Next Article in Journal
How Are Bystanders Involved in Cyberbullying? A Latent Class Analysis of the Cyberbystander and Their Characteristics in Different Intervention Stages
Next Article in Special Issue
Gender Gap in Mental Health during the COVID-19 Pandemic in South Korea: A Decomposition Analysis
Previous Article in Journal
Comparison of the Thermal Reaction of Patients after Conserving Procedures and after Mastectomy to the Radiation Dose Obtained during Radiotherapy
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Level of COVID-19 Anxiety among Oncology Patients in Poland
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Opinion

Should I Help? Prosocial Behaviour during the COVID-19 Pandemic

1
Faculty of Business and Communications, INTI International University, Nilai 71800, Negeri Sembilan, Malaysia
2
Faculty of Health and Life Sciences, INTI International University, Nilai 71800, Negeri Sembilan, Malaysia
3
Faculty of Engineering and Quantity Surveying, INTI International University, Nilai 71800, Negeri Sembilan, Malaysia
4
Faculty of Data Science and Information Technology, INTI International University, Nilai 71800, Negeri Sembilan, Malaysia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(23), 16084; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192316084
Submission received: 28 August 2022 / Revised: 5 October 2022 / Accepted: 12 October 2022 / Published: 1 December 2022

Abstract

:
The Movement Control Order (MCO) enacted during the COVID-19 pandemic has profoundly altered the social life and behaviour of the Malaysian population. Because the society is facing huge social and economic challenges that need individuals to work together to solve, prosocial behaviour is regarded as one of the most important social determinants. Because it is related with individual and societal benefits, participating in prosocial activities may be a major protective factor during times of global crisis. Rather than focusing only on medical and psychiatric paradigms, perhaps all that is necessary to overcome the COVID-19 risks is for individuals to make personal sacrifices for the sake of others. In reality, a large number of initiatives proven to be beneficial in decreasing viral transmission include a trade-off between individual and collective interests. Given its crucial importance, the purpose of this concept paper is to provide some insight into prosocial behaviour during the COVID-19 period. Understanding prosocial behaviour during the COVID-19 pandemic is crucial because it may assist in the establishment of a post-COVID society and provide useful strategies for coping with future crises.

1. Introduction

In the first month of the year 2020, the SARS-CoV-2 (COVID-19) pandemic swept across the globe, and all nations instituted quarantine measures to prevent the virus from spreading further [1,2,3]. Starting on 8 March and continuing until 4 May, when it was partially lifted, Italy was the first European nation to implement a severe lockdown [4]. In addition, the government of Malaysia implemented the Movement Control Order (MCO) which is used when a pandemic becomes severe in the country [5,6,7]. This is done with the intention of reducing the likelihood that residents will become infected with a virus through direct interaction with one another. Although these safety measures were successful in halting the spread of the virus, they were responsible for a considerable disruption of the social and communal life in the regions that were affected by the outbreak [5,8,9]. As a result, the significance of prosocial behaviour becomes even more essential in times of crises, which is when a society is faced with significant social, economic, and political issues that require individuals to come together and work together to find solutions. During the COVID-19 pandemic, it is therefore of the utmost importance to gain an understanding of how to increase and encourage prosocial behaviour. Examples of prosocial behaviour include providing assistance, contributions, sharing, and comforting others [10]. The development of prosocial behaviour is an essential component of human socialization and has been linked to significant increases in mental health and well-being, both for those who are on the receiving end of beneficial acts and for those who perform them [1,11,12,13]. Prosocial behaviour is a multifaceted concept that can be broken down into many different subcategories. Some of these subcategories include compassion, caring, love, sympathy, empathy, altruism, and kindness [14].
Prosocial behaviours have been classified according to many different dimensions, such as whether they are spontaneous and informal, planned and formal, personal and impersonal, and the amount of effort required from the person providing the assistance [15,16]. Acts of generosity and assistance that are low in cost, such as sending an uplifting message, are typically straightforward and one-time deeds of kindness. As an illustration of a high-cost behaviour that requires perseverance, moral fortitude, and sustained commitment, emergency volunteer work is an option that may run counter to an individual’s own best interests [17,18]. From a psychological and social point of view, one explanation for prosocial behaviour is that people who help others feel a duty to do so [19]. At the community level, a sense of community responsibility (SOC-R), as coined by [20] has been conceptualized to represent a sense of duty toward others. Individuals are motivated to help one another and the community as a whole because they have a sense of obligation toward other members of the community as well as the community as a whole, which is what this phrase alludes to [20,21].

1.1. How Does Prosocial Behaviour Develop during Crises?

The term “catastrophe compassion” was coined to describe how people respond to widespread catastrophes by participating in acts of altruism toward others. This idea was devised to explain how people react to widespread disasters by engaging in acts of altruism [22]. These kind of selfless behaviors in the middle of widespread catastrophes are the consequence of individuals having similar social identities and an emotional connection with others who are going through the same kinds of challenges as they are [22,23]. An excellent approach to persuade people to commit to the in-group cause and drive them to behave in line with that commitment is to publicly demonstrate prosocial and unselfish behaviour [24]. Despite the fact that the COVID-19 pandemic posed a significant threat to the health of people all over the world and that its methods of containment were unprecedented, the pandemic was eventually brought under control. As a direct consequence of this, the crisis in question is unique, and it has compelled significant behavioural shifts, including changes in prosocial behaviour [24]. In addition, earlier studies have shown that exposure to similar calamity and anguish can lead to the development of more altruistic responses [25,26]. Cooperation was also demonstrated by musicians performing concerts from their balconies for the benefit of the community, and by individuals who clapped from their windows to show their appreciation to those working in frontline and healthcare roles [27]. In addition, in order to provide direct assistance to people who are in need, it was necessary for the collaborative efforts to be comprised of both organised volunteer efforts and informal community-based initiatives [28]. According to this point of view, having a traumatic experience creates a sense of shared fate and identity, which may lead to increased empathy and a greater desire to assist those who are in need of assistance.

1.2. Prosocial Behaviour during the COVID-19 Pandemic

During the COVID-19 pandemic, the majority of Canadians exhibited high levels of prosociality and compassion, as well as experienced those high levels themselves [1]. These findings are in line with those obtained in other nations including the United States of America, the United Kingdom, and Germany [29,30,31]. In point of fact, researchers have found that the public health guidelines developed in a country as a result of COVID-19 have created opportunities for increased empathy and prosociality toward other people [29,30]. For example, Andersson et al. found that people with high levels of prosociality are more likely to adhere to public health guidelines than people with low levels of prosociality [29]. These guidelines include things like keeping a social distance, wearing a mask properly, and always following hygiene practices in an effort to protect others from getting sick. This may be due to the fact that people who have higher levels of prosociality view interventions in public health as being acts of kindness performed towards other people.
During the COVID-19 pandemic, a grass-roots movement and various local initiatives emerged with the goal of providing assistance to populations that were at risk [32,33]. It is possible to define a grass-roots movement as the strategies that individuals in a specific district, region, or neighbourhood use as the foundation for political or economic movements [34]. This is analogous to the so-called “common-enemy effect”, which states that individuals are more likely to work together when confronted with a common foe, regardless of whether that foe is caused by nature, an individual, or a group [35,36]. Adversity indeed can bring out the best in people [32,37]. This finding demonstrates that prosocial behaviour will significantly increase during adversities such as the COVID-19 pandemic.

1.3. How Can We Foster More Prosocial Behaviour during the COVID-19 Pandemic?

1.3.1. Moral Elevation

When we see the virtuous actions of another person, we feel a spectrum of pleasant moral emotions, including moral elevation as one of those feelings [38,39]. The idea of moral elevation refers to a collection of reactions that individuals have that inspire them to participate in prosocial behaviour and that are akin to other moral sentiments. This collection of responses is what motivates people to engage in prosocial behaviour [38,39]. Motives associated with moral elevation include the desire to become a better person and the urge to open one’s heart to others. The interrelated components, which include ideas, emotions, motives, and physical changes [40,41]. The aspiration to become a better person and the want to extend one’s heart to others are both desires that are associated with the process of moral elevation [42,43]. It was also demonstrated by Algoe and Haidt that moral elevation involves one’s own subjective feelings, such as a feeling of inspiration or elation, as well as one’s own bodily sensations [44]. In addition to this, holding views that are better from a moral standpoint presents a favourable image of mankind [45,46].
As is the case with other emotions related with morality, moral elevation has the ability to operate as a substantial source of moral inspiration and motivation, directing a person toward acts of goodness and deterring them from participating in immoral behaviour [47,48,49]. Numerous research has provided evidence that demonstrates the enormous effect that moral progress has on prosocial behaviour. Those who have reached a greater degree of moral development, for example, are more likely to assist others and to work toward life objectives that are more ethically oriented. Those who have not reached a higher level of moral development are less inclined to help others [44,50]. There is a link, as stated by Freeman et al., between exposing individuals to acts of moral goodness and increasing the quantity of charitable gifts they make [45]. As a direct consequence of this, moral development has come to be seen as a key driver of prosocial behaviour in humans [38,51]. People will participate in prosocial behaviour during the COVID-19 pandemic when their moral feelings assist to bridge the gap between moral principles and real behaviour [47,52]. Because of this, evoking a higher moral standard not only motivates prosocial behaviour when moral ideas are present, but it also adds to the accumulation of the effects of moral judgement and moral standard in order to inspire prosocial behaviour [53].

1.3.2. Moral Judgement

The ability to generate moral judgments based on one’s own moral beliefs and the ability to conduct in line with those judgements is what is meant by the phrase “moral judgement competence” [54,55]. It is a measurement of how far a person has gone down the path of moral and cognitive growth over the course of their lifetime. According to Kohlberg, the maturation of an individual’s moral and cognitive capabilities takes place over the course of three separate levels, with each level including two phases [54]. These stages are representative of the level of moral reasoning that an individual reaches when they are able to differentiate between appropriate and inappropriate behaviours and are motivated to behave in a prosocial manner. When an individual reaches this level of moral reasoning, they are able to act in a manner that is beneficial to others. Because of the pivotal role that it plays in moral action, research on moral judgement has been carried out for a very long time. According to the theory of cognitive development outlined by [54], moral judgement may intrinsically motivate acts of prosocial behaviour. The growth of a person’s capacities for moral judgement will occur concurrently with the development of that person’s moral ideals. In addition, it is believed that individuals develop the capacity to use their moral beliefs as a foundation for decision making and to control the behaviour of both themselves and others via the cultivation of this skill. As a consequence of this, as individuals become older, their behaviours have a tendency to line more closely with the moral beliefs they’ve established. The results of research carried out by Ding et al. suggest that moral judgement is directly responsible for the motivation of prosocial behaviours such as moral elevation [53]. A person’s aptitude for moral judgement will play a part in deciding whether or not they are willing to engage in prosocial behaviour throughout the course of the COVID-19 epidemic.

1.3.3. Moral Identity

Aquino and Reed explain that the relevance of being moral to the self may be shown by demonstrating that moral identity is built of a set of moral qualities [56]. The foundation for the trait-based definition of honesty that was proposed by Kingsford et al. is the argument that some moral characteristics, such as being kind or helpful, may be more fundamental to an individual’s self-concept than others, such as being honest or generous [57]. A person who considers themselves to be moral will, in order to maintain coherence between their self-definition and their actions, pursue more moral goals and exhibit more prosocial behaviour, such as offering assistance to others and showing concern for the emotions that they are experiencing. This is because a person who considers themselves to be moral will want to maintain coherence between their self-definition and their actions [46]. As a consequence of this, moral identity has also been believed to serve as a means of self-regulation that encourages moral conduct [8,46,57].
Numerous empirical investigations have shown that there is a strong and positive association between moral identity and prosocial behaviour [46,56]. In addition, the study indicated that those who had a strong sense of their own moral identity were more likely to participate in acts of charity and contribute more money than those who did not have a strong sense of their own moral identity [58,59]. As a consequence of this, the outcomes of this research suggest that moral identity might be used in order to explain moral behaviour [58]. Research carried out by Ding et al. indicates that having a moral identity might boost the impact of moral elevation, which in turn motivates prosocial behavior [53]. To summarise, when people have the same moral identity, it may encourage them to imitate the model, but when there is a large gap in moral identity between them, it may cause them to be hesitant to imitate prosocial behaviour. This can be seen in situations where there is a large gap in moral identity between people.

1.4. Why Is Prosocial Behaviour Crucial in Times of Crises?

To build a community that is resilient in the face of hardship, such as the COVID-19 epidemic, members of the community must engage in behaviours that benefit others. One definition of community resilience is a community’s continual capacity to tolerate, adapt to, and recover from misfortune. According to PeConga et al., prosocial behaviours increased community resilience by giving the act of surviving the crises a feeling of purpose and adaptive significance [60]. Communities, for example, have a duty to help individuals in times of greatest need by providing them with both monetary and emotional resources. This obligation exists in order for communities to carry out their role of assisting people. Tolerance, support, and generosity have the potential to serve as a buffer against the negative effects of a crisis situation [60,61]. There are several factors that contribute to a community’s resilience; nevertheless, two of them are likely to be substantial benefits of prosocial behaviour [62]. The first factor is having transformational community potential, which involves the notion of being able to analyse and grasp collective experiences in order to get access to and build community capacities to cope with such experiences [62,63]. The second factor includes perceptions of the community’s capacity to deal with catastrophes, including disaster preparation as well as community rehabilitation [23,62]. Furthermore, it has been demonstrated that engaging in prosocial behaviours may benefit the helper’s bodily and psychological well-being even when the beneficiaries are remote and not physically present. This is true even when the helper is assisting those who are not physically there, and it is true even when the beneficiary is not physically present [25,26,64,65]. These prosocial activities will also have a positive collective impact, such as increased opportunities for social contacts, solidarity, reciprocal support, and a feeling of being a competent and contributing part of the society. These cumulative impacts will benefit the community as a whole [23,66]. The aforementioned components are critical for the development of community resilience [67,68]. As a result, practising prosocial behaviours among community members is projected to increase views of the group’s capacity to manage during the COVID-19 pandemic. It has been shown that practising prosocial behaviours reduces feelings of isolation among community members because it promotes stronger perceptions of the community’s ability to cope [63,69].
At the level of the neighbourhood or the community, Stavrova and Schlosser mentioned that being prosocial can be the foundation of social equity and justice. Consequently, in order to emphasise the significance of prosocial behaviour during the COVID-19 epidemic, it is also essential to understand individuals who did not help, or even worse, those who caused harm to others [70,71]. This is reflected by a personality characteristic known as “justice sensitivity”, which may be described as the individual’s subjective perception of the relative importance that justice plays in their day-to-day existence [72,73]. Individuals with a high level of this trait witness injustices more often, have stronger emotional responses to them, and are more motivated to remedy or prevent them. For instance, certain vulnerable groups have been subjected to ethnic discrimination and environmental racism, both of which have contributed to the absence of prosocial behaviour that these groups have experienced throughout the epidemic. During the COVID-19 outbreak in China, the transport policies have disadvantaged certain population groups and exacerbated transport inequalities [74,75,76]. This is especially true when there are travel restrictions and public transport shutdowns. For instance, residents of non-gated communities were only allowed to engage in outdoor activities within their own neighbourhoods as a result of transit rules, which prevented them from participating in a variety of social and physical activities outside of their communities. This resulted in a significant increase in the levels of stress and depression among the populations that were already at a disadvantage [74]. In this particular instance, it has been demonstrated that a lack of prosocial behaviour may be particularly harmful to certain underprivileged populations, including having a negative impact on their health [77,78].
Ayalon also mentioned that age is a significant risk factor for COVID-19 mortality, and that older people are the ones who are suffering the most as a result of this pandemic [79,80]. For example, research has shown that younger generations frequently mock and scold older generations, particularly when the older generations are unable to regain their mobility despite the strictest travel prohibitions. This is especially true when younger generations see older generations as being unable to restore their mobility [79,81]. As a direct consequence of this, ageism and intergenerational conflict have become more prevalent around the world, to the point where senior citizens frequently face prejudice, discrimination, and stereotyping [82,83]. Aside from that, as was stated earlier, people who have a higher level of prosociality have a tendency to view public health measures as an act of kindness towards society. Research also highlights the fact that the older generation is seen as a threat to society because they are more self-centered and have a lower level of prosociality [84]. This is due to the fact that they typically do not adhere to the recommendations provided by the public health sector, such as keeping an appropriate social distance and refusing to wear face masks correctly [30,85]. However, other studies contend that the older generation was portrayed as a selfless and vulnerable group that was willing to jeopardize its own well-being in times of resource scarcity in order to benefit society as a whole [86]. This is a claim that is supported by the fact that the older generation is the largest demographic in the United States. For example, Dan Patrick, the lieutenant governor of Texas, has stated that he would prefer to die than to bring the nation’s economy to its knees [87]. These claims are in line with the findings of a previous study, which discovered that older people are typically expected to step down from their positions in favour of younger generations simply due to the chronological age gap that exists between the two groups [88,89].

1.5. Helping One Another Is Essential during the COVID-19 Pandemic

During the COVID-19 outbreak, the physical separation and quarantine rules severely disrupted people’s normal daily social contacts and promoted social isolation, which stands in stark contrast to the natural need that people have to interact with one another [90]. It has become more difficult to help other people as a result of the decline in the number of social contacts as well as the emergence of feelings of dread, insecurity, and tension [91]. This can lead to having concern only for oneself and contempt for those around you. In point of fact, it was found that the pandemic and behaviours associated with it caused a variety of responses that could be categorized as either self-centered or antisocial. These included issues such as overbuying and food stockpiling [92], which resulted in a lack of those goods and led to shortages. This school of thought contends that this loss of prosocial tendencies is caused by competition for those resources [32]. During times of crisis, people lose their prosocial tendencies due to struggles for scarce resources.
Research also highlights the importance of social norms in guiding people’s decisions about whether or not to act in a manner that is more self-interested or prosocial [93,94]. In times of widespread hardship, prosocial behaviour can be linked to benefits not only for the individual but also for society as a whole, which positions it as a factor that has the potential to serve as an effective protective buffer. For instance, if one complies with public health measures (such as wearing a mask), this will reduce the likelihood of the COVID-19 virus being passed on to others [94]. Acts that are done with the intention of helping other people are particularly important in light of the immense suffering that was caused by the COVID-19 pandemic. A study from the United States offers preliminary evidence that being prosocial during the COVID-19 pandemic increases positive affect, empathy, and social connectedness. This research shows that helping others has an immediate positive impact on the mood of the person doing the helping [63]. This finding is consistent with the phenomenon called the “common-enemy effect”, which holds that confronting a common enemy (such as one created by nature, a person, or a group) fosters cooperation because of the need to work together to defeat that enemy [35].
When we choose to tolerate the conditions during the COVID-19 pandemic that allow self-centeredness to flourish, we are making decisions that affect not only those who are affected by our self-centeredness at the time, but also the world we all live in and share, and self-centeredness, like the COVID-19 virus, spreads. To counter the impact of self-centeredness, one must do more than merely reject it. It entails accepting prosociality as a strategy of creating an environment that fosters health. In order to combat COVID-19, perhaps all that is necessary is for us to practice more prosocial behaviour. Rather than fighting COVID-19, we must embrace the interconnectedness that prosociality generates by saying: “we are all in this together”.

2. Conclusions

Engaging in prosocial behaviours can help to build community resilience, which can lead to the sustained ability of a community to withstand, adapt, and recover from adversity quickly. To increase the number of individuals engaging in prosocial behaviour during the COVID-19 pandemic, it is essential to promote moral elevation, moral discernment, and moral identity. Our assertion is congruent with Ding et al.’s conceptual model, which explores the underlying mechanism of morality on prosocial behavior [53]. Determining the mediating role of moral elevation on the relationship between moral judgement and prosocial behaviour, as well as the moderating influence of moral identity within the moral-self regulation system, is necessary to further study this conceptual paradigm during the COVID-19 pandemic. This is because each of these moral aspects has a distinct role in the system. In addition, this article discussed the various ways in which age and justice sensitivity influence the prosocial behaviours and moral motivations that people exhibit during COVID-19. As a result, we recommend that future researchers incorporate these two variables into the model developed by [53]. Figure 1 presents the proposed conceptual framework of the moderated mediation model.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, W.W. and M.X.L.; methodology, W.W. and M.X.L.; software, W.W. and M.X.L.; validation, W.W. and M.X.L.; formal analysis, W.W. and M.X.L.; investigation, W.W. and M.X.L.; resources, W.W. and M.X.L.; data curation, W.W. and M.X.L.; writing—original draft preparation, W.W. and M.X.L.; writing—review and editing, L.S.W., C.K.C., & S.S.M.; visualization, W.W. and M.X.L.; supervision, W.W.; project administration, W.W. and M.X.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Shillington, K.J.; Vanderloo, L.M.; Burke, S.M.; Ng, V.; Tucker, P.; Irwin, J.D. A cross-sectional examination of Canadian adults’ prosocial behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic. J. Rural Ment. Health 2022, 46, 174–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Cucinotta, D.; Vanelli, M. WHO declares COVID-19 a pandemic. Acta Bio Med. Atenei Parm. 2020, 91, 157. [Google Scholar]
  3. Cerutti, G.; Guo, Y.; Zhou, T.; Gorman, J.; Lee, M.; Rapp, M.; Reddem, E.R.; Yu, J.; Bahna, F.; Bimela, J. Potent SARS-CoV-2 neutralizing antibodies directed against spike N-terminal domain target a single supersite. Cell Host Microbe 2021, 29, 819–833.e7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Plümper, T.; Neumayer, E. Lockdown policies and the dynamics of the first wave of the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic in Europe. J. Eur. Public Policy 2022, 29, 321–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Shah, A.U.M.; Safri, S.N.A.; Thevadas, R.; Noordin, N.K.; Abd Rahman, A.; Sekawi, Z.; Ideris, A.; Sultan, M.T.H. COVID-19 outbreak in Malaysia: Actions taken by the Malaysian government. Int. J. Infect. Dis. 2020, 97, 108–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Wider, W.; Taib, N.M.; Khadri, M.W.A.B.A.; Yip, F.Y.; Lajuma, S.; Punniamoorthy, P.A. The Unique Role of Hope and Optimism in the Relationship between Environmental Quality and Life Satisfaction during COVID-19 Pandemic. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 7661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Pang, N.T.P.; Hadi, N.M.N.; Mohaini, M.I.; Kamu, A.; Ho, C.M.; Koh, E.B.Y.; Loo, J.L.; Theng, D.Q.L.; Wider, W. Factors Contributing to Burnout among Healthcare Workers during COVID-19 in Sabah (East Malaysia). Healthcare 2022, 10, 1068. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Brooks, S.K.; Webster, R.K.; Smith, L.E.; Woodland, L.; Wessely, S.; Greenberg, N.; Rubin, G.J. The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: Rapid review of the evidence. Lancet 2020, 395, 912–920. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. de Figueiredo, C.S.; Sandre, P.C.; Portugal, L.C.L.; Mázala-de-Oliveira, T.; da Silva Chagas, L.; Raony, Í.; Ferreira, E.S.; Giestal-de-Araujo, E.; Dos Santos, A.A.; Bomfim, P.O.-S. COVID-19 pandemic impact on children and adolescents’ mental health: Biological, environmental, and social factors. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2021, 106, 110171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Eisenberg, N.; VanSchyndel, S.K.; Spinrad, T.L. Prosocial motivation: Inferences from an opaque body of work. Child Dev. 2016, 87, 1668–1678. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Layous, K.; Chancellor, J.; Lyubomirsky, S. Positive activities as protective factors against mental health conditions. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2014, 123, 3–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Layous, K.; Nelson, S.K.; Kurtz, J.L.; Lyubomirsky, S. What triggers prosocial effort? A positive feedback loop between positive activities, kindness, and well-being. J. Posit. Psychol. 2017, 12, 385–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Pressman, S.D.; Kraft, T.L.; Cross, M.P. It’s good to do good and receive good: The impact of a ‘pay it forward’style kindness intervention on giver and receiver well-being. J. Posit. Psychol. 2015, 10, 293–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Gilbert, P.; Basran, J.; MacArthur, M.; Kirby, J.N. Differences in the semantics of prosocial words: An exploration of compassion and kindness. Mindfulness 2019, 10, 2259–2271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Coyne, S.M.; Padilla-Walker, L.M.; Holmgren, H.G.; Davis, E.J.; Collier, K.M.; Memmott-Elison, M.K.; Hawkins, A.J. A meta-analysis of prosocial media on prosocial behavior, aggression, and empathic concern: A multidimensional approach. Dev. Psychol. 2018, 54, 331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Padilla-Walker, L.M.; Carlo, G. The study of prosocial behavior: Past, present, and future. In Prosocial Development: A Multidimensional Approach; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2014; pp. 3–16. [Google Scholar]
  17. Eisenberg, N.; Spinrad, T.L. Multidimensionality of prosocial behavior: Rethinking the conceptualization and development of prosocial behavior. In Prosocial Development: A Multidimensional Approach; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2014; pp. 17–39. [Google Scholar]
  18. Niesta Kayser, D.; Greitemeyer, T.; Fischer, P.; Frey, D. Why mood affects help giving, but not moral courage: Comparing two types of prosocial behaviour. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2010, 40, 1136–1157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Yang, Y.; Kong, X.; Guo, Z.; Kou, Y. Can self-compassion promote gratitude and prosocial behavior in adolescents? A 3-year longitudinal study from China. Mindfulness 2021, 12, 1377–1386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Nowell, B.; Boyd, N. Viewing community as responsibility as well as resource: Deconstructing the theoretical roots of psychological sense of community. J. Community Psychol. 2010, 38, 828–841. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Boyd, N.M.; Nowell, B. Psychological sense of community: A new construct for the field of management. J. Manag. Inq. 2014, 23, 107–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Zaki, J. Integrating empathy and interpersonal emotion regulation. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2020, 71, 517–540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Drury, J. The role of social identity processes in mass emergency behaviour: An integrative review. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 2018, 29, 38–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Bavel, J.J.V.; Baicker, K.; Boggio, P.S.; Capraro, V.; Cichocka, A.; Cikara, M.; Crockett, M.J.; Crum, A.J.; Douglas, K.M.; Druckman, J.N. Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2020, 4, 460–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Martela, F.; Ryan, R.M. Prosocial behavior increases well-being and vitality even without contact with the beneficiary: Causal and behavioral evidence. Motiv. Emot. 2016, 40, 351–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Curry, O.S.; Rowland, L.A.; Van Lissa, C.J.; Zlotowitz, S.; McAlaney, J.; Whitehouse, H. Happy to help? A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of performing acts of kindness on the well-being of the actor. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2018, 76, 320–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Taylor, L.K. The Developmental Peacebuilding Model (DPM) of children’s prosocial behaviors in settings of intergroup conflict. Child Dev. Perspect. 2020, 14, 127–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Haller, E.; Lubenko, J.; Presti, G.; Squatrito, V.; Constantinou, M.; Nicolaou, C.; Papacostas, S.; Aydın, G.; Chong, Y.Y.; Chien, W.T. To help or not to help? prosocial behavior, its association with well-being, and predictors of prosocial behavior during the coronavirus disease pandemic. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 775032. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Andersson, O.; Campos-Mercade, P.; Meier, A.N.; Wengström, E. Anticipation of COVID-19 vaccines reduces willingness to socially distance. J. Health Econ. 2021, 80, 102530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Pfattheicher, S.; Nockur, L.; Böhm, R.; Sassenrath, C.; Petersen, M.B. The emotional path to action: Empathy promotes physical distancing and wearing of face masks during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychol. Sci. 2020, 31, 1363–1373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Sin, N.L.; Klaiber, P.; Wen, J.H.; DeLongis, A. Helping amid the pandemic: Daily affective and social implications of COVID-19-related prosocial activities. Gerontologist 2021, 61, 59–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Hellmann, D.M.; Dorrough, A.R.; Glöckner, A. Prosocial behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic in Germany. The role of responsibility and vulnerability. Heliyon 2021, 7, e08041. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Zerbian, T.; Adams, M.; Dooris, M.; Pool, U. The Role of Local Authorities in Shaping Local Food Systems. Sustainability 2022, 14, 12004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Rodríguez, M.C.; Di Virgilio, M.M. A city for all? Public policy and resistance to gentrification in the southern neighborhoods of Buenos Aires. Urban Geogr. 2016, 37, 1215–1234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Henrich, N.; Henrich, J.P. Why Humans Cooperate: A Cultural and Evolutionary Explanation; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  36. Ostrom, E.; Burger, J.; Field, C.B.; Norgaard, R.B.; Policansky, D. Revisiting the commons: Local lessons, global challenges. Science 1999, 284, 278–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. Tatham, M.; Bauer, M.W. The state, the economy, and the regions: Theories of preference formation in times of crisis. J. Public Adm. Res. Theory 2016, 26, 631–646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Haidt, J. Elevation and the positive psychology of morality. In Flourishing: Positive Psychology and the Life Well-Lived; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2003; pp. 275–289. [Google Scholar]
  39. Vadera, A.K.; Pathki, C.S. Competition and cheating: Investigating the role of moral awareness, moral identity, and moral elevation. J. Organ. Behav. 2021, 42, 1060–1081. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Izard, C.E.; Kobak, R.R. Emotions system functioning and emotion regulation. In The Development of Emotion Regulation and Dysregulation; Cambridge Studies in Social and Emotional Development; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1991; pp. 303–321. [Google Scholar]
  41. Ekman, P. Are there basic emotions? Psychol. Rev. 1992, 99, 550–553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Keltner, D.; Haidt, J. Approaching awe, a moral, spiritual, and aesthetic emotion. Cogn. Emot. 2003, 17, 297–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Silvers, J.A.; Haidt, J. Moral elevation can induce nursing. Emotion 2008, 8, 291–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Algoe, S.B.; Haidt, J. Witnessing excellence in action: The ‘other-praising’emotions of elevation, gratitude, and admiration. J. Posit. Psychol. 2009, 4, 105–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Freeman, D.; Aquino, K.; McFerran, B. Overcoming beneficiary race as an impediment to charitable donations: Social dominance orientation, the experience of moral elevation, and donation behavior. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2009, 35, 72–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Aquino, K.; McFerran, B.; Laven, M. Moral identity and the experience of moral elevation in response to acts of uncommon goodness. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 100, 703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Hoffman, M.L. Toward a comprehensive empathy-based theory of prosocial moral development. In Constructive & Destructive Behavior: Implications for Family, School, & Society; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2001; pp. 61–86. [Google Scholar]
  48. Jones, A.; Fitness, J. Moral hypervigilance: The influence of disgust sensitivity in the moral domain. Emotion 2008, 8, 613–627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Lai, C.K.; Haidt, J.; Nosek, B.A. Moral elevation reduces prejudice against gay men. Cogn. Emot. 2014, 28, 781–794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Van de Vyver, J.; Abrams, D. Testing the prosocial effectiveness of the prototypical moral emotions: Elevation increases benevolent behaviors and outrage increases justice behaviors. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2015, 58, 23–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Schnall, S.; Roper, J.; Fessler, D.M. Elevation leads to altruistic behavior. Psychol. Sci. 2010, 21, 315–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Ayal, S.; Gino, F.; Barkan, R.; Ariely, D. Three principles to REVISE people’s unethical behavior. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2015, 10, 738–741. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Ding, W.; Shao, Y.; Sun, B.; Xie, R.; Li, W.; Wang, X. How can prosocial behavior be motivated? The different roles of moral judgment, moral elevation, and moral identity among the young Chinese. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Kohlberg, L. Revisions in the theory and practice of moral development. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 1978, 1978, 83–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Martins, V.S.M.; Santos, C.M.N.C.; Bataglia, P.U.R.; Duarte, I.M.R.F. The teaching of ethics and the moral competence of medical and nursing students. Health Care Anal. 2021, 29, 113–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Aquino, K.; Reed II, A. The self-importance of moral identity. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 83, 1423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Kingsford, J.M.; Hawes, D.J.; de Rosnay, M. The moral self and moral identity: Developmental questions and conceptual challenges. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2018, 36, 652–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Hardy, S.A. Identity, reasoning, and emotion: An empirical comparison of three sources of moral motivation. Motiv. Emot. 2006, 30, 205–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Reed, A.; Aquino, K.; Levy, E. Moral identity and judgments of charitable behaviors. J. Mark. 2007, 71, 178–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. PeConga, E.K.; Gauthier, G.M.; Holloway, A.; Walker, R.S.; Rosencrans, P.L.; Zoellner, L.A.; Bedard-Gilligan, M. Resilience is spreading: Mental health within the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychol. Trauma: Theory Res. Pract. Policy 2020, 12, S47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Thompson, J.; Grubb, A.R.; Ebner, N.; Chirico, A.; Pizzolante, M. Increasing crisis hostage negotiator effectiveness: Embracing awe and other resilience practices. Cardozo J. Confl. Resol. 2022, 23, 615. [Google Scholar]
  62. Pfefferbaum, B.; Newman, E.; Nelson, S.D.; Nitiéma, P.; Pfefferbaum, R.L.; Rahman, A. Disaster media coverage and psychological outcomes: Descriptive findings in the extant research. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2014, 16, 464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Varma, P.; Junge, M.; Meaklim, H.; Jackson, M.L. Younger people are more vulnerable to stress, anxiety and depression during COVID-19 pandemic: A global cross-sectional survey. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2021, 109, 110236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Dunn, M.; Sheehan, M.; Hordern, J.; Turnham, H.L.; Wilkinson, D. ‘Your country needs you’: The ethics of allocating staff to high-risk clinical roles in the management of patients with COVID-19. J. Med. Ethics 2020, 46, 436–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Pozzi, M.; Marta, E.; Marzana, D.; Gozzoli, C.; Ruggieri, R. The effect of the psychological sense of community on the psychological well-being in older volunteers. Eur. J. Psychol. 2014, 10, 598–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Vezzali, L.; Drury, J.; Versari, A.; Cadamuro, A. Sharing distress increases helping and contact intentions via social identification and inclusion of the other in the self: Children’s prosocial behaviour after an earthquake. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 2016, 19, 314–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Jetten, J. Together Apart: The Psychology of COVID-19; SAGE: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  68. Norris, F.H.; Stevens, S.P.; Pfefferbaum, B.; Wyche, K.F.; Pfefferbaum, R.L. Community resilience as a metaphor, theory, set of capacities, and strategy for disaster readiness. Am. J. Community Psychol. 2008, 41, 127–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Magis, K. Community Resilience: An Indicator of Social Sustainability. Soc. Nat. Resour. 2010, 23, 401–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Stavrova, O.; Schlösser, T. Solidarity and social justice: Effect of individual differences in justice sensitivity on solidarity behaviour. Eur. J. Personal. 2015, 29, 2–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Zou, Y.; Wang, Y.; Yang, X.; Jiang, R. Observed ostracism and compensatory behavior: A moderated mediation model of empathy and observer justice sensitivity. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2022, 198, 111829. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Baumert, A.; Schmitt, M. Justice Sensitivity. In Handbook of Social Justice Theory and Research; Sabbagh, C., Schmitt, M., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Gollwitzer, M.; Platzer, C.; Zwarg, C.; Göritz, A.S. Public acceptance of COVID-19 lockdown scenarios. Int. J. Psychol. 2021, 56, 551–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Liu, Q.; Liu, Y.; Zhang, C.; An, Z.; Zhao, P. Elderly mobility during the COVID-19 pandemic: A qualitative exploration in Kunming, China. J. Transp. Geogr. 2021, 96, 103176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Figliozzi, M.; Unnikrishnan, A. Exploring the impact of socio-demographic characteristics, health concerns, and product type on home delivery rates and expenditures during a strict COVID-19 lockdown period: A case study from Portland, OR. Transp. Res. Part A Policy Pract. 2021, 153, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Valenzuela-Levi, N.; Fuentes, L.; Ramirez, M.; Rodriguez, S.; Señoret, A. Urban sustainability and perceived satisfaction in neoliberal cities. Cities 2022, 126, 103647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Montada, L.; Schneider, A. Justice and emotional reactions to victims. Soc. Justice Res. 1988, 3, 313–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Oh, M.-J.; Jung, J.C. Does social exclusion cause people to make more donations? J. Asian Financ. Econ. Bus. 2018, 5, 129–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Ayalon, L. There is nothing new under the sun: Ageism and intergenerational tension in the age of the COVID-19 outbreak. Int. Psychogeriatr. 2020, 32, 1221–1224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Meisner, B.A. Are you OK, Boomer? Intensification of ageism and intergenerational tensions on social media amid COVID-19. Leis. Sci. 2021, 43, 56–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Zhang, J.; Liu, X. Media representation of older people’s vulnerability during the COVID-19 pandemic in China. Eur. J. Ageing 2021, 18, 149–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Ayalon, L. Satisfaction with aging results in reduced risk for falling. Int. Psychogeriatr. 2016, 28, 741–747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Ayalon, L.; Peisah, C.; de Mendonça Lima, C.; Verbeek, H.; Rabheru, K. Ageism and the state of older people with mental conditions during the pandemic and beyond: Manifestations, etiology, consequences, and future directions. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2021, 29, 995–999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Cutler, J.; Nitschke, J.P.; Lamm, C.; Lockwood, P.L. Older adults across the globe exhibit increased prosocial behavior but also greater in-group preferences. Nat. Aging 2021, 1, 880–888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Eckersley, P. Chinese Woman Refuses to Wear Face Mask Amid Coronavirus Crisis. Available online: https://www.dailymail.co.uk/news/article-7944045/Chinese-woman-confronted-police-refusing-wear-face-mask-coronavirus-outbreaks.html (accessed on 18 September 2022).
  86. Hubbard, J.; Harbaugh, W.T.; Srivastava, S.; Degras, D.; Mayr, U. A general benevolence dimension that links neural, psychological, economic, and life-span data on altruistic tendencies. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2016, 145, 1351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Beckett, L. Older People Would Rather Die than Let COVID-19 Harm U.S. Economy—Texas Official. The Guardian. 24 March 2020. Available online: https://www.theguardian.com/world/2020/mar/24/older-people-would-rather-die-than-let-covid-19-lockdownharm-us-economy-texas-official-dan-patrick (accessed on 18 September 2022).
  88. North, M.; Fiske, S. A prescriptive intergenerational-tension ageism scale: Succession, identity, and consumption (SIC). Psychol. Assess. 2013, 25, 706–713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  89. Calasanti, T. Brown slime, the silver tsunami, and apocalyptic demography: The importance of ageism and age relations. Soc. Curr. 2020, 7, 195–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Jarvis, C.I.; Van Zandvoort, K.; Gimma, A.; Prem, K.; Klepac, P.; Rubin, G.J.; Edmunds, W.J. Quantifying the impact of physical distance measures on the transmission of COVID-19 in the UK. BMC Med. 2020, 18, 124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Luo, M.; Guo, L.; Yu, M.; Jiang, W.; Wang, H. The psychological and mental impact of coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) on medical staff and general public–A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 2020, 291, 113190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  92. Wang, E.; An, N.; Gao, Z.; Kiprop, E.; Geng, X. Consumer food stockpiling behavior and willingness to pay for food reserves in COVID-19. Food Secur. 2020, 12, 739–747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  93. Abel, M.; Brown, W. Prosocial Behavior in the Time of COVID-19: The Effect of Private and Public Role Models; IZA Institute of Labor Economics: Bonn, Germany, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  94. Cappelen, A.W.; Falch, R.; Sørensen, E.Ø.; Tungodden, B. Solidarity and fairness in times of crisis. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2021, 186, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Conceptual model of the relationships between moral judgement, moral elevation, moral identity, justice sensitivity, age, and prosocial behaviour.
Figure 1. Conceptual model of the relationships between moral judgement, moral elevation, moral identity, justice sensitivity, age, and prosocial behaviour.
Ijerph 19 16084 g001
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Wider, W.; Lim, M.X.; Wong, L.S.; Chan, C.K.; Maidin, S.S. Should I Help? Prosocial Behaviour during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 16084. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192316084

AMA Style

Wider W, Lim MX, Wong LS, Chan CK, Maidin SS. Should I Help? Prosocial Behaviour during the COVID-19 Pandemic. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(23):16084. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192316084

Chicago/Turabian Style

Wider, Walton, Mei Xian Lim, Ling Shing Wong, Choon Kit Chan, and Siti Sarah Maidin. 2022. "Should I Help? Prosocial Behaviour during the COVID-19 Pandemic" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 23: 16084. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192316084

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop