Next Article in Journal
Influence of Fitness Apps on Sports Habits, Satisfaction, and Intentions to Stay in Fitness Center Users: An Experimental Study
Previous Article in Journal
Estimation of Health-Related Physical Fitness (HRPF) Levels of the General Public Using Artificial Neural Network with the National Fitness Award (NFA) Datasets
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Association of Sexual Attitudes with Sexual Function: General vs. Specific Attitudes

by
Juan Carlos Sierra
*,
Jennifer Gómez-Carranza
,
Ana Álvarez-Muelas
and
Oscar Cervilla
Mind, Brain, and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC), University of Granada, 18011 Granada, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18(19), 10390; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph181910390
Submission received: 10 August 2021 / Revised: 27 September 2021 / Accepted: 29 September 2021 / Published: 2 October 2021

Abstract

:
Background: Sexual attitudes are related to the expression of sexuality and have been associated with indicators for sexual health. The main aim of this study was to determine the explanatory capacity of general (i.e., erotophilia) and specific (i.e., toward sexual fantasies and masturbation) sexual attitudes on different sexual functioning dimensions (sexual desire, sexual arousal, lubrication/erection, ability to have an orgasm and orgasm satisfaction). Methods: The sample consisted of 2000 heterosexual adults (1044 women, 956 men) aged 18–83 years. Results: The explanatory models for women mainly showed that positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies (β range = −0.35, −0.249) and age (β range = −0.111, 0.086) explained sexual function. The models proposed for men revealed a more diverse pattern, although the variable essential for explaining sexual function was a positive attitude toward sexual fantasies (β range = −0.266, −0.097). Conclusions: These results indicate that specific sexual attitudes, particularly in relation to sexual fantasies, are more sensitive variables than erotophilia in examining sexual health.

1. Introduction

Sexual attitudes are beliefs with a heavy emotional weight that cause subjects to favorably or unfavorably respond to sexual stimuli. Sexual attitudes can be displayed towards sexuality in general (e.g., erotophilia) or certain sexual conducts (e.g., attitude toward sexual fantasies or masturbation). Both types of sexual attitudes significantly determine the way in which people live and express their sexuality, and are associated with different indicators of sexual health, such as skills to prevent sexually transmitted diseases [1], sexual victimization [2] or sexual function [3].
For general attitudes toward sexuality, erotophilia refers to the evaluation of sexual stimuli and is more widely considered and studied as part of the erotophilia–erotophobia continuum. If people come close to the erotophilic extreme, they have positive emotional reactions to sexual stimuli, which are favorably evaluated. Conversely, if people move toward the erotophobic extreme, they feel dissatisfied emotions about sexual stimuli, negatively evaluate them, and so they avoid them [4]. This construct is a traditional research objective in human sexuality research [5]. Erotophilia is related to sexual self-esteem [6], general sexual function [7,8,9,10], sexual desire [11,12,13,14], subjective and objective sexual arousal [15,16], propensity for sexual excitation [17,18], subjective orgasm experience [19], sexual satisfaction [20], sexual fantasies [21], sexual dreams [22], and sexual assertiveness [9,10,23,24], among other constructs.
One of the most well-studied specific sexual attitudes is the positive attitude toward sexual fantasies. Sexual fantasies are sexual images or thoughts which influence a person’s emotions and/or physiological status. They may occur while practicing sex with a partner or during auto-erotic practices. Most people experience them, and they are generally pleasurable and enriching for sexuality [25]. A positive attitude toward sexual fantasies is related to good sexual function [8,26], sexual satisfaction [27], sexual fantasies [28,29], frequency of sexual fantasies and sexual desire [14,30], positive sexual thoughts [31], sexual assertiveness [23,26], a positive attitude toward masturbation [32], intensity of the subjective orgasm experience in the masturbation context [33] and sexual guilt [34,35].
Another attitude toward certain sexual conduct, and one which is interesting for studying sexual health, is the attitude toward masturbation. Masturbation is traditionally considered an unacceptable conduct in some domains (e.g., the Jewish–Christian religion). Consequently, we still note the impact of this tradition as it is still stigmatized today [36,37]. Indeed, a negative attitude toward masturbation is associated with sexual guilt [34,35,38], less frequent orgasms while practicing masturbation [39], less subjective sexual arousal [40], less frequent masturbation [32,41], less solitary sexual desire and a worse sexual function [32].
In the clinical context (e.g., sex therapy), it is usual to assess and listen to the sexual attitudes of patients with sexual dysfunctions. However, it is unclear whether it is more relevant to address general sexual attitudes (e.g., erotophilia) or specific sexual attitudes (e.g., toward sexual fantasies or masturbation). Very few studies have jointly dealt with the association between a general attitude toward sexuality with certain specific attitudes toward given sexual conducts and other sexuality dimensions. One exception is the study by Santos-Iglesias et al. [23], which examined the role of sexual desire, arousal, sexual attitudes and the abuse suffered in a relationship to explain sexual assertiveness. Their results indicated that having an attitude toward a specific sexual conduct (sexual fantasies in their case) was more powerful than erotophilia for predicting sexual assertiveness. Sierra et al. [26] recently reported that a positive attitude toward sexual fantasies, unlike erotophilia, is capable of explaining sexual function in both men and women. More recently, Sierra et al. [2] reported that attitudes toward sexual fantasies, unlike erotophilia, distinguished women who had suffered physical and non-physical abuse by a partner from those who had not endured such experiences.
In short, general and specific sexual attitudes have normally been covered separately in the scientific literature when studying their relation to other sexuality dimensions, and no studies have examined how both types of attitudes act in relation to constructs like the different sexual function components. This is a relevant matter in clinical sexology because patients’ sexual attitudes tend to be contemplated in sexual therapy to make them positive for the patients [42,43]. Therefore, the present study was conducted to extend knowledge about sexual attitudes in the sexual function context. The main objective was to determine the association that erotophilia, positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies and negative attitudes toward masturbation had with different sexual function components: desire, arousal, erection/vaginal lubrication, ability to have an orgasm and orgasm satisfaction. Due to gender differences evidenced in both sexual attitudes [9,26,30] and sexual functions [7], this association was examined in men and women separately. We hypothesized that a general attitude toward sexuality (erotophilia), and specific attitudes toward sexual fantasies and masturbation, would have a different relevance in explaining sexual function, with more importance attached to attitudes toward specific sexual conducts (sexual fantasies and masturbation), as studies into other constructs have demonstrated [2,23,26].

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The sample consisted of 2000 heterosexual Spanish adults (1044 women, 956 men) aged between 18 and 83 years (M = 40.12; SD = 12.48). We determined this sample size based on a 97% confidence level and a 3% error estimation. Incidental sampling by quotas according to age was performed: 18–34 (n = 699), 35–49 (n = 670) and 50 years and older (n = 631). The inclusion criteria were: (a) aged 18 years or older; (b) Spanish nationality; (c) heterosexual orientation; (d) current sexual activity with another person of the opposite sex. Table 1 presents the sample’s socio-demographic characteristics.

2.2. Instruments

The socio-demographic and sexual history questionnaire collected data about sex, age, nationality, sexual orientation, level of education, sexual activity, number of sexual partners, partner relationships and religiosity.
The Spanish version of Sexual Opinion Survey-6 (SOS-6) [9] evaluated erotophilia with six items (e.g., I personally find that thinking about engaging in sexual intercourse is arousing) answered on a 7-Likert scale from 1 (totally disagree) to 7 (totally agree). Higher scores indicated more erotophilia. These scores indicated adequate evidence for internal consistency reliably (α = 0.74) and suitable evidence for validity based on the relation to other variables (sexual satisfaction, sexual desire, sexual function, sexual assertiveness and positive attitude toward sexual fantasies; [9,15]). In this study sample, the ordinal alpha values were 0.81 in women and 0.85 in men.
The Spanish version of Hurlbert Index of Sexual Fantasy (HISF) [26] had 10 items (e.g., I think sexual fantasies are healthy) which evaluated the extent of positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies on a Likert scale ranging from 0 (never) to 4 (all the time). Higher scores indicated a more positive attitude toward sexual fantasies. Its internal consistency reliably was 0.94 and it presented adequate evidence for validity with other similar measures. In this study, the ordinal alpha values were 0.88 in women and 0.87 in men.
The Spanish version of Negative Attitudes Toward Masturbation Inventory (NATMI) [32]. It was made up of 10 items (e.g., Masturbation in an adult is juvenile and immature) answered on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 (not at all true from me) to 5 (extremely true for me). Higher scores indicated a more negative attitude toward masturbation. Its internal consistency reliably was high (ordinal alpha of 0.95), and it presented adequate evidence for discriminant and convergent validity. In this sample, the ordinal alpha value was 0.95 for both women and men.
The Spanish version of the Arizona Sexual Experience Scale (ASEX) [44] was validated in the Spanish population by Sánchez-Fuentes et al. [7]. It comprised of six items answered on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (good functioning) to 6 (bad functioning). It assessed sexual function in the last 7 days during sexual intercourse, in terms of desire, arousal, erection (in men)/vaginal lubrication (in women), ability of having an orgasm and orgasm satisfaction. Higher scores indicated a worse sexual function. It presents suitable psychometric properties, with a Cronbach´s alpha value of 0.79 in women and 0.80 in men, and adequate evidence for this validity. In this study sample, the ordinal alpha values were 0.83 in women and 0.81 in men.

2.3. Procedure

Data collection was completed with an online survey on Facebook® (Facebook, Inc, California, United States) from January to March 2020 using the LimeSurvey® software (SourceForge.net, Hamburgm, Germany). This method was normally used in research works about sexual behaviors. There were no differences between the data obtained by this system and those acquired by the traditional paper and pencil methods [45,46]. The IP address was controlled, as were automatic responses to avoid automated responses. The participants accessed the survey by answering a security question consisting of a random arithmetic question. All the responses were examined in detail to control for anomalous patterns. Anonymity and confidentiality were guaranteed. Participation was voluntary and the participants filled in an informed consent form that described the type of study, and provided information on data privacy and confidentiality. The study was previously approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the University of Granada.

2.4. Data Analysis

The analyses were performed using the R® environment (version 3.6.3) (The R Fundation, Vienna, Austria) [47] with its RStudio® interface (version 1.2.5042) (RStudio PBC, Vienna, Austria) [48]. First, to process any missing data, an algorithm for non-parametric distributions was applied to create a random forest model for each variable by means of the other variables in the database. The missing values were input using the missForest package (versión 1.4) [49]. A MANCOVA was performed to examine the effect of sex on sexual attitudes (erotophilia, positive attitude toward sexual fantasies and negative attitude toward masturbation) and sexual function dimensions (desire, arousal, ability to have an orgasm and orgasm satisfaction), with age, level of education, having a partner, frequency of attending religious events and frequency of praying in private as covariables. Given the found differences per sex, the following analyses were carried out for the sample of women and men separately. The explanatory capacity of age (given its relation to sexual function), as well as sexual attitudes on each sexual function component, were examined by multiple linear regression using the enter method.

3. Results

3.1. Sex-Differences in Sexual Attitudes and Sexual Function

Age (Wilk’s lambda = 0.98; F(7, 1827) = 5.46, p < 0.001; ƞ2 = 0.02), level of education (Wilk’s lambda = 0.98; F(7, 1827) = 5.36, p < 0.001; ƞ2 = 0.02), having a partner (Wilk’s lambda = 0.99; F(7, 1827) = 2.91, p < 0.01; ƞ2 = 0.011), frequency of attending religious events (Wilk’s lambda = 0.97; F(7, 1827) = 9.19, p < 0.001; ƞ2 = 0.034) and frequency of praying in private (Wilk’s lambda = 0.98; F(7, 1827) = 4.46, p < 0.001; ƞ2 = 0.017) were significant multivariate covariates. Sex had a major effect on sexual attitudes and sexual function (Wilk’s lambda = 0.88; F(7, 1827) = 35.0, p < 0.001; ƞ2 = 0.118). The intersubject effect on sexual attitudes and sexual function was shown in Table 2. The significant differences per sex were found in specific sexual attitudes, but not in erotophilia, and also appeared in the sexual function dimensions, except in orgasm satisfaction. The effect sizes were medium, except in negative attitude toward masturbation, where they were small. Women showed a more positive attitude toward sexual fantasies than men, whereas men reported a more negative attitude toward masturbation than women. Mens’ scores for the sexual function dimensions indicated a better sexual function in general.

3.2. Regression Models

For women, significant models were obtained for desire (F (4, 1039) = 45.84; p < 0.001), arousal (F (4, 1039) = 39.98; p < 0.001), vaginal lubrication (F (4, 1039) = 24.62; p < 0.001), ability to have an orgasm (F (4, 1039) = 2.79; p < 0.001) and orgasm satisfaction (F (4, 1039) = 25.14; p < 0.001). In the first model, age (β = 0.086) and positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies (β = −0.350) explained 15% of the variance for sexual desire. The variables in the second model were age (β = 0.077) and positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies (β = −0.340), which accounted for 13% of the variance for sexual arousal. In the third model, positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies (β = −0.269) was the only variable, and it accounted for 9% of the variance for vaginal lubrication. In the fourth model, once again age (β = −0.111) and positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies (β = −0.249) explained 8% of the variance for the ability to have an orgasm. Finally, in the last model, positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies (β = −0.284) and negative attitudes toward masturbation (β = 0.067) accounted for 9% of the variance for orgasm satisfaction.
In men, significant models were also obtained for desire (F (4, 951) = 24.75; p < 0.001), arousal (F (4, 951) = 26.49; p < 0.001), erection (F (4, 951) = 36.15; p < 0.001), ability to have an orgasm (F (4, 951) = 18.24; p < 0.001) and orgasm satisfaction (F (4, 951) = 20.47; p < 0.001). In the first two models, only positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies explained 9% of the variance for sexual desire (β = −0.245) and 10% for sexual arousal (β = −0.266). In the third model, the variables that explained 13% of the variance for erection were age (β = 0.298), erotophilia (β = −0.081), positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies (β = −0.097) and negative attitudes toward masturbation (β = 0.093). In the fourth model, the variables that accounted for 7% of the variance for the ability to have an orgasm were erotophilia (β = −0.133) and positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies (β = −0.144). In the last model, both positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies (β = −0.184) and negative attitudes toward masturbation (β = −0.117) explained 8% of the variance for orgasm satisfaction. The variance inflation factors (VIF) values showed no multicollinearity problems in any of the models for the sexual function dimensions of both men and women (see Table 3 and Table 4).

4. Discussion

As far as we are aware, the scientific literature contains no studies that jointly examine the role of general and specific sexual attitudes to explain sexual function. Several research works focused instead on the relation between some attitudes and certain sexual response dimensions, such as desire [12] or orgasm [19], and very few have jointly examined the association of the two types of attitudes (general vs. specific) with other sexual health-related conducts [2,23,26]. For this reason, the objective of the present study was to analyze the capacity of both types of attitudes to explain each sexual function component in women and men. To do so, the general and specific sexual attitudes, with better evidence of their relation to sexual health, were considered: erotophilia, such as a general and positive attitude toward sexual fantasies, and a negative attitude toward masturbation as a specific attitude.
First, the differences between men and women in the study object variables were examined. Congruent differences were generally found compared to previous research. Women reported a worse sexual function than men for all the sexual response components, which agrees with other studies that applied similar measures to populations such as our study population [7,8,50], with the exception of orgasm satisfaction. Similar to other studies, women took a less positive attitude toward sexual fantasies [8,26,28] and a less negative attitude toward masturbation [36,51] than men. Several studies stress the importance of differences per sex in analyzing attitudes and sexual conducts, and often show that men are more prone to contemplate positive aspects of sexuality, such as taking a more positive attitude toward sexual fantasies [30] or better sexual function [8,50]. Women tended to employ masturbation as a strategy to relax or cope with stress, as well as a source of pleasure, rather than a conduct to compensate for not having a partner or as a substitution for sexual intercourse, which was the case for men [37]. This finding might explain why women take a less negative attitude toward masturbation than men.
It is worth pointing out the lack of significant differences between men and women regarding erotophilia, which tends to be traditionally expressed in the Spanish population [9,23,52,53]. The fact that both men and women take a similar erotophilic attitude could reflect a tendency toward egalitarian views for both sexes regarding sexuality, but could also be the result of using an invariate measure per sex like that herein employed and, unlike other previous works to assess erotophilia, which could eliminate this measure’s possible biases [15]. Finding no differences per sex in orgasm satisfaction coincides with other studies, which found no differences in sexual satisfaction between men and women [27,54]. The fact that significant differences appeared in the ability to have an orgasm, but not in orgasm satisfaction, seems to indicate that orgasmic capacity and sexual satisfaction do not always go hand in hand in women, as previously evidenced in the elderly [8].
For the association of both types (general and specific) of sexual attitudes with different sexual function components (desire, arousal, erection/vaginal lubrication, ability to have an orgasm, orgasm satisfaction), we found that sexual function for both men and women was basically explained by taking a positive attitude toward sexual fantasies and was significant for all its dimensions. In other words, taking a positive sexual attitude toward sexual fantasies was associated with more sexual desire and arousal, better vaginal lubrication/erection capacity, the ability to have an orgasm, and more orgasm satisfaction. These results fall in line with those reported by Sierra et al. [26]. The orgasm satisfaction explanation is also related to a negative attitude toward masturbation, but less significantly than attitude toward sexual fantasies insofar as negative attitude toward masturbation is associated with less orgasm satisfaction. The recent study by Cervilla et al. [32] indicated that those people with a negative attitude toward masturbation reported less orgasm satisfaction in sexual relations than those with a more positive attitude. These results indicate a differential effect of both specific sexual attitudes about sexual response components. The preferred attitudes toward fantasies favored all the sexual function dimensions of both men and women, but the attitudes toward masturbation only did so regarding orgasm satisfaction for both sexes, and for erections for men. The fact that men attached more importance to attitudes toward masturbation than women could be associated with men taking a more negative attitude toward this conduct, compared to women [32]. These results support the relevance of sexual fantasies as a central sexual function component [25]. The DSM-5 [55] considers that having few or no erotic thoughts or fantasies is one of the diagnostic criteria for not only feminine sexual interest/excitation disorder, but also for hypoactive masculine sexual desire.
As for taking a general attitude toward erotophilia, we observed that while women had no explanatory capacity for sexual function components, men played a secondary role in accounting for erection capacity and the ability to have an orgasm; that is, men with an erotophilic attitude would be associated with a better erection and orgasm capacity. These results supported the proposed hypothesis and, based on previous studies [2,23,26] of erotophilia and attitudes toward sexual fantasies and masturbation, took different weights to explain sexual function, with more importance attached to specific attitudes. These findings evidenced that specific sexual attitudes (particularly positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies) could be more sensitive variables compared to having a general attitude toward sexuality (i.e., erotophilia) to examine sexual health.
Age is seen as a predictor variable because its relation to sexual function is well-known [50,56]. In light of the results herein obtained, the association between age and sexual function dimensions were not presented consistently. Age can show an explanatory capacity for the desire and arousal capacities in women, and for the erection capacity in men. Sexual function becomes more difficult with age [50,57,58], which most likely has something to do with its effect on the physiology of sexual response, especially the lack of hormones which affect women’s sexual desire and men’s erection capacity.
It should be noted that the study sample was composed only of people whose sexual relations were exclusively heterosexual. This fact does not reflect the sexual reality and responds uniquely to the methodological reasons related to the instruments used in the evaluation of sexual attitudes and sexual function, which are validated in the Spanish heterosexual population. Future studies should be interested in their validation in other populations and in examining the invariance of their measures, to be able to integrate sexual diversity into the research. We are aware that sexuality is more diverse; it should not be considered exclusively as sexual intercourse between persons of two different sexes.
Despite the sample being large, this study presents a series of limitations that must be taken into account when interpreting its results. First, patients were selected by non-probabilistic sampling using an online survey only, which meant that users had to have a social network. Second, the sample was formed by people mostly with a university education. Therefore, these limitations must be taken into account to generalize the results. On the other hand, the aim of this study was to explore sexual function in the context of heterosexual intercourse, but it should be emphasized that sexuality is broader and should not be simplified as such.

5. Conclusions

By way of conclusion, specific sexual attitudes, especially positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies, are more relevant than a general attitude toward sexuality (erotophilia) to explain sexual function. These results could contribute to extend the knowledge regarding the effect of attitudes on sexual function, and in both the clinical domain and research. First, different models were obtained for both men and women to explain all the sexual function dimensions using different types of sexual attitudes. Second, assessing not only sexual function, but also the variables associated with it, is essential in clinical practice. Thus, professionals must evaluate those attitudes that may play a relevant role in sexual problems, because this is a central part of the interventions in both sexual education and changes in sexual attitudes [42,43,59,60,61]. This means that therapies must focus on sexual function-related elements, particularly on sexual attitudes. Those patients with sexual dysfunctions who seek therapy tend to take more negative attitudes toward different aspects of sexuality. Therefore, they must be dealt with by focusing on the most relevant attitudes in light of the results obtained herein. The findings of this study are also interesting for prevention programs. For example, interventions in negative attitudes toward masturbation are included in sexual education programs to improve different sexual health aspects [62,63]. Therefore, sexual education can promote positive sexual attitudes to do away with, for example, gender roles [45,64] or discrimination against sexual minorities [65,66] which limit sexual health. Therefore, these findings can help effective therapies and programs to be developed that promote, in particular, sexual wellbeing and quality of life in general.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.C.S.; methodology, J.C.S., A.Á.-M. and O.C.; formal analysis, J.G.-C., A.Á.-M. and O.C.; investigation, J.C.S., J.G.-C., A.Á.-M. and O.C.; writing—original draft preparation, J.C.S., J.G.-C., A.Á.-M. and O.C.; writing—review and editing, J.C.S., J.G-C., A.Á.-M. and O.C.; and funding acquisition, J.C.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Bursaries FPU 16/04429 and FPU18/03102 for University Professor Training as part of the A.Á-M’s thesis and the O.C.´s thesis, respectively (Psychological Doctoral Program B13 56 1; RD 99/2011).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical review and approval were waived for this study because it was not a clinical study.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Xu, H.; Xie, J.; Xiao, Z.; Xiao, H.; Li, X.; Goldsamt, L.; Williams, A.B.; Wang, H. Sexual attitudes, sexual behaviors, and use of HIV prevention services among male undergraduate students in Hunan, China: A cross-sectional survey. BMC Public Health 2019, 19, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Sierra, J.C.; Arcos-Romero, A.I.; Álvarez-Muelas, A.; Cervilla, O. The impact of intimate partner violence on sexual attitudes, sexual assertiveness, and sexual functioning in men and women. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Shayan, A.; Khodabandeloo, R.; Solatani, F.; Esfandeh, K.; Masoumi, S.Z.; Mohammadi, Y.; Dargahi, S.; Esmaili, M. The relationship between attitudes and sexual function in pregnant women referred to health centers-medical city of Hamadan. Pajouhan Sci. J. 2020, 18, 20–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Fisher, W.A.; White, L.A.; Byrne, D.; Kelley, K. Erotophobia-erotophilia as a dimension of personality. J. Sex. Res. 1988, 25, 123–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Rye, B.J.; Fisher, W.A. Sexual Opinion Survey. In Handbook of Sexuality-Related Measures, 4th ed.; Milhausen, R.R., Sakaluk, J.K., Fisher, T.D., Davis, C.M., Yarber, W.L., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2020; pp. 570–572. [Google Scholar]
  6. Soler, F.; Gómez Lugo, M.; Espada, J.P.; Morales, A.; Sierra, J.C.; Marchal Bertrand, L.; Vallejo Medina, P. Adaptation and validation of the Brief Sexuality Scale in Colombian and Spanish populations. Int. J. Psychol. Psychol. Ther. 2016, 16, 343–356. [Google Scholar]
  7. Sánchez-Fuentes, M.M.; Moyano, N.; Granados, R.; Sierra, J.C. Validation of the Spanish Version of the Arizona Sexual Experience Scale (ASEX) using self-reported and psychophysiological measures. Rev. Iberoam. Psicol. Salud 2019, 10, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Sierra, J.C.; Vallejo-Medina, P.; Santos-Iglesias, P.; Moyano, N.; Granados, M.R.; Sánchez-Fuentes, M.M. Funcionamiento sexual en personas mayores: Influencia de la edad y de factores psicosexuales. Rev. Int. Androl. 2014, 12, 64–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Vallejo-Medina, P.; Granados, M.R.; Sierra, J.C. Propuesta y validación de una versión breve del Sexual Opinion Survey en población española. Rev. Int. Androl. 2014, 12, 47–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Vallejo-Medina, P.; Marchal-Bertrand, L.; Gómez-Lugo, M.; Espada, J.P.; Sierra, J.C.; Soler, F.; Morales, A. Adaptation and validation of the Brief Sexual Opinion Survey (SOS) in a Colombian sample and factorial equivalence with the Spanish version. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, 0162531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Ortega, V.; Zubeidat, I.; Sierra, J.C. Further examination of measurement properties of Spanish of the Sexual Desire Inventory with undergraduates and adolescent students. Psychol. Rep. 2006, 99, 147–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Santos-Iglesias, P.; Calvillo, G.; Sierra, J.C. A further examination of Levine’s model of sexual desire. Psychol. Sex. 2013, 4, 34–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Sierra, J.C.; Zubeidat, I.; Carretero-Dios, H.; Reina, S. Estudio psicométrico preliminar del Test del Deseo Sexual Inhibido en una muestra española no clínica. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 2003, 3, 489–504. [Google Scholar]
  14. Zubeidat, I.; Ortega, V.; Sierra, J.C. Evaluación de algunos factores determinantes del deseo sexual: Estado emocional, actitudes sexuales y fantasías sexuales. Anal. Modif. Conducta 2004, 30, 105–128. [Google Scholar]
  15. Arcos-Romero, A.I.; Calvillo, C.; Granados, R.; Álvarez-Muelas, A.; Sierra, J.C. The Spanish version of the Sexual Opinion Survey (SOS-6): Evidence of validity of a short version. Int. J. Psychol. Res. 2020, 13, 40–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Sierra, J.C.; Arcos-Romero, A.I.; Granados, M.R.; Sánchez-Fuentes, M.M.; Calvillo, C.; Moyano, N. Escalas de Valoración de Excitación Sexual y Valoración de Sensaciones Genitales: Propiedades psicométricas en muestras españolas. Rev. Int Androl. 2017, 15, 99–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Granados, M.R.; Salinas, J.M.; Sierra, J.C. Spanish version of the Sexual Excitation/Sexual Inhibition for Women: Factorial structure, reliability and validity evidences. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 2017, 17, 65–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Granados, M.R.; Salinas, J.M.; Sierra, J.C. Psychometric properties of the Spanish version of the Sexual Inhibition/Sexual Excitation Scales for men. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2018, 47, 783–796. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Arcos-Romero, A.I.; Sierra, J.C. Factors associated with subjective orgasm experience in heterosexual relationships. J. Sex. Marital. Ther. 2020, 46, 314–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Sánchez-Fuentes, M.M.; Salinas, J.M.; Sierra, J.C. Use of an ecological model to study sexual satisfaction in a heterosexual Spanish sample. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2016, 45, 1973–1988. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Viedma, M.I.; Gutiérrez, P.; Ortega, V.; Sierra, J.C. Escala de Ensoñaciones Sexuales: Primeros datos psicométricos en muestras españolas. Anal. Modif. Conducta 2005, 31, 407–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Hmidan, A.; Weaver, A.D. Sex dreams: Gender, erotophilia, and sociosexuality as predictors of content, valence, and frequency. Can. J. Hum. Sex. 2019, 28, 177–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Santos-Iglesias, P.; Sierra, J.C.; Vallejo-Medina, P. Predictors of sexual assertiveness: The role of sexual desire, arousal, attitudes and partner abuse. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2013, 42, 1043–1052. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Sierra, J.C.; Santos, P.; Gutiérrez-Quintanilla, J.R.; Gómez, P.; Maeso, M.D. Un estudio psicométrico del Hurlbert Index of Sexual Assertiveness en mujeres hispanas. Ter. Psicol. 2008, 26, 117–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Moyano, N.; Sierra, J.C. Fantasías y pensamientos sexuales: Revisión conceptual y relación con la salud sexual. Rev. Puertorriqueña Psicol. 2014, 25, 376–393. [Google Scholar]
  26. Sierra, J.C.; Arcos-Romero, A.I.; Calvillo, C. Validity evidence and norms of the Spanish version of the Hurlbert Index of Sexual Fantasy. Psicothema 2020, 32, 429–436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Sánchez-Fuentes, M.M.; Santos-Iglesias, P.; Sierra, J.C. A systematic review of sexual satisfaction. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 2014, 14, 67–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Pérez-González, S.; Moyano, N.; Sierra, J.C. La capacidad de ensoñación sexual: Su relación con la actitud hacia las fantasías sexuales y rasgos de personalidad. Cuad. Med. Psicosom. Psiquiatr. 2011, 99, 9–20. [Google Scholar]
  29. Perla, F.; Sierra, J.C.; Vallejo-Medina, P.; Gutiérrez-Quintanilla, R. Un estudio psicométrico de la versión española reducida del Hurlbert Index of Sexual Fantasy. Bol. Psicol. 2009, 96, 7–16. [Google Scholar]
  30. Desvarieux, A.R.; Salamanca, Y.; Ortega, V.; Sierra, J.C. Validación de la versión en castellano del Hurlbert Index of Sexual Fantasy: Una medida de actitud hacia las fantasías sexuales. Rev. Mex. Psicol. 2005, 22, 529–539. [Google Scholar]
  31. Moyano, N.; Sierra, J.C. Adaptación y validación de la versión española del Sexual Cognitions Checklist (SCC). An. Psicol. 2012, 28, 904–914. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Cervilla, O.; Vallejo-Medina, P.; Gómez-Berrocal, C.; Sierra, J.C. Development of the Spanish short version of Negative Attitudes Toward Masturbation Inventory. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 2021, 21, 100222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Cervilla, O.; Vallejo-Medina, P.; Gómez-Berrocal, C.; de la Torre, D.; Sierra, J.C. Validación de la Orgasm Rating Scale en el contexto de la masturbación. Psicothema 2021, in press. [Google Scholar]
  34. Ortega, V.; Ojeda, P.; Sutil, F.; Sierra, J.C. Culpabilidad sexual en adolescentes: Estudio de algunos factores relacionados. An. Psicol. 2005, 21, 268–275. [Google Scholar]
  35. Sierra, J.C.; Perla, F.; Santos-Iglesias, P. Culpabilidad sexual en jóvenes: Influencia de las actitudes y la experiencia sexual. Rev. Latinoam. Psicol. 2011, 43, 73–81. [Google Scholar]
  36. Burri, A.; Carvalheira, A. Masturbatory Behavior in a Population Sample of German Women. J. Sex. Med. 2019, 16, 963–974. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Haus, K.R.; Thompson, A.E. An examination of the sexual souble standard pertaining to masturbation and the impact of assumed motives. Sex. Cult. 2020, 24, 809–834. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Hogarth, H.; Ingham, R. Masturbation among young women and associations with sexual health: An exploratory study. J. Sex. Res. 2009, 46, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Mosher, D.L.; O’Grady, K.E. Homosexual threat, negative attitudes toward masturbation, sex guilt, and male’s sexual and affective reactions to explicit films. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1979, 47, 860–873. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Mosher, D.L.; Abramson, P.R. Subjective sexual arousal to films of masturbation. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1977, 45, 796–807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Sierra, J.C.; Santos-Iglesias, P.; Monge, F.S. Inventario de Actitudes hacia la Masturbación: Validez, fiabilidad y propuesta de una versión reducida para población adolescente. Rev. Argent. Clin. Psicol. 2013, 22, 57–65. [Google Scholar]
  42. Font, P. Evaluación y tratamiento de las disfunciones sexuales: Un modelo de intervención. Inf. Psicològica 2013, 103, 46–61. [Google Scholar]
  43. Sierra, J.C.; Buela-Casal, G. Evaluación y tratamiento de las disfunciones sexuales. In Manual de Evaluación y Tratamientos Psicológicos, 2nd ed.; Buela-Casal, G., Sierra, J.C., Eds.; Biblioteca Nueva: Barcelona, Spain, 2004; pp. 439–485. [Google Scholar]
  44. McGahuey, C.A.; Gelenberg, A.J.; Laukes, C.A.; Moreno, F.A.; Delgado, P.L.; McKnight, K.M.; Manber, R. The Arizona Sexual Experience Scale (ASEX): Reliability and validity. J. Sex. Marital Ther. 2000, 26, 25–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Álvarez-Muelas, A.; Gómez-Berrocal, C.; Sierra, J.C. Study of sexual satisfaction in different typologies of adherence to the sexual double standard. Front. Psychol. 2021, 11, 609571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Sierra, J.C.; Moyano, N.; Vallejo-Medina, P.; Gómez-Berrocal, C. An abridge Spanish version of Sexual Double Standard Scale: Factorial structure, reliability and validity evidence. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 2018, 18, 69–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: The R Fundation: Vienna, Austria, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  48. RStudio Team. R Studio: Integrated Development for R (Computer Software); R Studio PBC: Boston, MA, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  49. Stekhoven, D.J.; Bühlmann, P. MissForest-non-parametric missing value imputation for mixed-type data. Bioinformatics 2011, 28, 112–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Sierra, J.C.; Vallejo-Medina, P.; Santos-Iglesias, P.; Lameiras Fernández, M. Validación del Massachusetts General Hospital-Sexual Functioning Questionnaire (MGH-SFQ) en población española. Aten. Primaria 2012, 44, 516–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Muñoz-García, L.E. Actitud Hacia la Masturbación en Función del Sexo, Orientación Sexual y Edad. In Proceedings of the XX Congreso Latinoamericano de Sexología y Educación Sexual (CLASES), Online, 14–15 June 2013. [Google Scholar]
  52. Carpintero, E.; Fuertes, A. Validación de la versión castellana del “Sexual Opinion Survey” (SOS). Cuad. Med. Psicosom. Psiquiatr. 1994, 31, 56–61. [Google Scholar]
  53. Del Río Olvera, F.J.; López, D.J.; Cabello, F. Adaptación del Cuestionario Sexual Opinion Survey: Encuesta Revisada de Opinión Sexual. Rev. Int. Androl. 2013, 11, 9–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Sánchez-Fuentes, M.M.; Sierra, J.C. Sexual satisfaction in a heterosexual and homosexual Spanish sample: The role of sociod-demographic characteristics, health indicators, and relational factors. Sex. Relatsh. Ther. 2015, 30, 226–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  56. Dunn, K.M.; Croft, P.R.; Hackett, G.I. Sexual problems: A study of the prevalence and need for health care in the general population. Fam. Pract. 1998, 15, 519–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Lucas Matheu, M. Sexualidad Madura; Editorial Síntesis: Madrid, Spain, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  58. Arcos-Romero, A.I.; Sierra, J.C. Factorial invariance, differential ítem functioning, and norms of the Orgasm Rating Scale. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 2019, 19, 57–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Basson, R.; Brotto, L.A. Disorders of sexual desire and subjective arousal in women. In Clinical Manual of Sexual Disorders; Balon, R., Taylor Segraves, R., Eds.; American Psychiatric Publishing: Washington, DC, USA, 2009; pp. 119–159. [Google Scholar]
  60. Cabello, F. Manual de Sexología y Terapia Sexual; Editorial Síntesis: Madrid, Spain, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  61. Clayton, A.H.; Hamilton, D.V. Female orgasmic disorder. In Clinical Manual of Sexual Disorders; Balon, R., Taylor Segraves, R., Eds.; American Psychiatric Publishing: Washington, DC, USA, 2009; pp. 251–271. [Google Scholar]
  62. Lameiras Fernández, M.; Carrera Fernández, M.V. Educación Sexual. De la Teoría a la Práctica; Pirámide: Madrid, Spain, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  63. Lameiras Fernández, M.; Rodríguez Castro, Y.; Ojea Rúa, M.; Dopereiro Rodríguez, M. Programa Coeducativo de Desarrollo Psicoafectivo y Sexual; Pirámide: Madrid, Spain, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  64. Álvarez-Muelas, A.; Gómez-Berrocal, C.; Sierra, J.C. Relación del doble estándar sexual con el funcionamiento sexual y las conductas sexuales de riesgo: Revisión sistemática. Rev. Iberoam. Psicol. Salud 2020, 11, 103–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Calvillo, C.; Sánchez-Fuentes, M.M.; Parrón-Carreño, T.; Sierra, J.C. Validation of the Interpersonal Exchange Model of Sexual Satisfaction Questionnaire in adults with a same-sex partner. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 2020, 20, 140–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Calvillo, C.; Sánchez-Fuentes, M.M.; Sierra, J.C. An explanatory model of sexual satisfaction in adults with a same-sex partner: An analysis based on gender differences. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1. Socio-demographic characteristics and differences between women and men.
Table 1. Socio-demographic characteristics and differences between women and men.
Total
N = 2000
Women
N = 1044
Men
N = 956
t2Cohen’s d
Age (M, SD)40.12 (12.48)39.04 (12.25)41.29 (12.63)4.05 ***0.18
Level of education (n, %) 7.29 *
Primary Education108 (5.60)56 (5.60)52 (5.60)
Secondary Education670 (34.70)319 (31.90)351 (37.70)
University degree (ongoing or completed)1152 (59.70)624 (62.50)528 (56.70)
Number of sexual partners (M, SD)11.91 (19.64)11.61 (16.13)12.24 (22.88)0.69
Currently have a partner (n, %) 25.25 ***
Yes1626 (81.30)805 (77.10)821 (85.90)
No374 (18.70)239 (22.90)135 (14.10)
Frequency of attending religious events 11.74
More than once a day2 (0.1%)1 (0.1%)1 (0.1%)
Once a day3 (0.2%)2 (0.2%)1 (0.1%)
A few times a week17 (0.9%)13 (1.4%)4 (0.4%)
Once a week54 (2.8%)28 (3%)26 (2.6%)
A few times a month75 (3.9%)41 (4.4%)34 (3.4%)
Once a month22 (1.1%)7 (0.8%)15 (1.5%)
Less than once a month445 (23.1%)225 (24.2%)220 (22.1%)
Never1309 (67.9%)613 (65.9%)696 (69.8%)
Frequency of praying in private 11.7
More than once a day54 (2.8%)29 (3.1%)25 (2.5%)
Once a day97 (5%)58 (6.3%)39 (3.9%)
A few times a week106 (5.5%)48 (5.2%)58 (5.8%)
Once a week10 (0.5%)3 (0.3%)7 (0.7%)
A few times a month100 (5.2%)40 (4.3%)60 (6%)
Once a month20 (1%)7 (0.8%)13 (1.3%)
Less than once a month205 (10.6%)97 (10.5%)108 (10.8%)
Never1340 (69.4%)646 (69.6%)694 (69.1%)
Note. * p < 0.05; *** p < 0.001.
Table 2. Comparison of sexual attitudes and sexual function between women and men.
Table 2. Comparison of sexual attitudes and sexual function between women and men.
Women
(n = 1044)
Men
(n = 956)
M (SD)M (SD)F(6, 1833)pƞ2
Erotophilia37.75 (4.81)37.67 (5.19)0.290.0592-
Positive attitude toward sexual fantasies29.76 (6.64)32.45 (6.03)88.56<0.0010.046
Negative attitude toward masturbation10.78 (2.24)11.25 (2.99)12.46<0.0010.007
Sexual desire2.92 (1.15)2.56 (0.92)53.81<0.0010.029
Sexual arousal2.97 (1.08)2.64 (0.88)52.0<0.0010.028
Ability to have an orgasm2.80 (1.18)2.39 (0.92)−61.96<0.0010.033
Orgasm satisfaction2.02 (1.14)1.97 (0.91)0.130.724-
Note. M = mean; SD = Standard deviation; ƞ2 = partial eta square.
Table 3. Hierarchical regression for sexual function in women.
Table 3. Hierarchical regression for sexual function in women.
Predictors BSEβ95%CItpVIF R 2
Model 1. Sexual desireAge0.0080.0030.0860.003, 0.0133.0020.0031.0090.15
Erotophilia−0.0130.008−0.052−0.028, 0.003−1.6040.1091.271
Positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies−0.0610.006−0.350−0.072, −0.050−10.944<0.0011.249
Negative attitudes toward masturbation0.0010.0150.002−0.030, 0.0310.0540.9571.092
Model 2. Sexual arousalAge0.0070.0030.0770.002, 0.0122.6570.0081.0090.13
Erotophilia−0.0090.007−0.038−0.023, 0.006−1.1770.2391.271
Positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies−0.0560.005−0.340−0.066, −0.045−10.536<0.0011.249
Negative attitudes toward masturbation−0.0070.015−0.014−0.036, 0.022−0.4530.6511.092
Model 3. Vaginal lubricationAge0.0050.0030.0540.000, 0.0101.8270.0681.0090.08
Erotophilia−0.0060.008−0.025−0.021, 0.009−0.7450.4561.271
Positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies−0.0440.005−0.269−0.055, −0.034−8.114<0.0011.249
Negative attitudes toward masturbation0.0130.0150.026−0.017, −0.0430.8540.3931.092
Model 4. Ability to have an orgasmAge−0.0110.003−0.111−0.016, −0.005−3.714<0.0011.009
Erotophilia0.0010.0080.005−0.015, 0.0170.1350.8931.271
Positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies−0.0440.006−0.249−0.056, −0.033−7.481<0.0011.249
Negative attitude toward masturbation0.0260.0160.049−0.006, 0.0581.5710.1161.092
Model 5. Orgasm satisfactionAge−0.0050.003−0.055−0.011, 0.000−1.8530.0641.0090.09
Erotophilia0.0090.0080.037−0.00, 0.0241.1130.2661.271
Positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies−0.0490.006−0.284−0.06, −0.038−8.586<0.0011.249
Negative attitude toward masturbation0.0340.0160.0670.003, 0.0652.1550.0311.092
Note. Higher scores for desire, arousal, vaginal lubrication, ability to have an orgasm and orgasm satisfaction, worse sexual function. B: non standardized beta; SE: standard error; β: standardized beta; 95%CI: 95% confidence interval.
Table 4. Hierarchical regression for sexual function in men.
Table 4. Hierarchical regression for sexual function in men.
Predictors BSEβ95%CItpVIF R 2
Model 1. Sexual desireAge0.0030.0020.037−0.002, 0.0071.1820.2371.0080.09
Erotophilia−0.0100.006−0.058−0.023, 0.002−1.6210.1051.364
Positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies−0.0370.005−0.245−0.047, −0.026−6.956<0.0011.302
Negative attitudes toward masturbation0.0200.0100.0650.000, 0.0401.9540.0511.180
Model 2. Sexual arousalAge0.0040.0020.0560.000, 0.0081.7980.0721.0080.10
Erotophilia−0.0100.006−0.061−0.022, −0.002−1.6930.0911.364
Positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies−0.0380.005−0.266−0.048, −0.028−7.585<0.0011.302
Negative attitudes toward masturbation0.0100.0100.034−0.009, 0.0291.0120.3121.180
Model 3. ErectionAge0.0220.0020.2980.018, 0.0279.825<0.0011.0080.13
Erotophilia−0.0150.007−0.081−0.028, −0.002−2.3080.0211.364
Positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies−0.0150.005−0.097−0.026, −0.005−2.8030.0051.302
Negative attitudes toward masturbation0.0300.0110.0930.009, 0.0512.8190.0051.180
Model 4. Ability to have an orgasmAge0.0010.0020.014−0.003, 0.0050.4500.6531.008
Erotophilia−0.0230.006−0.133−0.036, −0.011−3.655<0.0011.364
Positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies−0.0220.005−0.144−0.032, −0.011−4.029<0.0011.302
Negative attitudes toward masturbation0.0190.0100.060−0.002, 0.0391.7730.0771.180
Model 5. Orgasm satisfactionAge−0.0020.002−0.026−0.006, 0.003−0.8290.4071.0880.07
Erotophilia−0.0100.006−0.058−0.022, 0.002−1.5910.1121.364
Positive attitudes toward sexual fantasies−0.0280.005−0.184−0.038, −0.017−5. 184<0.0011.302
Negative attitudes toward masturbation0.0360.0100.1170.016, 0.0563.4680.0011.180
Note. Higher scores for desire, arousal, erection, ability to have an orgasm and orgasm satisfaction, worse sexual function. B: non standardized beta; SE: standard error; β: standardized beta; 95%CI: 95% confidence interval.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Sierra, J.C.; Gómez-Carranza, J.; Álvarez-Muelas, A.; Cervilla, O. Association of Sexual Attitudes with Sexual Function: General vs. Specific Attitudes. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 10390. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph181910390

AMA Style

Sierra JC, Gómez-Carranza J, Álvarez-Muelas A, Cervilla O. Association of Sexual Attitudes with Sexual Function: General vs. Specific Attitudes. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(19):10390. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph181910390

Chicago/Turabian Style

Sierra, Juan Carlos, Jennifer Gómez-Carranza, Ana Álvarez-Muelas, and Oscar Cervilla. 2021. "Association of Sexual Attitudes with Sexual Function: General vs. Specific Attitudes" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 19: 10390. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph181910390

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop