SE B = 0.08, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.31, 0.62]) for individuals who scored low on the CCSS; a relatively moderate relationship (B = 0.56, SE B = 0.06, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.45, 0.67]) for individuals scoring moderate on the CCSS; and a relatively high relationship (B = 0.65, SE B = 0.07, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.51, 0.79]) for individuals scoring high.

4. Discussion

The present research investigated how EF, daily functioning, and cognitive strategy use interact to gain a deeper theoretical and clinical understanding. Furthermore, the present study’s results highlight the significance of using cognitive strategies to enhance these relationships. This study found that the use of cognitive strategies moderates this relationship, showing that their frequent use strengthens the relationships and thus contributes to better daily functioning.
This study’s frequency-of-use results support previous studies that used the CCSS questionnaire [1,18]. The current study results indicate that some strategies are found with high frequency among the research participants. Examples include (No. 1) reminder functions on a computer or personal electronic device (e.g., smartphone, tablet) and (No. 20) prioritizing “to-do” lists to aid time management and organization. It also shows that the participants used other strategies with very low frequency, such as (No. 15) logging off email periodically to limit distractions and (No. 18) using a digital recorder to make and later review short reminder memos.
The literature is replete with lists and descriptions of cognitive-strategy types (e.g., [2,23,24]. By looking at the most (above the average of 2.66) and least frequent strategies reported by research participants, it is possible to divide and characterize the strategies in the current study according to the accepted divisions in the literature. These three classifications address (a) external and internal strategies [6,25,26], (b) learning or self-regulation strategies, and (c) strategies directed at the task, environment, or person [2].
This study’s most frequent strategies can be typed as internal, and the less frequent as external. Internal strategies are those not apparent to others, such as (No. 9) performing one task at a time rather than multitasking and (No. 11) minimizing the number of open tabs/windows when on the Internet. External strategies are those apparent to another person, such as (No. 13) requesting accommodations as needed and (No. 16) requesting agenda or meeting notes.
The most frequent strategies in this study also can be typed as self-regulation, and the less frequent as learning strategies. Self-regulation strategies require checking, monitoring, planning, predicting, and gauging performance, such as (No. 3) organizing to accomplish the most challenging task at the best time of day and (No. 8) rechecking work after a break. Learning strategies include acquiring, selecting, organizing, or rehearsing material to be learned, such as (No. 17) asking others to minimize interruptions when working and (No. 19) slowing down to avoid situations that require fast reaction time.
Finally, cognitive strategies can be directed at the task, environment, or person. Environment or task-oriented strategies involve analyzing or modifying external demands or adjusting one’s attention to a specific task or environmental feature. Person-oriented strategies enhance the person’s retention, comprehension, and general abilities to attend, monitor, and regulate performance [23]. In this study, the most frequent strategies were directed toward the person. Examples include (No. 10) establishing a routine of putting “key” items in the same place in the house or workspace and (No.14) using personal electronic devices to record written documents. The less frequent strategies were directed toward the task or environment, such as (No. 5) slowing the pace of communication in meetings and (No. 6) confirming what others say by repeating the “gist” back to them.
Although this discussion of the strategy division is only descriptive, it distinguishes the more common from the less common strategies. This study’s participants report more frequent use of internal strategies, which enable self-regulation and are directed at the person.

4.1. Correlations between Cognitive Strategies, EF and Daily Functioning

The results reveal a highly significant correlation between EF as assessed by the DEX and daily functioning as assessed by the TOPS and significant weak correlations between EF and daily functioning to using cognitive strategies. Theoretical models and research findings previously connected daily functional deficits with deficient EF (e.g., [27,28] and support the correlations found in this study.
Planning and organizing time for daily activities is an important prerequisite for managing daily life [29]. Proper time organization enables a sense of control and satisfaction; it reduces feelings of stress, leading to participation that is more meaningful [30,31]. Effective time organization and its related EF are key to executing daily activities and to independence [32,33].
Whereas most prior studies addressed the relationships between cognitive strategies and learning and achievement [34], this study examines cognitive strategies and daily functioning as assessed by the TOPS. Very few studies dealt with this connection. Sturkenboom et al. [35] found compromised efficiency of applied cognitive-strategy behaviors in a certain pattern of people with Parkinson’s disease that declines with disease progression. Meyer [36] described how cognitive strategies support adolescents with celiac disease self-management or with less effective management abilities.
The correlation in this study between EF as assessed by the DEX and cognitive strategies was found to be significant but weak. As such, the weak relationship may imply that the EF relationship with strategies is indirect and that increased strategy use could strengthen the direct relationship between EF and daily functioning.

4.2. Executive Function Effect on Daily Functioning: Moderating Effect of Cognitive Strategies

All individuals use strategies to manage the performance of occupations undertaken in daily life, regardless of whether they are aware of these strategies [2]. As the study results demonstrate, a strategy can be more than an intervention’s goal; it can extend beyond compensatory methods. However, using strategies daily plays a vital role in learning and in performing tasks [37,38]. The contribution of cognitive strategies to the relationship between EF and daily functioning can be explained from a theoretical viewpoint through cognitive load theory (CLT) and self-regulated learning (SRL) and from a clinical viewpoint through therapeutic interventions in which daily functioning improves by using cognitive strategies.
When discussing how cognitive strategies moderate the relationships between EF and daily functioning, CLT [39] and SRL [40] can help explain the moderation effect. The CLT posits that, because of working memory’s limited facilities, understanding effective teaching and learning revolves around the amount of information a learner is asked to process and how that information is presented [41]. Cognitive load commonly describes the mental-resource level demanded from the human brain for a specific task [42] and closely correlates with the person’s performance. A task whose demands are optimally matched to a user will keep the cognitive load at an appropriate level, leading to good user performance with high learning efficiency [39,43].
A crucial aspect of strategy use is reducing demands on working memory. For instance, students with dyslexia often struggle with working memory overload and insufficient processing space for new information [44]. Students who use strategies to support EF processes can effectively recognize their weaknesses and strengths [45,46] and can directly influence their academic performance [46,47]. Strategies may also support EF to manage the cognitive load better, concentrate on one topic at a time, and remain goal-oriented [46,48].
Within SRL, “learners set goals for their learning and then attempt to monitor, regulate and control their cognition, motivation and behavior, guided and constrained by their goals and the contextual features of the environment” [49] (p. 453). Knowledge and application of strategies play a significant role in the SRL process. It is key to a student’s ability to organize better the learning environment (time, place, material), plan learning activities, set goals, choose appropriate strategies to achieve goals, self-monitor the learning process, self-evaluate the learning outcome, and make appropriate adjustments [45,46]. Self-regulation embodies a crucial part of academic performance and cognitive development in student learning because it involves the student’s control over selecting and applying cognitive strategies [50,51].
The CLT and SRL theories complement each other and help explain the significant contribution of frequent cognitive-strategy use to the relationship between EF and daily functioning.
The literature highlights the importance, from a clinical viewpoint, of using strategies to promote cognitive functional performance. Within the rehabilitation context, the term strategy often is linked with compensation and viewed as the product of intervention [52,53,54]. Evidence-based reviews and meta-analyses of research studies consistently demonstrated that intervention approaches focused on strategies produce the best outcomes across groups. These groups have included persons with a learning disability [55,56], brain injury [5,57,58], stroke [59,60], Parkinson’s disease [61], and schizophrenia [62].
Huckans et al. [63] found that, following cognitive-strategy training treatment, participants reported significantly increased use of compensatory cognitive strategies and day planners; increased perception that these strategies were useful to them; increased life satisfaction; and decreased depressive, memory, and cognitive symptom severity. Kaizerman-Dinerman et al. [64] found that among people with schizophrenia, the intervention group had the larger number of cognitive strategies, better efficiency, more correct answers, and presented a significant increase in the use of four cognitive strategies (rereading, talking aloud, fixing appointments, and covering lines).
That evidence supports the contribution of cognitive-strategy use in identifying clinical populations. However, to the author’s knowledge, this study is the first to spotlight the strengthening effect of using cognitive strategies on the relationship. Only a few studies have dealt with the moderation effect of strategies. For example, Menéndez-Espina et al. [65] found that women acquire a greater variety of coping strategies that moderate between job insecurity and mental health. In contrast, men have only one, social withdrawal, which is also significant in the female group. Leone et al. [15] examined the effects of maladaptive cognitive–emotion regulation strategies on the relationship between alcohol use problems and negative relationship-conflict behaviors.
Costley [50] found that cognitive-strategy use moderates the EF–daily functioning relationship, demonstrating that it can help overcome unclear instruction and produce higher levels of student learning. This study highlights the value of applying cognitive strategies in general and specifically in relationships with extraneous cognitive loads. It shows that cognitive strategies have a positive relationship with germane cognitive loads, illustrating the value of promoted cognitive strategies in online instructional situations and that they mitigate the extraneous load’s harmful effects on the germane load. Many students may struggle to learn when extraneous-load levels are high; this study suggests that applying cognitive learning strategies may help them overcome some difficulties.

4.3. Limitations and Future Research

This study referred to strategies in general, which is its main limitation—specifically, the reference to strategies was to only their frequency of use. The literature showed that strategies should be examined beyond their frequency, referring to their effectiveness and the degree of mental effort invested in implementing them [26]. Further, additional elements (e.g., demographic variables, awareness of strategy use, contexts in which strategies are used, simultaneous use of several strategies, and control over their effectiveness) might enable or hinder the frequency of using strategies. A follow-up study examining strategies for the frequency of use, effectiveness, and effort to implement may be very effective for understanding the elements underlying their use.
Additionally, there was no attribution in this study to the students who reported disabilities. Follow-up studies should divide the population into groups and examine differences between the groups in daily functioning, EF, and strategy use. Finally, this research is based on students’ self-reported EF, daily functioning, and cognitive strategies. Future research should look at more concrete, objective measures of student outcomes, such as a controlled experiment wherein students perform a daily task with the frequency of using cognitive strategies observed.

5. Conclusions

University students experience significant cognitive demands in their coursework, which could lead to mental and behavioral health problems (e.g., depression or alcohol use disorders) [66] that may adversely affect cognitive functioning [67].
This study’s results have practical implications on two levels. First, they can raise awareness among students of common strategies students reported in this and previous studies. Exposure to multiple strategies increases the chance that students will use them to promote their daily functioning. Second, the results can be used to map strategies as part of intervention processes. This is especially meaningful for students who are challenged to manage their day-to-day functioning and balance occupation, daily functioning, work, and leisure.
These clinicians might consider possible intervention modes that include addressing mental health problems, recommending academic accommodations, or providing psychoeducation on compensatory cognitive strategies to support academic performance. An instrument that quantifies current strategy use would benefit clinicians in educating patients on compensatory cognitive strategies as part of the interventions. Such an instrument would allow the clinicians to gauge whether psychoeducation was necessary (i.e., determine whether the student currently uses strategies in their everyday life) and track changes in the strategy used throughout the intervention [1].

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The participating institution’s Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Health Sciences, Ariel University ethics committee approved this study (AU-HEA-YF-20210811) on 11 August 2021.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are not publicly available due to ethical restrictions but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Karr, J.E. Compensatory Cognitive Strategy Use by Young Adults: A Psychometric Evaluation of Self-Report Measures. Assessment 2020, 29, 441–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Toglia, J.P.; Rodger, S.A.; Polatajko, H.J. Anatomy of Cognitive Strategies: A Therapist’s Primer for Enabling Occupational Performance. Can. J. Occup. Ther. 2012, 79, 225–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Toglia, J.P. The Dynamic Interactional Model of Cognition in Cognitive Rehabilitation. In Cognition, Occupation, and Participation across the Life Span: Neuroscience, Neurorehabilitation and Models for Intervention in Occupational Therapy; Katz, N., Toglia, J., Eds.; AOTA Press: Bethesda, MD, USA, 2018; pp. 355–387. [Google Scholar]
  4. Toglia, J.; Berg, C. Performance-Based Measure of Executive Function: Comparison of Community and At-Risk Youth. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2013, 67, 515–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Bottari, C.; Wai Shun, P.L.; Le Dorze, G.; Gosselin, N.; Dawson, D. Self-Generated Strategic Behavior in an Ecological Shopping Task. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2014, 68, 67–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Toglia, J.; Foster, E.R. The Multicontext Approach to Cognitive Rehabilitation; Gatekeeper Press: Grove City, OH, USA, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  7. Roth, R.M.; Lance, C.E.; Isquith, P.K.; Fischer, A.S.; Giancola, P.R. Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult Version in Healthy Adults and Application to Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2013, 28, 425–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Connor, L.T.; Maeir, A. Putting Executive Performance in a Theoretical Context. OTJR Occup. Particip. Health 2011, 31 (Suppl. 1), S3–S7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Kaplan, S.; Berman, M.G. Directed Attention as a Common Resource for Executive Functioning and Self-Regulation. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2010, 5, 43–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Diamond, A. Executive Functions. In Handbook of Clinical Neurology; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2020; Volume 173, pp. 225–240. [Google Scholar]
  11. Proctor, B. Relationships between Cattell–Horn–Carroll (CHC) Cognitive Abilities and Math Achievement within a Sample of College Students with Learning Disabilities. J. Learn Disabil. 2012, 45, 278–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Ganbari-Taleb, M.; Ghanbari, A.; Yousefi, Z.; Botlani, S. Cognitive Strategies Instruction: Attitudes toward Learning and Academic Functioning in Science. Bulg. J. Sci. Educ. Policy 2013, 7, 104. [Google Scholar]
  13. Rosenblum, S. Reliability and Validity of the Time Organisation and Participation Scale (TOPS). Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 2012, 22, 65–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Kreider, C.; Slamka, M.; Medina, S. Multidimensional Strategies for Overcoming Time-Related Challenges of Transition-Age Students with Learning and Attention Disorders. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2019, 73, 7311505096p1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Leone, R.M.; Jarnecke, A.M.; Gilmore, A.K.; Flanagan, J.C. Alcohol Use Problems and Conflict among Couples: A Preliminary Investigation of the Moderating Effects of Maladaptive Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies. Couple Fam. Psychol. 2022, 11, 290–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Auerbach, R.P.; Alonso, J.; Axinn, W.G.; Cuijpers, P.; Ebert, D.D.; Green, J.G.; Hwang, I.; Kessler, R.C.; Liu, H.; Mortier, P.; et al. Mental Disorders among College Students in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. Psychol. Med. 2016, 46, 2955–2970. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Buchner, A.; Lang, A.G. Statistical Power Analyses Using G* Power 3.1: Tests for Correlation and Regression Analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 2009, 41, 1149–1160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Becker, H.; Stuifbergen, A.K.; Henneghan, A.; Morrison, J.; Seo, E.J.; Zhang, W. An Initial Investigation of the Reliability and Validity of the Compensatory Cognitive Strategies Scale. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 2019, 29, 739–753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Wilson, B.A.; Alderman, N.; Burgess, P.W.; Emslie, H.C.; Evans, J.J. Behavioral Assessment of Dysexecutive Syndrome Manual; Thames Valley Test Company: Bury St. Edmunds, UK, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  20. Burgess, P.W.; Alderman, N.; Evans, J.; Emslie, H.; Wilson, B.A. The Ecological Validity of Tests of Executive Function. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 1998, 4, 547–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  21. Keith, T.Z. Multiple Regression and Beyond: An Introduction to Multiple Regression and Structural Equation Modeling; Routledge: London, UK, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  22. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences; Routledge: London, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  23. Singer, R.N.; Chen, D. A Classification Scheme for Cognitive Strategies: Implications for Learning and Teaching Psychomotor Skills. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 1994, 65, 143–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Weinstein, C.E.; Acce, T.W.; Jung, J. Self-Regulation and Learning Strategies. New Dir. Teach. Learn 2011, 126, 45–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Bouazzaoui, B.; Isingrini, M.; Fay, S. Aging and Self-Reported Internal and External Memory Strategy Uses: The Role of Executive Functioning. Acta Psychol. 2010, 135, 59–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Toglia, J.; Goverover, Y.; Johnston, M.V.; Dain, B. Application of the Multicontextual Approach in Promoting Learning and Transfer of Strategy Use in an Individual with TBI and Executive Dysfunction. OTJR Occup. Particip. Health 2011, 31, S53–S60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Rosenblum, S. Handwriting Measures as Reflectors of Executive Functions among Adults with Developmental Coordination Disorders (DCD). Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Sharfi, K.; Rosenblum, S. Executive Functions, Time Organization and Quality of Life among Adults with Learning Disabilities. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0166939. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  29. White, S.M.; Janeslätt, G.; Holmefur, M. Assessment of Time Management Skills: Psychometric Properties of the Swedish Version. Scand J. Occup. Ther. 2018, 25, 153–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. American Occupational Therapy Association. Occupational Therapy Practice Framework: Domain & Process (4th ed.). Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2020, 74, 1–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Law, M. Participation in the Occupations of Everyday Life. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2002, 56, 640–649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Lamash, L.; Ricon, T.; Rosenblum, S. Time Organization Patterns of Adolescents: Agreement between Self-Report and Parent Report. Phys. Occup. Ther. Pediatr. 2022, 42, 319–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Meltzer, L. (Ed.) Executive Function in Education: From Theory to Practice; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  34. Hartwig, M.K.; Dunlosky, J. Study Strategies of College Students: Are Self-Testing and Scheduling Related to Achievement? Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2012, 19, 126–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Sturkenboom, I.H.; Nott, M.T.; Bloem, B.R.; Chapparo, C.; Steultjens, E.M.J. Applied Cognitive Strategy Behaviours in People with Parkinson’s Disease during Daily Activities: A Cross-Sectional Study. J. Rehabil. Med. 2020, 52, jrm00010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Meyer, S. Promoting Effective Self-Management of the Gluten-Free Diet: Children’s and Adolescents’ Self-Generated Do’s and Don’ts. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 14051. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Polatajko, H.J.; Mandich, A. (Eds.) Enabling Occupation in Children: The Cognitive Orientation to Daily Occupational Performance; CAOT ACE: Ottawa, CA, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  38. Walsh, M.M.; Anderson, J.R. The Strategic Nature of Changing your Mind. Cogn. Psychol. 2009, 58, 416–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Sweller, J.; van Merrienboer, J.J.G.; Paas, F.G.W.C. Cognitive Architecture and Instructional Design. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 1998, 10, 251–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Bjork, R.A.; Dunlosky, J.; Kornell, N. Self-Regulated Learning: Beliefs, Techniques, and Illusions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 417–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Cierniak, G.; Scheiter, K.; Gerjets, P. Explaining the Split-Attention Effect: Is the Reduction of Extraneous Cognitive Load Accompanied by an Increase in Germane Cognitive Load? Comput. Human Behav. 2009, 25, 315–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Baddeley, A.; Thomson, N.; Buchanan, M. Word Length and the Structure of Short-Term Memory. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 1975, 14, 575–589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Yu, K.; Epps, J.; Chen, F. Cognitive Load Evaluation of Handwriting Using Stroke-Level Features. In Proceedings of the 16th International Conference on Intelligent User Interfaces, Palo Alto, CA, USA, 13–16 February 2011; pp. 423–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Martinussen, R.; Major, A. Working Memory Weakness in Students with ADHD: Implications for Instructions. Theory Pract. 2011, 50, 68–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Dawson, P.; Guare, R. Coaching Students with Executive Skills Deficits; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  46. Meltzer, L.; Basho, S. Creating a Classroomwide Executive Function Culture that Fosters Strategy Use, Motivation, and Resilience. In Promoting Executive Function in the Classroom; Meltzer, L., Ed.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2010; pp. 28–56. [Google Scholar]
  47. Vassallo, S. Self-Regulated Learning: An Application of Critical Educational Psychology; Peter Lang: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  48. Akbasli, S.; Sahin, M.; Gürel, M. A Model to Manage EFL Learners with ADHD and Dyslexia. J. Educ. Pract. 2017, 8, 201–214. [Google Scholar]
  49. Pintrich, P.R. The Role of Goal Orientation in Self-Regulated Learning. In Enabling Occupation in Children: The Cognitive Orientation to Daily Occupational Performance; Boekaerts, M., Polatajko, H.J., Mandich, A., Eds.; CAOT ACE: Ottawa, CA, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  50. Costley, J. The Relationship between Social Presence and Cognitive Load. Interact. Technol. Smart Educ. 2019, 16, 172–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Schunk, D.H.; Zimmerman, B.J. Motivation and Self-Regulated Learning: Theory, Research, and Applications; Routledge: London, UK, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  52. Dawson, D.; Richardson, J.; Troyer, A.; Binns, M.; Clark, A.; Polatajko, H.; Winocur, G.; Hunt, A.; Bar, Y. An Occupation-Based Strategy Training Approach to Managing Age-Related Executive Changes: A Pilot Randomized Controlled Trial. Clin. Rehabil. 2014, 28, 118–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Giles, G.M. A Neurofunctional Approach to Rehabilitation after Brain Injury. In Cognition, Occupation and Participation across the Life Span: Neuroscience, Neurorehabilitation and Models of Intervention in Occupational Therapy, 3rd ed.; Katz, N., Ed.; AOTA Press: Bethesda, MD, USA, 2011; pp. 351–381. [Google Scholar]
  54. Katz, N.; Baum, C.; Maier, A. Introduction to Cognitive Intervention and Cognitive Functional Evaluation. In Cognition, Occupation and Participation across the Life Span: Neuroscience, Neurorehabilitation and Models of Intervention in Occupational Therapy, 3rd ed.; Katz, N., Ed.; AOTA Press: Bethesda, MD, USA, 2011; pp. 3–12. [Google Scholar]
  55. Chitiyo, A. Evidence Based Reading Strategies for Struggling Readers with Learning Disabilities and Emotional Disorders. In Learning Disabilities-Neurobiology, Assessment, Clinical Features and Treatments; Misciagna, S., Ed.; IntechOpen: London, UK, 2022; pp. 283–296. [Google Scholar]
  56. Afeli, S.A. Academic Accommodation Strategies for Pharmacy Students with Learning Disabilities: What Else Can Be Done? Curr. Pharm. Teach. Learn. 2019, 11, 751–756. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Caplan, B.; Bogner, J.; Brenner, L. CogSMART Compensatory Cognitive Training for Traumatic Brain Injury: Effects over 1 Year. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 2015, 30, 391–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Cicerone, K.D.; Langenbahn, D.M.; Braden, C.; Malec, J.F.; Kalmar, K.; Fraas, M.; Felicetti, T.; Laatsch, L.; Harley, J.P.; Bergquist, T.; et al. Evidence-Based Cognitive Rehabilitation: Updated Review of the Literature from 2003 through 2008. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2011, 92, 519–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Ahn, S.N.; Yoo, E.Y.; Jung, M.Y.; Park, H.Y.; Lee, J.Y.; Choi, Y.I. Comparison of cognitive orientation to daily occupational performance and conventional occupational therapy on occupational performance in individuals with stroke: A randomized controlled trial. NeuroRehabilitation 2017, 40, 285–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. McEwen, S.E.; Huijbregts, M.P.; Ryan, J.D.; Malec, J.F.; Kalmar, K.; Fraas, M.; Felicetti, T.; Laatsch, L.; Harley, J.P.; Berquist, T.; et al. Cognitive Strategy Use to Enhance Motor Skill Acquisition Post-Stroke: A Critical Review. Brain Inj. 2009, 23, 263–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Foster, E.R.; Spence, D.; Toglia, J. Feasibility of a Cognitive Strategy Training Intervention for People with Parkinson’s Disease. Disabil. Rehabil. 2018, 40, 1127–1134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Wykes, T.; Huddy, V.; Cellard, C.; McGurk, S.R.; Czobor, P. A Meta-Analysis of Cognitive Remediation for Schizophrenia: Methodology and Effect Sizes. Am. J. Psychiatry 2011, 168, 472–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  63. Huckans, M.; Pavawalla, S.; Demadura, T.; Kolessar, M.; Seelye, A.; Roost, N.; Twamley, E.W.; Storzbach, D. A Pilot Study Examining Effects of Group-Based Cognitive Strategy Training Treatment on Self-Reported Cognitive Problems, Psychiatric Symptoms, Functioning, and Compensatory Strategy Use in OIF/OEF Combat Veterans with Persistent Mild Cognitive Disorder and History of Traumatic Brain Injury. J. Rehabil. Res. Dev. 2010, 47, 43–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Kaizerman-Dinerman, A.; Josman, N.; Roe, D. The Use of Cognitive Strategies among People with Schizophrenia: A Randomized Comparative Study. Open J. Occup. Ther. 2019, 7, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Menéndez-Espina, S.; Llosa, J.A.; Agulló-Tomás, E.; Rodriguez-Suárez, J.; Sáiz-Villar, R.; Lahseras-Diez, H.F. Job Insecurity and Mental Health: The Moderating Role of Coping Strategies from a Gender Perspective. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Blanco, C.; Okuda, M.; Wright, C.; Hasin, D.S.; Grant, B.F.; Liu, S.M.; Olfson, M. Mental health of college students and their noncollege-attending peers: Results from the national epidemiologic study on alcohol and related conditions. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2008, 65, 1429–1437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Semkovska, M.; Quinlivan, L.; O’Grady, T.; Johnson, R.; Collins, A.; O’Connor, J.; Knittle, H.; Ahern, E.; Gload, T. Cognitive Function following a Major Depressive Episode: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 2019, 6, 851–861. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Moderating effects of the use of cognitive strategies on the relationship between EF and daily functioning.
Figure 1. Moderating effects of the use of cognitive strategies on the relationship between EF and daily functioning.
Ijerph 19 16845 g001
Figure 2. Moderation effect of cognitive strategy use on the relationships between executive functions and daily functioning.
Figure 2. Moderation effect of cognitive strategy use on the relationships between executive functions and daily functioning.
Ijerph 19 16845 g002
Table 1. Participants’ demographic characteristics.
Table 1. Participants’ demographic characteristics.
DemographicMean (±SD)
Age (years)24.61 (2.62)
Frequency (%)
Gender
Men156 (46.4)
Women180 (53.6)
Family status
Single275 (81.8)
Married without children29 (8.6)
Married with children27 (8.0)
Other (divorced, widowed)5 (1.5)
Field of study
Medicine18 (5.4)
Therapy61 (18.2)
Education32 (9.5)
Computers44 (13.1)
Engineering68 (20.2)
Social science18 (5.4)
Law17 (5.1)
Architecture4 (1.2)
Communication16 (4.8)
Economic29 (8.6)
Life science19 (5.7)
Humanities10 (3.0)
Grade range (% score)
90–100112 (33.3)
80–90144 (42.9)
70–8064 (19.0)
60–7015 (4.5)
50–601 (0.3)
Employed
Yes214 (63.7)
No122 (36.3)
Previous first degree
Yes9 (2.7)
No327 (97.3)
Disability
Motor4 (1.2)
Mental21 (6.3)
Cognitive2 (0.6)
Communication2 (0.6)
Neurodevelopmental (ADHD, DCD, SLD)56 (16.7)
Not specified12 (3.6)
No diagnosis 239 (71.1)
Table 2. Descriptive statistics, self-reported engagement in compensatory cognitive strategies: Compensatory Cognitive Strategies Scale frequency and means.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics, self-reported engagement in compensatory cognitive strategies: Compensatory Cognitive Strategies Scale frequency and means.
Strategy Frequency (%)
Mean (±SD)NeverRarelySometimesOftenAll the Time
1Reminder functions on computer or personal electronic device (e.g., smartphone, tablet)3.77 (1.21)17 (5.1)44 (13.1)60 (17.9)94 (28.0)121 (36.0)
2Break down complex tasks into smaller steps2.94 (1.05)33 (9.8)75 (22.3)128 (38.1)79 (23.5)21 (6.3)
3Organize to do most challenging tasks at best (i.e., most energetic) time of day3.02 (1.15)41 (12.2)68 (20.2)97 (28.9)103 (30.7)27 (8.0)
4Minimize workspace distractions by closing the door, etc.3.40 (1.17)24 (7.1)56 (16.7)81 (24.1)113 (33.6)62 (18.5)
5Slow pace of communication in meetings2.00 (1.14)152 (45.2)87 (25.9)50 (14.9)38 (11.3)9 (2.7)
6Confirm what others say by repeating the “gist” back to them2.50 (1.11)81 (24.1)82 (24.4)106 (31.5)59 (17.6)8 (2.4)
7Take notes while listening/viewing presentations to organize/reinforce information2.45 (1.36)119 (35.4)65 (19.3)64 (19.0)58 (17.3)30 (8.9)
8Recheck work after a break3.16 (1.28)47 (14.0)56 (16.7)84 (25.0)93 (27.7)56 (16.7)
9Do one thing at a time rather than multitask3.12 (1.21)44 (13.1)51 (15.2)108 (31.1)88 (26.2)45 (13.4)
10Establish routine of putting “key” items in the same place in the house or workspace 3.10 (1.55)91 (27.1)29 (8.6)58 (17.3)72 (21.4)86 (25.6)
11Minimize number of open tabs/windows when on the Internet 2.92 (1.46)88 (26.2)45 (13.4)73 (21.7)66 (19.6)64 (19.0)
12Write out notes (in advance) to help verbally relay a sequence of events 1.94 (1.16)179 (53.3)49 (14.6)68 (20.2)30 (8.9)10 (3.0)
13Request accommodations as needed (e.g., extra time to complete assignments)1.84 (1.15)190 (56.5)57 (17.0)54 (16.1)22 (6.5)13 (3.9)
14Use personal electronic devices to record written documents 3.24 (1.34)54 (16.1)40 (11.9)81 (24.1)92 (27.4)69 (20.5)
15Log off email periodically to limit distractions1.77 (1.17)211 (62.8)46 (13.7)40 (11.9)24 (7.1)15 (4.5)
16Request agenda/meeting notes2.41 (1.46)127 (37.8)54 (16.1)73 (21.7)55 (16.4)27 (8.0)
17Ask others to minimize interruptions when working 2.33 (1.31)127 (37.8)66 (19.6)74 (22.0)42 (12.5)27 (8.0)
18Use digital recorder to make/later review short reminder memos 1.77 (1.16)215 (64.0)32 (9.5)51 (15.2)27 (8.0)11 (3.3)
19Slow down; avoid situations that require fast reaction time1.82 (1.04)181 (53.9)68 (20.2)59 (17.6)23 (6.8)5 (1.5)
20Prioritize “to-do” list to aid time management and organization3.76 (1.29)33 (9.8)24 (7.1)60 (17.9)91 (27.1)128 (38.1)
Table 3. Means, standard deviations, and correlations among study variables.
Table 3. Means, standard deviations, and correlations among study variables.
VariableMean(±SD)1234
Age24.612.62
Gender 0.16 **
Compensatory Cognitive Strategy Scale2.660.570.05−0.16 **
Dysexecutive Questionnaire3.852.120.00−0.090.13 *
Time Organization and Participation Scale4.442.31−0.00−0.18 **0.12 *0.51 ***
Note. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table 4. Stepwise regression: Predicting TOPS.
Table 4. Stepwise regression: Predicting TOPS.
VariableBSE BßBSE Bß
Step 1Step 2
Age0.000.010.020.000.010.00
Gender−0.220.07−0.18−0.150.06−0.12
Dysexecutive Questionnaire 0.550.060.50
Compensatory Cognitive Strategy Scale 0.040.050.04
Note. Step 1: R2 = 0.03, Δ R2 = 0.03, p = 0.008; Step 2: R2 = 0.28, Δ R2 = 0.25, p = 0.000.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Fogel, Y. Cognitive Strategies: Moderating the Relationship between Executive Functions and Daily Functioning. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 16845. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192416845

AMA Style

Fogel Y. Cognitive Strategies: Moderating the Relationship between Executive Functions and Daily Functioning. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(24):16845. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192416845

Chicago/Turabian Style

Fogel, Yael. 2022. "Cognitive Strategies: Moderating the Relationship between Executive Functions and Daily Functioning" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 24: 16845. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192416845

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop