Next Article in Journal
Factors That Affect the COVID-19 Pandemic in Summer 2022 Compared to Summer 2021
Next Article in Special Issue
Intergenerational Relations in the Climate Movement: Bridging the Gap toward a Common Goal
Previous Article in Journal
Rural Farmers’ Cognition and Climate Change Adaptation Impact on Cash Crop Productivity: Evidence from a Recent Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
Psychosocial Outcomes of Age Integration Status: Do Age-Integrated Social Networks Benefit Older Adults?
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Digital and Non-Digital Solidarity between Older Parents and Their Middle-Aged Children: Associations with Mental Health during the COVID-19 Pandemic

1
Department of Human Development and Family Sciences, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX 79409, USA
2
Department of Human Development and Family Science, Syracuse University, Syracuse, NY 13244, USA
3
School of Social Work, Syracuse University, Syracuse, NY 13244, USA
4
Aging Studies Institute, Syracuse University, Syracuse, NY 13244, USA
5
Department of Sociology, Syracuse University, Syracuse, NY 13244, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(19), 12560; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191912560
Submission received: 11 September 2022 / Revised: 27 September 2022 / Accepted: 29 September 2022 / Published: 1 October 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Intergenerational Programs and Health)

Abstract

:
We incorporated intergenerational digital communication (frequency of texting, video call, and social media interaction) into the intergenerational solidarity paradigm and identified new types of intergenerational and digital solidarity with adult children among older parents during the COVID-19 pandemic. In addition, we examined whether those types are associated with older parents’ mental health (depressive symptoms, psychological well-being, and self-esteem). We used the 2021/2022 wave of the Longitudinal Study of Generations (LSOG), and a sample of 519 older parents (mean age = 69 years). Latent class analysis identified four classes describing intergenerational and digital solidarity with adult children (distant-but-digitally connected, tight-knit-traditional, detached, and ambivalent). We found that older parents who had distant-but-digitally connected and tight-knit-traditional relationships with their adult children reported better mental health, compared to those who had detached and ambivalent relationships with their adult children during the COVID-19 pandemic. Our findings suggest that intergenerational digital communication should be considered as a digital solidarity in intergenerational solidarity paradigm, which is useful for measuring multidimension of intergenerational relationships within family members during and after the pandemic.

1. Introduction

Lockdown restrictions and social distancing measures in response to the COVID-19 pandemic have resulted in dramatic reductions in social interaction between family members, increasing the risk of social isolation and worsening mental health among older adults [1,2]. Psychological distress and loneliness are higher than usual at older ages during the pandemic [3,4,5]. Being a major part of older adults’ social systems, intergenerational relationships are primary sources of social contact and support to alleviate depression or isolation during the outbreak of COVID-19 [6,7].
However, with limited face-to-face contact, family members may turn to digital forms of communication to strengthen social integration between generations during the pandemic [8]. Many studies focused on the types of intergenerational contact and their associations with older adults’ well-being [9,10]. However, older parents and adult children are interconnected in various dimensions of intergenerational relationships simultaneously [11,12]. Changes in the form of contact are associated with changes in other domains of relationships. It is difficult to understand the impact of intergenerational relationships on older parents’ mental health without considering the interrelationships among different dimensions of intergenerational relationships. As a result, little is known about how multiple dimensions of intergenerational ties combined with digital communication affect older adults’ mental health during the pandemic. To fill this gap, this study developed a typology of intergenerational and digital solidarity to examine the different patterns of the multifaceted parent–child relationships during the pandemic and the association between types of parent–child solidarity and older parents’ mental health.

2. Literature Review

2.1. Intergenerational Solidarity

Intergenerational solidarity is defined as the social cohesion among family members across generations [11]. As a systematic scheme to organize and understand the interrelated domains in intergenerational relationships, the intergenerational solidarity paradigm was initially composed of six dimensions: affection (emotional closeness), association (frequency of interaction), structure (geographic proximity), consensus (agreement on values), filial norms (familial obligations), and function (exchange of support and assistance) [11]. While intergenerational solidarity was originally developed to depict the positive interactions in intergenerational relations, the dimension of conflict was added to the framework in later refinements to account for both the positive and negative aspects of these relationships [13]. Instead of being the opposite of affection, conflict can simultaneously occur with affection in interpersonal relationships. Such contradiction experienced in family relations due to the coexistence of positive and negative emotions is described as ambivalence [14]. The solidarity-conflict model thus was adapted to capture both the negative aspects and the ambivalence in family life.
In accordance with the theory, empirical studies have shown that intergenerational solidarity is a multidimensional construct. Researchers have identified different types of intergenerational relations based on the dimensions of relations examined in clustering analysis. Studies that examined positive attributes of solidarity have revealed five intergenerational solidarity types: tight-knit, sociable, intimate but distant, obligatory, and detached [12,15]. Studies focusing on the affection and conflict dimensions have identified four types of intergenerational solidarity across cultures: amicable, detached, ambivalent, and disharmonious [16,17]. When investigating a larger range of solidarity attributes and conflict, Van Gaalen and Dykstra [18] classified intergenerational relations into five types: harmonious, ambivalent, obligatory, affective, and discordant. Although studies vary in the selected domains examined in their analyses, only a few typology studies, have examined associational solidarity [12,16,17,19]. Even more rare are studies examining digital communications as a complement to in-person and telephone contact [18,20,21].

2.2. Digital Solidarity in Intergenerational Relationships

Digital solidarity is proposed as an extended dimension of associational solidarity to acknowledge the role of digital forms of communication in maintaining intergenerational solidarity [22]. With the widespread use of smartphones, digital communication use is growing among older generations. For example, in 2021, 73% of adults ages 50–64 reported using social media sites, and 45% of adults ages 65 and older reported doing so [23]. The advance of communication technology is linked to increased frequency of interaction with family members of another generation. In data collected from 2008 to 2010, only 31.1% of surveyed mothers reported instant messaging or emailing their adult children in the previous year, whereas in 2016, 95.4% of mothers reported texting or emailing their adult children in the previous year [22]. Digital communication has known impacts on other dimensions of solidarity. Shifts in digital communication are associated with corresponding changes in-person communication and helping behavior in intergenerational relationships [24]. Further, parents and adult children can exchange expressive support promptly by virtue of the immediacy of digital communication [25]. Therefore, digital communication across generations represents a novel and possibly independent domain of intergenerational solidarity and requires further investigation.
Digital communication has played a vital role in family members staying connected and supported during the COVID-19 pandemic. The main consequence of the policy measures to contain the COVID-19 pandemic was the reduction in physical contact and lack of social support, which were detrimental to older adults’ mental health [2]. Because of older adults’ higher risks of COVID-19 infections and severe illness [26], younger generations tended to reduce their face-to-face contact with older parents. Digital technologies provided various options for intergenerational communication that required no physical contact, less time investment, and lower or no cost, such as FaceTime, Messenger, and Skype [27]. The virtuality of digital communication made it exceptionally valuable for older parents and adult children to stay connected during the COVID-19 pandemic. Using a national panel sample of adults older than 50 years, one study found an increase in physical and social isolation, but no change in digital isolation, when the social distancing policy was implemented [28]. About half of older adults increased digital communication (i.e., video calls, instant messages, and social media) during COVID lockdowns, especially among those who reduced face-to-face contact [8]. Therefore, digital communication enables older parents, who are geographically distant from adult children or have insufficient face-to-face contact, to stay connected.

2.3. Intergenerational and Digital Solidarity and Older Adults’ Mental Heath

The role of intergenerational solidarity in older adults’ mental health has been examined extensively in previous decades [29,30]. Existing literature assessing the well-being of older adults has focused on one dimension of intergenerational relationships [29,31,32] or typologies of intergenerational relationships based on multiple dimensions [33]. Older adults in the tight-knit type of parent–child relationship reported the lowest depressive symptoms, while those in the ambivalent type reported the highest depressive symptoms [34]. In the context of the COVID-19 pandemic, it was not clear whether the association between typologies of intergenerational solidarity and older adults’ mental health would be impacted by the reduction in face-to-face contact.
Although digital communication has been recommended as a strategy to combat social isolation and mental illness caused by COVID-19 [35], empirical studies have yielded mixed findings regarding the association between digital communication and older adults’ mental health (Teo et al., 2015). Some studies found that older adults who reported increased or unchanged frequency of digital contact with non-coresident family members were less likely to experience increased depressive symptoms compared to those who reported decreased digital contact [3,10]. Other research addressed the positive emotions associated with more frequent contact with family members using phone or email for older adults in long-term care facilities [7]. Using longitudinal data, digital communication was found to be negatively associated with loneliness among older adults through the mechanism of social support and social contact [36,37]. However, other studies found that digital forms of communication were associated with negative effects on older adults’ mental health [38], especially for those who lived alone [39]. Some studies have shown that digital contact is not a high-quality alternative to in-person contact, given the unique benefits of face-to-face contact with family members to older adults’ general mental health during the outbreak [9,40]. These previous studies have focused on the frequency of physical and digital contact during the pandemic, but little attention has been paid to the other dimensions of solidarity that interrelate with various forms of contact in parent and adult-child relationships. Thus, in the current study, we examine overall patterns of intergenerational and digital solidarity and traditional non-digital dimensions of solidarity, and their associations with older adults’ mental health.

2.4. The Current Study

The first aim of this study was to integrate digital communication with legacy dimensions of intergenerational solidarity to identify types of relationships with adult children as reported by older parents during the COVID-19 pandemic. We included affectual, consensual, associational (in-person and phone contact), structural, and conflict dimensions of solidarity, and added digital solidarity (texting, video call, and social media interaction). We anticipated that a distant-but-digitally connected type would emerge in the data alongside other types such as types that are strongly connected on all dimensions of solidarity and weakly connected on all dimensions of solidarity. The second aim was to determine whether having digitally connected relationships with children was associated with older parents’ mental health (depressive symptoms, psychological well-being, and self-esteem). We hypothesized that older parents who had strong solidarity with their adult children on all dimensions during the pandemic would report better mental health, compared to those with weak solidarity with their adult children, but that digital communication would compensate for low amounts of more traditional forms of contact.

3. Methods

3.1. Sample

This study used data from the Longitudinal Study of Generations (LSOG), a multigenerational and multi-panel study of 3681 individuals within 418 families. The LSOG began in 1971 (Wave-1) as a cross-sectional study. Three-generation families including grandparents (G1), parents (G2), and grandchildren (G3) were recruited from a large health maintenance organization in southern California. The LSOG became the longitudinal study in 1985 (Wave-2) and continued to collect data from respondents up to 2022 for a total of ten waves of measurement. In addition, great-grandchildren (G4) were added to the sample on a rolling basis beginning in the 1991 study (Wave-4) when they reached the age of 16. Data were initially collected solely by mail-back paper questionnaire and the web-survey option was added beginning with the 2005 study (Wave-8) (for more details, see Bengtson, 2013).
Given that this investigation focused on older adults’ intergenerational and digital solidarity with middle-aged children and their well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic, we used data from the G3 generation in Wave-10 (November 2021 to April 2022). Among the 618 G3s who participated in Wave-10, we derived a subsample of 553 G3s and relied on their reports of intergenerational solidarity with their oldest child because firstborns are, on average, more intensively involved with their parents than are other children [41,42]. Given that our interest was in older parents who lived apart from their adult children, we excluded 34 G3s who lived with their oldest child. The final sample consisted of 519 G3s with a mean age of 69 years.

3.2. Measures

Dependent Variables. Analyses assessed three aspects of mental health: depressive symptoms, psychological well-being, and self-esteem. Depressive symptoms were measured by eight negative affect items (e.g., “I was bothered by things that do not usually bother me”) and two positive affect items (e.g., “I enjoyed life”) from the Center of Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CES-D) short form [43,44]. Response options ranged from (1) rarely or none of the time to (4) most or all of the time. After reverse coding the two positively worded items, we used a mean score of ten items, with higher scores indicating more depressive symptoms (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.80).
Psychological well-being was measured by five positive affect items (e.g., “Did you ever feel pleased about having accomplished something?”) and five negative items (e.g., “Did you ever feel upset because someone criticized you?) from the Bradburn Scale of Psychological Well-Being [45]. Response options were (0) no or (1) yes. After reverse coding the negative affect items, we summed all items (range: 0–10), with higher scores indicating better psychological well-being (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.65).
Self-esteem was measured by two negatively worded items (e.g., “All in all, I am inclined to feel that I am a failure”) and two positively worded items (e.g., “On the whole, I am satisfied with myself”) from the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale [46]. Response options ranged from (1) strongly disagree to (4) strongly agree. After reverse coding the two negatively worded items, we used a mean score of four items, with higher scores indicating greater self-esteem (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.75).
Independent Variables. The LSOG collected data on six dimensions of intergenerational solidarity—affectual, consensual, associational, digital, structural, and conflict—with adult children. Affectual solidarity with a child was measured by one item: “Taking everything into consideration, how close do you feel the relationship is between you and your child at this point in your life?” Responses ranged from (1) not at all close to (6) extremely close. Consensual solidarity with a child was measured by one item: “How similar are your opinions and values about life to those of your child at this point in time?” Responses ranged from (1) not at all similar to (6) extremely similar. Associational solidarity was measured by two separate items for frequency of in-person contact and frequency of audio telephone contact with a child. Responses ranged from (1) not at all to (6) daily or more often. Digital solidarity was measured by three separate items for frequency of texting, video calls, and social media interaction with a child. Responses ranged from (1) not at all to (6) daily or more often. Structural solidarity with a child was measured by one item regarding geographical proximity. Responses ranged from (1) more than 500 miles from me to (6) less than 5 miles. Conflict with a child was measured by one item: “Taking everything into consideration, how much conflict, tension, or disagreement do you feel there is between you and your child at this point in your life?” Responses ranged from (1) not at all to (6) a great deal.
Control variables. We included controls for G3s’ age, gender (0 = male, 1 = female), race (0 = others, 1 = white), education (0 = did not finish high school, 6 = doctoral degree), marital status (0 = others, 1 = married or cohabiting), and annual income (1 = less than $20,000, 11 = $200,000 and more). G3s’ health status was measured by three binary items about the presence of a serious chronic health condition (cardiovascular diseases, cancer, diabetes, and Alzheimer’s disease). We converted these measures to a dichotomous variable (0 = no chronic condition, 1 = at least one chronic condition). We also included controls for children’s age, gender (0 = male, 1 = female), and biological/stepparent–child relationship (0 = step relationship, 1 = biological or adoptive relationship).

3.3. Analytic Strategy

To develop a typology of solidarity from its constituent dimensions, we used latent class analysis, a person-centered approach that identifies unobserved subgroups from combinations of observed indicators based on respondents’ reports [47]. We employed a BCH three-step latent class analysis, a bias-adjusted method that corrects classification errors [48]. Given that intergenerational solidarity measures were originally comprised of ordinally scaled items, we initially conducted a latent profile analysis. However, we were not able to identify the best-fitting model because optimal latent classes were not well-differentiated. Therefore, we dichotomized intergenerational solidarity indicators and employed latent class analysis. Based on a six-point scale, each solidarity indicator (except for the three noted below) was dichotomized into low (range from 1 to 3) and high (range from 4 to 6). Given that video call, social media interaction, and conflict were strongly negatively skewed, they were dichotomized using a different cut-off (low = 1 to 2; high = 3 to 6) (for more details, see Table 1).
In the first step, we employed a latent class analysis using the nine dichotomized intergenerational solidarity indicators. We selected the optimal number of latent classes using three criteria: the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC), the Consistent Akaike Information Criterion (CAIC), and entropy. The latent classes with the smallest BIC and CAIC, and entropy values over 0.8 were chosen as the best-fitting model [47]. In the second step, older adults were assigned a probability of intergenerational solidarity membership in each latent class. In the third step, analyses of associations between intergenerational solidarity class membership and covariates with mental health were conducted, with analyses weighted by latent class probability membership. We used full-information maximum likelihood estimation to account for missing values [49].

4. Results

4.1. Results of Descriptive Analysis

Results of descriptive analysis of older adults’ demographic characteristics and study variables are presented in Table 1. Most participants in the sample were white and married or cohabiting. Children’s mean age was 43 years and 52% were sons and 48% daughters. In terms of intergenerational solidarity with children, the measures for video calls, social media interaction, and conflict were skewed toward lower values and measures for affection, consensus and texting skewed toward higher values.

4.2. Results of Latent Class Analysis

Latent class analysis statistics and fit indices are displayed in Table 2. Two fit indices (BIC and CAIC) and entropy values indicated that a four-class model was the optimal fitting model. Item response and latent class probabilities of the four-class model are displayed in Figure 1. We identified the following types of intergenerational solidarity with adult children: (1) distant-but-digitally connected (affection, consensual, phone contact, texting, video call, and social media interaction were high but proximity, in-person contact, and conflict were low; 32.2%), (2) tight-knit-traditional (all item response probabilities were high except video call, social media interaction, and conflict; 29.3%), (3) detached (all item response probabilities except conflict were low; 25.1%), and (4) ambivalent (affection, in-person contact, phone contact, and conflict were high; 13.4%).

4.3. Associations between Intergenerational Solidarity Latent Classes and Mental Health

We employed multivariate regressions weighted by probabilities of latent class membership to determine how membership in different classes of intergenerational solidarity with adult children (reference group: detached) was associated with three measures of older parents’ mental health. Results are presented in Table 3. We found that older parents who had distant-but-digitally connected and tight-knit-traditional relationships with adult children reported fewer depressive symptoms and better psychological well-being and self-esteem, compared to those who had a detached relationship with adult children. Older parents who had an ambivalent relationship with adult children reported higher depressive symptoms than those who had a detached relationship with adult children. Additionally, we employed a multi-group contrast of mean values of mental health in four groups (Table 4). We found that older parents who had distant-but-digitally connected and tight-knit-traditional relationships with adult children reported fewer depressive symptoms and better psychological well-being and self-esteem, compared to those who had an ambivalent relationship with adult children. In addition, these three aspects of mental health were not significantly different between older parents in the distant-but-digitally connected and tight-knit-traditional latent classes.

5. Discussion

Our results suggest that digital communication (i.e., video call, instant messaging, and social media) enabled older and younger generations to maintain contact as a substitute for in-person visits during the COVID-19 pandemic. Historically, we know little about how the use of digital communication created new forms of intergenerational solidarity between generations, or whether these new forms were associated with mental health. In this study, we aimed to provide an updated intergenerational solidarity theoretical framework, including digital communication, and examined the association of this new framework with mental health of older parents. That our data were collected in 2021–2022 during the COVID-19 pandemic (albeit in its less acute period) provided the opportunity to examine these issues when in-person contact carried some risk and families were under stress.
The first hypothesis, that we would identify distinct latent classes of intergenerational solidarity with adult children among older parents, was confirmed in this sample. We identified four distinct intergenerational solidarity latent classes: distant-but-digitally connected, tight-knit-traditional, detached, and ambivalent. The identification of the distant-but-digitally connected latent class among older parents extends the intergenerational solidarity model by demonstrating how digital communication was integrated into other dimensions of the intergenerational solidarity model during the pandemic. As we expected, the main difference between distant-but-digitally connected and tight-knit-traditional latent classes were geographical proximity and frequency of in-person and digital contact between older parents and their adult children. We found that older parents who lived far away from their adult children had more video call and social media interaction with adult children during the pandemic compared to those who lived close to their adult children. Consequently, our finding can be interpreted that video call and social medial interaction with adult children replaced in-person contact with adult children because of geographic distance and social distancing during the pandemic. Interestingly, the frequency of texting was similar among older parents who were in the distant-but-digitally connected and tight-knit-traditional latent classes. Given that the use of smartphones has become more common across all generations, texting including instant messaging can be seen as a widely used digital communication between older parents and adult children regardless of physical distance and to some degree substitutes for voice-phone use.
We also identified an ambivalent latent class in older parents’ relationships with their oldest child. Parents in this class were also emotionally closer with their children than would be expected, a finding supporting previous studies that positive forms of solidarity and conflict can co-occur with conflict in intergenerational relationships—a condition often referred to as ambivalence [13,21]. We found that older parents in this class also reported low consensual solidarity with adult children. For this reason, we speculate that high conflict might stem from differences in values and opinions between older parents and adult children.
We found that older adults in distant-but-digitally connected and tight-knit-traditional latent classes reported better mental health during the pandemic compared to those who were in detached and ambivalent latent classes. This finding confirms our second hypothesis that older parents who perceived strong solidarity with and without a digital element (distant-but-digitally connected and tight-knit-traditional) with their adult children would report better psychological well-being compared to those who perceived weak solidarity (detached and ambivalent) with their adult children. As we mentioned, the main difference between the distant-but-digitally connected and tight-knit-traditional classes is the level of digital communication between older parents and adult children. However, older adults’ mental health was not significantly different between these two latent classes. Therefore, our finding provides some evidence that digital solidarity would be beneficial for older parents’ psychological well-being by compensating for low in-person contact.
Our finding should be carefully interpreted because of several limitations. First, the analysis did not consider functional solidarity with adult children (receiving support from adult children and providing support to adult children) because the LSOG measured exchange of support with all children rather than exchange of support with a specific child. Second, it is possible that digital solidarity could be influenced by both older parents’ and adult children’s socioeconomic status. For example, highly educated older parents and adult children are more likely to communicate digitally with each other than those who with lower education levels. Therefore, we recommend that future studies examine whether intergenerational digital communication can vary according to both older parents’ and adult children’s socioeconomic conditions using dyadic data analysis. Third, our analyses were based on cross-sectional data. Thus, we were not able to explore the relationship between intergenerational solidarity classes and older parents’ mental health over time to strengthen causal attribution of the results. Fourth, the original LSOG sample overrepresented white and middle-income families and underrepresented minority and low-income families. In addition, the LSOG began as a regional study in Southern California. Therefore, we recommend caution when generalizing our results.
Nevertheless, the present study is theoretically and methodologically innovative, advancing our understanding of how traditional and digital forms of solidarity with adult children are associated with their psychological well-being during a unique period when constraints imposed by pandemic highlighted the importance of alternative strategies older parents use to maintain intergenerational solidarity. The findings from this study reveal several applications of intergenerational solidarity for programmatic interventions, policy development, and clinical research. First, programs that foster intergenerational integration have relied on the intergenerational solidarity paradigm in their development and assessment. A recent review explicitly used the dimensions of intergenerational solidarity as a unifying framework to provide a meta-review of 31 programs in 11 countries designed to strengthen intergenerational solidarity [50]. Therefore, the integration of digital solidarity into the intergenerational solidarity paradigm will encourage researchers and practitioners to include digital communication when evaluating multidimensional solidarity between generations in the digital age.
Second, as a guiding principle for policy formation, intergenerational interdependence is often stressed as a means for achieving a humane society for older and younger people. Seedsman [51] writes that “the notion of intergenerational solidarity is held to be the social glue that offers the opportunity for families, communities, and society to work together to create a stable and supportive environment across all generations” (p. 215). Policymakers have keen interest in how families serve the needs of their older relatives through intergenerational family solidarity. Therefore, the intergenerational and digital solidarity paradigm would be informative for developing policies that promote healthy and active aging.
Third, regarding the COVID-19 pandemic, gerontologists and geriatricians are rightfully concerned about the consequent social isolation experienced by many older adults. Enhancing social and family connectivity through intergenerational digital engagement, as well as through more conventional means, is viewed as a highly promising intervention to reduce loneliness and activate support networks among older people [52]. When evaluating an intergenerational mentoring program, Lee and Kim [53] found that technology mentoring between older adults and youth increased e-health literacy and reduced social isolation among older participants. Based on our findings, we suggest that digital communication within families has the potential to strengthen the delivery of informal care by adult children as medical partners. For the above reasons and more, we suggest that greater attention be devoted to strengthening the ability of older adults to adapt to the challenges imposed by the COVID-19 pandemic in maintaining intergenerational ties, and, by extension, to the relentless pace of technological innovation which predated and will postdate this once-in-a-hundred-year health crisis.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, W.H. and M.S.; Formal analysis, W.H.; Funding acquisition, M.S.; Writing—original draft, W.H. and X.F.; Writing—review and editing, M.T.B. and M.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The project described was supported by awards #61457 from the John Templeton Foundation, R21AG064512 from the National Institute on Aging, and #2020203 from the Retirement Research Foundation.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Syracuse University (Approval number: 21-006).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Longitudinal Study of Generation dataset is available: https://www.icpsr.umich.edu/web/DSDR/studies/22100. The 2022 survey data is currently not available.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Cudjoe, T.K.M.; Kotwal, A.A. “Social distancing” Amid a crisis in social isolation and loneliness. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2020, 68, E27–E29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Luykx, J.J.; Vinkers, C.H.; Tijdink, J.K. Psychiatry in times of the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic: An imperative for psychiatrists to act now. JAMA Psychiatry 2020, 77, 1097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Arpino, B.; Pasqualini, M.; Bordone, V.; Solé-Auró, A. Older people’s nonphysical contacts and depression during the COVID-19 lockdown. Gerontologist 2020, 61, 176–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Killgore WD, S.; Cloonan, S.A.; Taylor, E.C.; Dailey, N.S. Loneliness: A signature mental health concern in the era of COVID-19. Psychiatry Res. 2020, 290, 113117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Kumar, A.; Nayar, K.R. COVID 19 and its mental health consequences. J. Ment. Health 2020, 30, 1–2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Dykstra, P.A. Cross-national differences in intergenerational family relations: The influence of public policy Arrangements. Innov. Aging 2018, 2, igx032. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Monin, J.K.; Ali, T.; Syed, S.; Piechota, A.; Lepore, M.; Mourgues, C.; Gaugler, J.E.; Marottoli, R.; David, D. Family communication in long-term care during a pandemic: Lessons for enhancing emotional experiences. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2020, 28, 1299–1307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Arpino, B.; Pasqualini, M.; Bordone, V. Physically distant but socially close? Changes in non-physical intergenerational contacts at the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic among older people in France, Italy and Spain. Eur. J. Ageing 2021, 18, 185–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Hu, Y.; Qian, Y. COVID-19, Inter-household contact and mental well-being among older adults in the US and the UK. Front. Sociol. 2021, 6, 714626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Sommerlad, A.; Marston, L.; Huntley, J.; Livingston, G.; Lewis, G.; Steptoe, A.; Fancourt, D. Social relationships and depression during the COVID-19 lockdown: Longitudinal analysis of the COVID-19 Social Study. Psychol. Med. 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Roberts, R.E.L.; Richards, L.N.; Bengtson, V. Intergenerational solidarity in families: Untangling the ties that bind. Marriage Fam. Rev. 1991, 16, 11–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Silverstein, M.; Bengtson, V.L. Intergenerational solidarity and the structure of adult child-parent relationships in American families. Am. J. Sociol. 1997, 103, 429–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Bengtson, V.; Giarrusso, R.; Mabry, J.B. Solidarity, conflict, and ambivalence: Complementary or competing perspectives on intergenerational relationships? J. Marriage Fam. 2002, 64, 568–576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Lüscher, K. Intergenerational ambivalence: Further steps in theory and research. J. Marriage Fam. 2002, 64, 585–593. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Hwang, W.; Kim, J.H.; Brown, M.T.; Silverstein, M. Are filial eldercare norms related to intergenerational solidarity with older parents? A typological developmental approach. J. Fam. Psychol. 2022, 36, 585–596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Nauck, B. Affection and conflict in intergenerational relationships of women in sixteen areas in Asia, Africa, Europe, and America. Comp. Popul. Stud. 2014, 39, 2014–2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Silverstein, M.; Gans, D.; Lowenstein, A.; Giarrusso, R.; Bengtson, V.L. Older parent–child relationships in six developed nations: Comparisons at the intersection of affection and conflict. J. Marriage Fam. 2010, 72, 1006–1021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. van Gaalen, R.I.; Dykstra, P.A. Solidarity and conflict between adult children and parents: A latent class analysis. J. Marriage Fam. 2006, 68, 947–960. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Peng, C.; Cheng, Q.; Yip, P.S.F. Does gender difference exist in typologies of intergenerational relations? Adult son-parent and daughter-parent relations in Hong Kong. J. Fam. Issues 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Guo, M.; Chi, I.; Silverstein, M. The structure of intergenerational relations in rural China: A latent class analysis. J. Marriage Fam. 2012, 74, 1114–1128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Steinbach, A. Intergenerational solidarity and ambivalence: Types of relationships in German families. J. Comp. Fam. Stud. 2008, 39, 115–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Peng, S.; Silverstein, M.; Suitor, J.J.; Gilligan, M.; Hwang, W.; Nam, S.; Routh, B. Use of communication technology to maintain intergenerational contact: Toward an understanding of “digital solidarity”. Connect. Fam. 2018, 159–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Pew Research Center. Social Media Use in 2021. Available online: https://www.pewresearch.org/internet/2021/04/07/social-media-use-in-2021/https://www.pewresearch.org/internet/2021/04/07/social-media-use-in-2021/ (accessed on 16 August 2022).
  24. Fingerman, K.L.; Huo, M.; Birditt, K.S. A decade of research on intergenerational ties: Technological, economic, political, and demographic changes. J. Marriage Fam. 2020, 82, 383–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Taipale, S. (Ed.) Intergenerational solidarity. In Intergenerational Connections in Digital Families; Springer International Publishing: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2019; pp. 103–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Older Adults. Available online: https://www.cdc.gov/aging/covid19/covid19-older-adults.html?CDC_AA_refVal=https%3A%2F%2Fwww.cdc.gov%2Fcoronavirus%2F2019-ncov%2Fneed-extra-precautions%2Folder-adults.html (accessed on 15 August 2022).
  27. Antonucci, T.C.; Ajrouch, K.J.; Manalel, J.A. Social relations and technology: Continuity, context, and change. Innov. Aging 2017, 1, igx029. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Peng, S.; Roth, A.R. Social isolation and loneliness before and during the COVID-19 pandemic: A longitudinal study of U.S. adults older than 50. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2022, 77, e185–e190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Lee, G.R.; Netzer, J.K.; Coward, R.T. Depression among older parents: The role of intergenerational exchange. J. Marriage Fam. 1995, 57, 823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Silverstein, M.; Cong, Z.; Li, S. Intergenerational transfers and living arrangements of older people in rural China: Consequences for psychological well-being. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2006, 61, S256–S266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Li, M.; Dong, X. The association between filial piety and depressive symptoms among U.S. Chinese older adults. Gerontol. Geriatr. Med. 2018, 4, 2333721418778167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Markides, K.S.; Krause, N. Intergenerational solidarity and psychological well-being among older Mexican Americans: A three-generations study. J. Gerontol. 1985, 40, 390–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Li, M.; Luo, Y.; Li, P. Intergenerational solidarity and life satisfaction among empty-nest older adults in rural China: Does distance matter? J. Fam. Issues 2021, 42, 626–649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Guo, M.; Stensland, M.; Li, M.; Dong, X. Parent-adult child relations of Chinese older immigrants in the United States: Is there an optimal type? J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2020, 75, 889–898. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Galea, S.; Merchant, R.M.; Lurie, N. The mental health consequences of COVID-19 and physical distancing: The need for prevention and early intervention. JAMA Intern. Med. 2020, 180, 817–818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  36. Yu, K.; Wu, S.; Chi, I. Internet use and loneliness of older adults over time: The mediating effect of social contact. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2021, 76, 541–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Zhang, K.; Kim, K.; Silverstein, N.M.; Song, Q.; Burr, J.A. Social media communication and loneliness among older adults: The mediating roles of social support and social contact. Gerontologist 2021, 61, 888–896. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Skałacka, K.; Pajestka, G. Digital or in-person: The relationship between mode of interpersonal communication during the COVID-19 pandemic and mental health in older adults from 27 countries. J. Fam. Nurs. 2021, 27, 275–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Fingerman, K.L.; Ng, Y.T.; Zhang, S.; Britt, K.; Colera, G.; Birditt, K.S.; Charles, S.T. Living Alone During COVID-19: Social Contact and Emotional Well-being Among Older Adults. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2021, 76, e116–e121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Litwin, H.; Levinsky, M. Social networks and mental health change in older adults after the Covid-19 outbreak. Aging Ment. Health 2022, 26, 925–931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Emery, T. Intergenerational transfers and European families: Does the number of siblings matter? Demogr. Res. 2013, 29, 247–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Leopold, T.; Raab, M.; Engelhardt, H. The transition to parent care: Costs, commitments, and caregiver selection among children. J. Marriage Fam. 2014, 76, 300–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Björgvinsson, T.; Kertz, S.J.; Bigda-Peyton, J.S.; McCoy, K.L.; Aderka, I.M. Psychometric properties of the CES-D-10 in a psychiatric sample. Assessment 2013, 20, 429–436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Radloff, L.S. The CES-D scale a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 1977, 1, 385–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Mechanic, D.; Bradburn, N.M. The Structure of Psychological Well-Being. Am. Sociol. Rev. 1970, 35, 948. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Rosenberg, M. Rosenberg self-esteem scale (RSE). Accept. Commit. Therapy. Meas. Package 1965, 61, 18. [Google Scholar]
  47. Nylund-Gibson, K.; Choi, A.Y. Ten frequently asked questions about latent class analysis. Transl. Issues Psychol. Sci. 2018, 4, 440–461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Bolck, A.; Croon, M.; Hagenaars, J. Estimating Latent Structure Models with Categorical Variables: One-Step Versus Three-Step Estimators. Politi-Anal. 2004, 12, 3–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Vermunt, J.K.; Magidson, J. Upgrade Manual for Latent GOLD 5.1; Statistical Innovations Inc.: Belmont, MA, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  50. Zlotnick, C.; van Groenou, M.B.; Orban, A.; Corrigan, T.; Coimbra, S.; Kirtava, Z.; Holdsworth, C.; Comas-D’Argemir, D.; Aliprant, L.; Gadet, C.; et al. Lessons learned from 11 countries on programs promoting intergenerational solidarity. Fam. Relat. 2021, 70, 670–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Seedsman, T. Building a humane society for older people: Compassionate policy making for integration, participation, and positive ageing within a framework of intergenerational solidarity. J. Intergenerational Relatsh. 2017, 15, 204–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Chatterjee, P.; Yatnatti, S.K. Intergenerational digital engagement: A way to prevent social isolation during the COVID-19 crisis. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2020, 68, 1394–1395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Lee, O.E.K.; Kim, D.H. Bridging the digital divide for older adults via intergenerational mentor-up. Res. Soc. Work Pract. 2019, 29, 786–795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Item response and latent class probabilities in four international solidarity with adult children classes.
Figure 1. Item response and latent class probabilities in four international solidarity with adult children classes.
Ijerph 19 12560 g001
Table 1. Results of Descriptive Analysis regarding Demographic and Study Variables.
Table 1. Results of Descriptive Analysis regarding Demographic and Study Variables.
Variables Older Adults (n = 519)
RangeM (SD)n (%)
Demographic Variables
     Age 69.07 (3.78)
     Gender
          Male 219 (42.2)
          Female 297 (57.2)
     Race
          White 497 (95.8)
          Other ethnic groups 22 (4.2)
     Education1–63.58 (1.28)
     Marital status
          Married or cohabitating 410 (79.0)
          Others 108 (20.8)
     Annual income1–115.68 (3.03)
     Health status
          Healthy 400 (77.1)
          Unhealthy 119 (22.9)
     Children’s age 43.27 (7.22)
     Children’s gender
          Son 270 (52.0)
          Daughter 247 (47.6)
     Parent–child relations
          Biological or adoptive children 442 (85.2)
          Stepchildren 76 (14.6)
     Number of children 2.75 (1.47)
Intergenerational Solidarity
     Affectual solidarity1–64.26 (1.48)
          Low group (range 1–3) 134 (25.8)
          High group (range 4–6) 385 (74.2)
     Consensual solidarity1–63.97 (1.31)
          Low group (range 1–3) 169 (32.6)
          High group (range 4–6) 341 (65.7)
     Associational solidarity: In-person contact1–63.32 (1.38)
          Low group (range 1–3) 289 (55.7)
          High group (range 4–6) 228 (43.9)
     Associational solidarity: Phone contact1–63.91 (1.41)
          Low group (range 1–3) 173 (33.3)
          High group (range 4–6) 344 (66.3)
     Digital solidarity: Texting1–63.88 (1.66)
          Low group (range 1–3) 194 (37.4)
          High group (range 4–6) 324 (62.4)
     Digital solidarity: Video call1–62.05 (1.56) a
          Low group (range 1–2) 361 (69.6)
          High group (range 3–6) 155 (29.9)
Intergenerational Solidarity
     Digital solidarity: Social media1–61.96 (1.34) a
          Low group (range 1–2) 363 (69.9)
          High group (range 3–6) 152 (29.3)
     Structural solidarity1–63.29 (1.94)
          Low group (range 1–3) 251 (48.4)
          High group (range 4–6) 267 (51.4)
     Conflict1–62.02 (1.30) a
          Low group (range 1–2) 388 (74.8)
          High group (range 3–6) 128 (24.7)
Mental Health
     Depressive symptoms1–41.59 (0.44)
     Psychological well-being0–107.46 (1.98)
     Self-esteem1–43.43 (0.46)
Note: a = the absolute value of skewness exceeds 1.
Table 2. Latent Class Analysis Statistics and Fit Indices.
Table 2. Latent Class Analysis Statistics and Fit Indices.
ModelClasses (n)BICCAICEntropy
Model 115902.045911.04-
Model 225386.615405.610.80
Model 335255.245284.240.81
Model 445236.765275.760.80
Model 555249.595298.590.83
Model 665276.175335.170.82
Note: Bolded values indicate best fit for each respective statistic. BIC = Bayesian Information Criterion. CAIC = Consistent Akaike Information Criterion.
Table 3. Associations of Class Memberships with Mental Health.
Table 3. Associations of Class Memberships with Mental Health.
VariablesOlder Adults (n = 519)
Depressive SymptomsPsychological Well-BeingSelf-Esteem
b (SE)b (SE)b (SE)
Class Memberships (ref: Detached)
     Distant-but-digitally connected−0.12 (0.06) 0.65 (0.28) *0.26 (0.06) ***
     Tight-knit-traditional−0.11 (0.06) 0.73 (0.27) **0.24 (0.06) ***
     Ambivalent0.17 (0.08) *−0.59 (0.36)0.04 (0.08)
Covariates
     Age−0.00 (0.05)0.02 (0.02)−0.00 (0.00)
     Female (ref: male)0.02 (0.03)−0.22 (0.17)−0.06 (0.04)
     White (ref: others)−0.02 (0.08)−0.93 (0.33) **−0.09 (0.07)
     Education−0.01 (0.01)0.01 (0.07)0.00 (0.01)
     Married/cohabiting (ref: others)−0.16 (0.05) **0.59 (0.23) *0.11 (0.05) *
     Annual income−0.01 (0.00)0.04 (0.03)0.01 (0.00) *
     Healthy (ref: unhealthy)0.08 (0.04)−0.06 (0.20)−0.04 (0.04)
     Children’s age0.00 (0.00)−0.00 (0.01)−0.00 (0.00)
     Daughters (ref: sons)−0.00 (0.03)−0.14 (0.17)0.00 (0.03)
     Biological/adoptive children (ref: stepchildren)0.14 (0.05) *−0.77 (0.26) **−0.15 (0.06) *
Note: ref. = reference group. p < 0.10. * p < 0.05. ** p < 0.01. *** p < 0.001.
Table 4. Results of Paired Comparison Test of Mental Health across Four Intergenerational Solidarity Latent Classes.
Table 4. Results of Paired Comparison Test of Mental Health across Four Intergenerational Solidarity Latent Classes.
Class 1
Distant-But-Digitally Connected
Class 2
Tight-Knit-Traditional
Class 3
Detached
Class 4
Ambivalent
Mental healthM (SE)M (SD)M (SD)M (SD)p-value
Depressive symptoms1.52 (0.03)1.53 (0.03)1.64 (0.05)1.82 (0.06)4 > 1 & 2 ***
4 > 3 *
3 > 1 & 2
Psychological well-being7.77 (0.15)7.85 (0.15)7.12 (0.22)6.52 (0.28)1 & 2 > 4 ***
1 > 3 *
2 > 3 **
Self-esteem3.54 (0.03)3.52 (0.03)3.27 (0.05)3.32 (0.06)1 & 2 > 3 ***
1 & 2 > 4 **
Note: Covariates were controlled but not presented in Table. p < 0.10. * p < 0.05. ** p < 0.01. *** p < 0.001.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Hwang, W.; Fu, X.; Brown, M.T.; Silverstein, M. Digital and Non-Digital Solidarity between Older Parents and Their Middle-Aged Children: Associations with Mental Health during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 12560. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191912560

AMA Style

Hwang W, Fu X, Brown MT, Silverstein M. Digital and Non-Digital Solidarity between Older Parents and Their Middle-Aged Children: Associations with Mental Health during the COVID-19 Pandemic. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(19):12560. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191912560

Chicago/Turabian Style

Hwang, Woosang, Xiaoyu Fu, Maria Teresa Brown, and Merril Silverstein. 2022. "Digital and Non-Digital Solidarity between Older Parents and Their Middle-Aged Children: Associations with Mental Health during the COVID-19 Pandemic" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 19: 12560. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191912560

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop