Next Article in Journal
Male-On-Male Child and Adolescent Sexual Abuse in the Caribbean Region of Colombia: A Secondary Analysis of Medico-Legal Reports
Previous Article in Journal
The Influence of Maturity Status on Anthropometric Profile and Body Composition of Youth Goalkeepers
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Subjective Well-Being in Early Adolescence: Observations from a Five-Year Longitudinal Study

by
Mònica González-Carrasco
1,*,
Marc Sáez
2,* and
Ferran Casas
1
1
Research Group on Childhood, Adolescence, Children’s Rights and their Quality of Life, Research Institute on Quality of Life, University of Girona, Pujada de Sant Domènec, 9, 17004 Girona, Spain
2
Research Group on Statistics, Econometrics and Health. (GRECS), University of Girona and CIBER of Epidemiology and Public Health (CIBERESP), Carrer de la Universitat de Girona 10, Campus de Montilivi, 17003 Girona, Spain
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17(21), 8249; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17218249
Submission received: 17 September 2020 / Revised: 2 November 2020 / Accepted: 3 November 2020 / Published: 8 November 2020
(This article belongs to the Section Adolescents)

Abstract

:
This article aims to redress the lack of longitudinal studies on adolescents’ subjective well-being (SWB) and highlight the relevance of knowledge deriving from such research in designing public policies for improving their health and wellbeing in accordance with the stage of development they are in. To achieve this, the evolution of SWB during early adolescence (in adolescents aged between 10 and 14 in the first data collection) was explored over a five year period, considering boys and girls together and separately. This involved comparing different SWB scales and contrasting results when considering the year of data collection versus the cohort (year of birth) participants belonged to. The methodology comprised a generalized linear mixed model using the INLA (Integrated Nested Laplace Approximation) estimation within a Bayesian framework. Results support the existence of a decreasing-with-age trend, which has been previously intuited in cross-sectional studies and observed in only a few longitudinal studies and contrasts with the increasing-with-age tendency observed in late adolescence. This decrease is also found to be more pronounced for girls, with relevant differences found between instruments. The decreasing-with-age trend observed when the year of data collection is taken into account is also observed when considering the cohort, but the latter provides additional information. The results obtained suggest that there is a need to continue studying the evolution of SWB in early adolescence with samples from other cultures; this, in turn, will make it possible to establish the extent to which the observed decreasing-with-age trend among early adolescents is influenced by cultural factors.

1. Introduction

Subjective well-being (SWB) is among the core concepts within the quality of life paradigm and is considered as part of a positive approach to the study of health, human behaviour, and its determinants. SWB has been conceptualized as how people evaluate their lives, both in general and regarding specific life domains (family, friends, leisure time, etc.) [1]. This definition is applicable not only to adults, but also to children and adolescents [2], for whom SWB is considered to be a very important predictor of different outcomes, including mental health and academic achievement [3].
There is widespread agreement that SWB is composed of three elements, comprising the combination of one cognitive process (satisfaction/dissatisfaction) and two affective processes (positive and negative affect) [4]. The majority of studies focus on either cognitive or emotional aspects, but rarely on both, although some exceptions do exist [5]. The cognitive dimension (specifically, life satisfaction) is considered to be a much more stable component of SWB than the affective component (positive and negative affect) and has been more frequently analysed [3].
There is an increasing awareness of the need for children and adolescents’ SWB-based indicators to aid decision making within the context of public policies aimed at this population (see a discussion of this in [6]). Recently, strong emphasis has also been placed on children’s and adolescents’ SWB being considered a product of the intersection between the different contexts in which they are embedded, mainly the family, the neighbourhood, and the school [7], with Bronfenbrenner’s ecological approach [8,9,10] serving as an umbrella in this respect.
However, the question of whether there are changes in SWB during late childhood and early adolescence has only recently received the attention of researchers working in this field, despite it being a period in which wellbeing would appear to have its own idiosyncrasies with respect to previous and subsequent age groups [11]. Among the reasons for this is that most research has focused on adult samples, in which a relative stability in their levels of SWB has been observed, although with certain oscillations at certain periods of life [12]. This had also led researchers to think that a certain stability would be the predominant pattern during childhood and adolescence, although there are not many studies to support this.
Models that include time as a relevant variable when theorizing on SWB do so in such a generic way that it is difficult to make predictions regarding variations that can occur throughout the life span. Further, most models are not capable of explaining changes in the levels of SWB that may occur at a given time either. As an exception to this, although still formulated with the adult population in mind, we found the homeostatic model [13].
This model uses the biological systems of the body that control functions as basic as blood pressure and heart rate as an analogy to explain variations in SWB. Following the example, minimum and maximum values exist for both blood pressure and heart rate that define the expected range of normative levels. Although numerous environmental and internal factors can alter these expected levels (stress and disease, for instance), under normal circumstances, they tend to return to prior values. Based on the above, a homeostatically genetically-driven mechanism is considered to control SWB [13,14]. This would explain the existence of normative values for SWB and relatively small variations for people belonging to the same culture (a standard deviation of only 0.8 points on a 0-100 scale from 26 surveys conducted with Australian adults), except in those cases where the buffers have failed.
The interest of this model in explaining variations in levels of SWB is unquestionable. However, despite expected levels in the aforementioned biological basic functions varying with age (blood pressure tends to increase, while heart rate tends to decrease), this model has not yet incorporated the time variable in a way that helps explain changes occurring in the SWB of children and adolescents, which are quite the opposite of static. This is why it considers the normative values to be the same as for adults [15], even though mean SWB levels have been observed to be higher for most children.
To this, we must add the fact that the majority of studies on children’s and adolescents’ SWB are of a cross-sectional nature, which acts as a hindrance to gaining in-depth knowledge on how the evolutionary changes that take place throughout their development affect or are affected by their SWB. Some years ago, various cross-sectional studies started revealing that this supposed stability might not be taking place [11], a result that was later confirmed when scales with more response options were included (0–10-point scales) and particularly, when involving longitudinal studies. Two main results were observed. On the one hand, a decreasing-with-age trend, according to which levels of SWB decrease from 10 years of age onwards [5,16], although some studies situate this decrease between 13 and 15 (Shek & Liu, cited in [3]). On the other hand, that the pattern of decline is more pronounced among girls than boys, the former obtaining lower levels than the latter despite starting from higher scores [16]. It has also been observed that factors contributing to a decrease in SWB levels differ from those contributing to its increase and are gender sensitive [17].
However, some contradictory results have been observed in reference to late adolescence when it comes to gender. For instance, Steinmayr et al. [3] did not observe any association between gender and satisfaction with life at any of the four data collection points throughout the three years of the longitudinal study they performed with late German adolescents (mean of 16.43 at the beginning of the study). In a study of Finnish samples, which ranged from 642 to 749 cases depending on the time of data collection, Salmela-Aro and Tuominen-Soini [12] found that life satisfaction increased among 15 to 17-year-old girls (and also among boys, although they displayed more homogenous pathways than girls) during the transition from comprehensive school to their subsequent educational pathway.
Knowing exactly when this decrease takes place and what can cause it will allow for actions to be introduced for its prevention, especially in those cases where it might be particularly pronounced and in which external environment factors can play a very important role. Another noteworthy observation is that longitudinal studies have frequently used only single-item life satisfaction measures (Lucas & Donnellan, cited in [12]), despite different instruments for measuring SWB appearing to be sensitive to detecting these changes to a greater or lesser degree and multiple-item SWB scales being more sensitive (i.e., they take into account the measurement of different life domains when evaluating SWB) than single-item scales [16]. However, these results were observed with only a two year follow-up study. This would need to be lengthened to determine the extent to which these first observations remain valid with a longer follow up—precisely one of the aims of this article.
A relevant issue that has not yet been sufficiently explored is which indicator best monitors the evolution of SWB over time: year of birth (generally referred to as cohort), year of data collection, or both at the same time. This debate cannot be separated from that regarding which statistical technique is most appropriate for studying this evolution. Since SWB does not follow a Gaussian distribution due to the existence of an “optimistic bias” (the mean is not at the midpoint, but shifted to the right on a scale from less to more), many statistical tests have important limitations in this regard.
Taking into account all of the above, the aim of this article is to explore the evolution of early adolescents’ SWB over a five year period, considering boys and girls together and separately. This involves comparing different SWB scales and results when considering the year of data collection versus the cohort participants belonged to. To achieve this, a generalized linear mixed model will be constructed using the INLA (Integrated Nested Laplace Approximation) estimation within a Bayesian framework, as described in [18,19].

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Data were collected from an overall sample of 2180 Catalan adolescents (i.e., from Catalonia, north-east Spain) over a period of five consecutive years. Of these adolescents, 484 only responded once, 755 twice, 539 three times, 213 responded for four consecutive years, and 189 answered for five consecutive years (Table 1). In order to allow detailed analysis of the differences between data collections and cohorts, data from participants who only answered the questionnaire once were retained.
Data collection included nine cohorts, born between 1998 and 2006. Those that answered five times were born between 1999 and 2002. All cohorts included slightly more girls than boys, except for 2001 (Table 2). In the first year of data collection, most participants were aged between 10 and 14—and therefore, belong to the category of early adolescence [20] with some, though only few, 9 and 16-year-olds. The following years, new cohorts of 10–11 year-olds were added. By the fifth year of data collection, those aged 10 in the first year were 14 years old, those aged 11 were 15, and so on.

2.2. Instruments

Following suggestions by Casas [6], several instruments have been used in order to compare the results obtained by each of them, the chosen scales being among the most widely used according to the age of the participants in the study. Scales for measuring SWB can be classified into the following categories [21]: (1) Single item scales (which evaluate SWB using only one item, formulated in a generic way), (2) Multiple item context free scales (which evaluate SWB using multiple items, but referring to the same construct), and (3) Multiple item domain based scales (including the measurement of different life domains when evaluating SWB). All of the instruments were administered in the Catalan language since this is the vehicular language at schools in Catalonia. They have also been used in previous studies after back-translation from English and piloting with children and adolescents of different ages.

2.2.1. Single Item Scales of Subjective Wellbeing

Overall Life Satisfaction (OLS)

The importance of including a single item scale on overall satisfaction when studying SWB has been highlighted by Campbell et al. [1]. In our research, we included a question on satisfaction with your overall life, using an end-labelled 0–10 scale, from completely dissatisfied to completely satisfied.

Happiness Taking Into Account Overall Life (HOL)

Campbell et al. [1] also pointed out the importance of including a single item scale on happiness when studying SWB. In our research, we have included the following question: “Taking into account your overall life, would you say you are…?” and options are then offered using a 0–10 scale, from extremely unhappy to extremely happy.

2.2.2. Multiple Item Context Free Scale of Subjective Wellbeing

Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) and Student’s Life Satisfaction Scale (SLSS)

The five original items of the SWLS scale [22] were used for all children during the first two years of data collection. From the third year on, we decided to use the SLSS modified version from the Children’s Worlds project ([23]; www.isciweb.org)—based on Huebner’s [24] SLSS scale—for the youngest children in order to improve understanding and data quality. They are both context free, multi item scales with equivalent items, with adapted wording for younger children in the case of the SLSS.
The SLSS includes five items; responses were originally encoded on a scale of 1–7 according to level of agreement. The version adapted by Casas et al. [25] was used in this research, with the 1–7 scale changed to a 0–10 scale in order to make it more sensitive and only four of the items used for data analysis, with Item 5 being excluded for the reasons explained in Casas et al. [25]. The labels Strongly agree and Strongly disagree were only used for the end values 0 and 10.

2.2.3. Multiple Item Domain Based Subjective Wellbeing Scales

Personal Well-Being Index (PWI)

This scale was designed as part of the Australian Unity Wellbeing Index. It originally included seven items on satisfaction with different life domains. Although the PWI was designed for use with adults, it has been tested on 12 year-olds and older adolescents in some countries, showing good psychometric properties [25,26]. In this research, we applied the adult version (PWI-A) to the second year of the 2nd cohort (mostly 12 year-olds) and subsequent cohorts. The seven items explored are: satisfaction with your standard of living, with your health, with what you are achieving in life, with feeling part of the community, with your personal relationships, with how safe you feel, and with your future security. The scale has an end-labelled format, from completely dissatisfied (0) to completely satisfied (10). The item referring to “satisfaction with feeling part of the community” has been substituted by “satisfaction with the groups of people I belong to”, as in previous Spanish samples, explained in Casas et al. [25,26].
Because the longitudinal study involved participants of different ages and therefore with different cognitive skills, a schoolchildren’s version of the PWI (PWI-SC), developed by Cummins and Lau [27], was applied to the 1st cohort (mostly 10 year-olds) and the first year of the 2nd cohort (mostly 11 year-olds). The seven items on this index are as follows: satisfaction about the things you have, with your health, with the things you want to be good at, about how safe you feel, about doing things away from your home, and about what may happen to you later on in your life. The scale has an end-labelled format, from completely dissatisfied (0) to completely satisfied (10). In order to improve participants’ comprehension, three emoticons corresponding to the values 0, 5, and 10 were included in this version, in line with the findings from a pilot study conducted previously [28].

Brief Multidimensional Student’s Life Satisfaction Scale (BMSLSS)

This scale was developed to be used with students aged 8–18. It includes five items referring to satisfaction with different life domains plus one item on overall satisfaction. The psychometric properties of this scale have been published in different articles [29,30,31]. Responses were originally encoded on a scale of 1–7, from terrible to delighted. The 1–7 scale was changed to a 0–10 scale in order to make it more sensitive. Labels were given to each value, describing satisfaction with each life domain from terrible to delighted.

2.3. Procedure and ethics

After the Department of Education had authorized the research, the schools willing to participate in the study were asked about the most convenient dates for administering the questionnaire. The main reason for some schools not wishing to participate was that a longitudinal study requires commitment over several years, requiring their participation in many data collections, hence the sampling being non-probabilistic.
When the research was initiated, approval from the Ethics Committee was not compulsory at the university to which the authors belong. Consent was previously requested from the parents or legal guardians of the students involved in the study. Once consent was obtained, paper questionnaires were administered in the classroom setting in consecutive years. Researchers were present during administration in order to answer questions and give instructions on how to complete the questionnaires. The regular teacher was also present to help the researchers organize the classroom.
Participants were notified that confidential treatment of the information they provided was explicitly guaranteed. All participants were informed that they were free to refuse participation and not to answer any of the questions if they accepted to participate.

2.4. Data Analysis

Linear mixed models with variable response from the Gaussian family were used. The following were included as explanatory variables in the model: gender, year of response to the questionnaire, and cohort (year of birth: 1998–2006). A random effect was also included in the model to capture individual heterogeneity; this comprised unobserved confounders specific to each child and invariant in time that in addition to sex, year, and cohort, could explain the response variable.
The relationships between both the response (or dependent) variables and the year and the cohort were allowed to be non-linear. Inferences were made following a Bayesian perspective, by means of the INLA approach [32,33]. Priors that penalize complexity (called PC priors) were used. These priors are robust in the sense that they do not have an impact on the results and in addition, have an epidemiological interpretation [34]. All analyses were carried out using the free software R (version 4.0.0) (The R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria) [35,36], through the INLA package [31,32,35].

3. Results

3.1. Single Item Scales of Subjective Wellbeing

Overall Life Satisfaction (OLS)

There was a statistically significant reduction in the OLS scores across the five year period considered; that is, comparing the first and fifth data collection, this reduction is significantly higher among girls (1.18 points) (Table 3).
The reduction in the mean was significant for all data collections, regardless of gender (Table 4).
The comparison between boys’ and girls’ OLS scores when year of data collection is considered showed that girls’ (gender = 2) OLS scores were higher than boys’ (gender = 1) at the start of the longitudinal study, but lower by the end, meaning this decrease was more pronounced among girls (Figure 1).
A decreasing-with-age trend was observed regardless of gender (Figure 2). However, for boys born in the years 2001 and 2002 and girls born in 2001, this trend was not significant (Table 5).
The older the participants, the lower their score on the OLS, regardless of gender (Figure 2).

Happiness Overall (HOL)

There was a statistically significant reduction in HOL scores across the five year period considered; that is, comparing the first and fifth data collection, this reduction is significantly higher among girls (1.46 points) (Table 6).
The reduction in the mean was significant for all data collections, regardless of gender (Table 7).
Girls’ (gender = 2) HOL scores were slightly higher than boys’ (gender = 1) at the start of the longitudinal study and slightly lower by the end (Figure 3). Differences between boys and girls were smaller for the HOL compared to the PWI and the OLS.
The decreasing-with-age trend was observed for all cohorts, with the exception of boys and girls born in the years 2001 and 2002 (Table 8).
The older the participants, the lower their score, regardless of gender (Figure 4).

3.2. Multiple Item Context Free Scale of Subjective Wellbeing

Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) and Student’s Life Satisfaction Scale (SLSS)

There was a statistically significant reduction in SWLS/SLSS scores across the five year period considered; that is, comparing the first and fifth data collection, this reduction is significantly higher among girls (4.57 points) (Table 9).
The reduction in the mean was significant for all data collections for both boys and girls (Table 10).
Girls’ (gender = 2) SWLS/SLSS scores were the same as boys’ (gender = 1) at the start of the longitudinal study and almost the same at the end (Figure 5).
The decreasing-with-age trend was observed for all cohorts, with the exception of boys and girls born in the years 2001 and 2002 (Table 11).
The older the participants, the lower the mean score, regardless of gender (Figure 6).

3.3. Multiple Item Domain Based Subjective Wellbeing Scale

Personal Wellbeing Index (PWI)

There was a statistically significant reduction in the PWI scores (almost 1 point on a 0–11 point scale departing from a mean of 85.55) across the five year period considered; that is, comparing the first and fifth data collection, this reduction is significantly higher among girls (1.23 points) (Table 12).
When the year of data collection is taken into account, we observed that boys from the third year of data collection fell outside this trend as the reduction in the mean was not significant. For the rest of the years of data collection, this decrease in the PWI mean was statistically significant (Table 13). In the case of girls, this decrease was statistically significant for all years when data were collected.
The comparison of boys’ and girls’ PWI scores showed that girls’ (gender = 2) scores were higher than boys’ (gender = 1) at the start of the longitudinal study but lower at the end, meaning this decrease was more pronounced for girls (Figure 7).
The decreasing-with-age trend observed was significant for all cohorts, with the exception of both boys and girls born in the years 2001 and 2002 (Table 14).
The older the participants, the lower the PWI scores, regardless of gender (Figure 8).

Brief Multidimensional Student’s Life Satisfaction Scale (BMSLSS)

There is a statistically significant reduction in BMSLSS scores across the five year period considered; that is, comparing the first and fifth data collection, this reduction is significantly higher among girls (1.75 points) (Table 15).
The reduction in the mean is significant for all data collections (Table 16), regardless of gender.
Girls’ (gender = 2) BMSLSS scores were higher than boys’ (gender = 1) at the start of the longitudinal study, but lower at the end. This decrease was more pronounced for girls. For boys, the trend in BMSLSS scores stabilizes between the fourth and fifth years of data collection (Figure 9).
The decreasing-with-age trend described is observed for all cohorts, with the exception of boys born in 2002 and girls born in 2001 (Table 17).
Boys and girls born in 1998 have lower mean scores than participants from other cohorts and those born in 2005 have the highest mean scores (Figure 10).

4. Discussion

The global means (considering all five data collections at the same time) for the five instruments used to evaluate SWB differ by 7.9 points on a 0–100 scale (from 78.33 for the SWLS/SLSS to 86.23 for the OLS). Although these differences are not very high, the fact that they exist raises the question of whether they are measuring exactly the same concept, as has already been highlighted by Casas [6], which would make it advisable not to limit data collection to only one of them. For all instruments, the means are above the normative values defined for Western adult populations (mean of 75 on a 0–100 scale, Cummins [14]), although the mean is only slightly above this for the SWLS/SLSS. This does not imply that the homeostatic model is not equally applicable to the non-adult population, but simply that normative values for adolescents should be revised, as opposed to that argued by Cummins [15]. Because instruments are generally used either to assess the cognitive or affective dimensions of SWB, in this study, two instruments of the same type (single item scales) that assess the cognitive (the OLS) and affective dimensions (the HOL), respectively, were applied. The means obtained with the two instruments are similar (86.23 versus 85.91).
The results obtained support the decreasing-with-age trend previously intuited in cross-sectional studies and clearly observed in a Spanish longitudinal study starting with 10-year-olds [5,16] and in a 6-year longitudinal study with a large sample of Chinese adolescents (N = 3.328) starting with 12-year-olds [37]. This statistically significant reduction in levels of SWB with age, taking the global mean, is observed regardless of the instrument used. Thus, it is observed in the case of single item scales (the OLS and HOL), a multiple item context free scale (the SWLS/SLSS), and multiple item domain based scales (the PWI and BMSLSS). It is worth mentioning that the decreasing-with-age tendency is opposite to that found with samples of late adolescents (16 to 19 year-olds), for whom an increase in SWB is detected (see the work by Steinmayr et al. [3]). This reinforces the importance of conducting in-depth research into the period before puberty and the moment when puberty takes place (around age 10: Landsford & Banati [20]) as they differ from earlier and later stages of adolescence.
However, these results were obtained from cross-sectional studies (longitudinal comparative studies with samples from different socio-cultural contexts are also necessary), suggesting that there are differences in the age at which the decreasing-with-age tendency stabilizes. There is therefore a need for more studies with children under 12 in order to further our knowledge regarding the age at which this tendency starts, even if it is suspected that there are also cultural variations in this regard (see [37]).
This decrease is also found to be more pronounced for girls, with relevant differences found between instruments, especially with regard to the SWLS/SLSS (a decrease of 4.57 points on a 100-point scale) compared to the rest (between 1.18 for the OLS and 1.75 for the BMSLSS). This differs from the study conducted by Steinmayr et al. [3], who did not observe any association between gender and satisfaction with life at any of the four data collection points throughout the three years of the longitudinal study they performed with late German adolescents (mean of 16.43 at the beginning of the study). It even contradicts the results obtained by Salmela-Aro and Tuominen-Soini [12] who found that life satisfaction increased among 15 to 17-year-olds girls (and also among boys, although they displayed more homogenous pathways than girls) during the transition from comprehensive school to their subsequent educational pathway. The authors hypothesized that this increase might be the result of a better fit with their school environment (they can choose what to continue studying at this age).
However, it should be noted that in both studies, participants were late adolescents, not early adolescents as in this study. Another explanation may be found in social role theory, according to which boys’ and girls’ behaviours differ because above and beyond their physical differences, they have been attributed different roles in society [38]). Thus, the more different the roles they are attributed, the more differences there are expected to be in terms of SWB pathways.
When the year of data collection is taken into account, interesting differences are observed among instruments. In the case of the OLS, the PWI, and the BMSLSS, the decrease is clearly higher among girls across the five year period considered, despite their initial scores being higher than boys’. In the case of the BMSLSS, the decreasing-with-age trend stabilizes for boys from the fourth to the fifth year of data collection, a result that was not observed with any of the other four instruments. With regard to the HOL, differences between girls and boys are much smaller and almost non-existent when measured using the SWLS/SLSS. This again indicates not only the suitability of using different types of instruments to explore SWB at these ages, as pointed out by Casas [6] and also done in Casas and González-Carrasco [39], but also that the available instruments seem to be more gender-sensitive than may have been thought.
The decreasing-with-age trend previously observed when the year of data collection was taken into account is also observed when considering the cohort, but the latter provides additional information. Specifically, there are exceptions to this trend for some cohorts, such as those born in the years 2001 and 2002, for both girls and boys. This applies to all instruments except the OLS, for which this exception only applies for girls born in 2001. These results suggest that despite the trend observed for the cohort and the year of data collection being the same, nuances exist that would not have been observed if the two variables had not been considered at the same time. The aim of this article was to identify variations in SWB in early adolescence by age and cohort. For future studies, the challenge remains to delve deeper into the factors that contribute to such variations, something that was beyond the scope of this study.

5. Conclusions

Thanks to the longitudinal design adopted here, the study has given support to the existence of a decreasing-with-age trend in SWB scores within the period of early adolescence, a trend that previous authors have considered to be a “developmental phenomenon” (Goldbeck et al. [40], p. 969). It contrasts with an increasing-with-age trend observed among late adolescence in some other longitudinal studies. Knowing at what age this decline occurs and the factors that cause it is very important in order to carry out preventive actions that reduce any negative consequences that may be associated with this decrease. Further, for this, longitudinal studies are essential. The fact that the pattern of decrease is different for boys and girls suggests it is worth considering targeting different actions at different times.
Another consequence deriving from the results obtained here is the need to carry out longitudinal studies with more culturally diverse and wider samples than the one used here in order to establish the role that culture has on the age of onset of this decline. This article therefore constitutes just one example of the huge cultural diversity that potentially exists among early adolescents’ SWB trajectories and opens the door to continue investigating these issues in greater depth. In order to do this, it would also be necessary to continue comparing the results obtained with different instruments that cover both the affective and cognitive dimensions of SWB, while also including a complementary construct, that of psychological well-being (PWB).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.G.-C. and F.C.; methodology, M.G.-C. and F.C.; software, M.S.; formal analysis, M.S.; writing—original draft preparation, M.G.-C. writing—review, F.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (MINECO), grant number PSICO2010-19404 and PSICO2013-41512-P.

Acknowledgments

Thanks are due to all adolescents that kindly accepted to answer the questionnaires and to Barney Griffiths for the English edition of this paper.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Campbell, A.; Converse, P.E.; Rodgers, W.L. The Quality of American Life: Perceptions, Evaluations, and Satisfactions; Russell Sage: New York, NY, USA, 1976. [Google Scholar]
  2. Ben-Arieh, A.; Casas, F.; Frones, I.; Korbin, J.E. (Eds.) Multifaceted concept of child well-being. In Handbook of Child Well-Being; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2014; Volume 1, pp. 1–27. [Google Scholar]
  3. Steinmayr, R.; Wirthwein, L.; Modler, L.; Barry, M.M. Development of subjective well-being in adolescence. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 3690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Cummins, R.A.; Cahill, J. Avances en la comprensión de la calidad de vida subjetiva. [Advances in the comprehension of subjective quality of life]. Psychosoc. Int. 2000, 9, 185–198. [Google Scholar]
  5. Casas, F.; González-Carrasco, M. The evolution of positive and negative affect in a longitudinal sample of children and adolescents. Child Indic. Res. 2020, 13, 1503–1521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Casas, F. Subjective social indicators and child and adolescent well-being. Child Indic. Res. 2011, 4, 555–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. González-Carrasco, M.; Casas, F.; Viñas, F.; Malo, S.; Crous, G. The interplay between school and home location and its relationship with children’s subjective well-being. Child. Geogr. 2019, 17, 676–690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Bronfenbrenner, U. The Ecology of Human Development; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1981. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bronfenbrenner, U.; Evans, G.W. Developmental science in the 21st century: Emerging theoretical models, research designs, and empirical findings. Soc. Dev. 2000, 9, 115–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Bronfenbrenner, U.; Morris, P. The ecology of developmental processes. In Handbook of Child Psychology; Theoretical Models of Human Development; Damon, W., Lerner, R.M., Eds.; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1998; Volume 1, pp. 993–1027. [Google Scholar]
  11. Viejo, C.; Gómez-López, M.; Ortega-Ruiz, R. Adolescent’s psychological well-being: A multidimensional measure. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 2325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Salmera-Aro, K.; Tuominen-Soini, H. Adolescent’s life satisfaction during the transition to post-comprensive education: Antecedents and consequences. J. Happiness Stud. 2010, 11, 683–701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Cummins, R.A. Normative life satisfaction: Measurement issues and homeostatic model. In Social Indicators and Quality of Life Research Methods: Methodological Developments and Issues; Zumbo, B., Ed.; Kluwer: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  14. Cummins, R.A. Subjective wellbeing, homeostatically protected mood and depression: A synthesis. J. Happiness Stud. 2010, 11, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Cummins, R.A. Understanding the well-being of children and adolescents through homeostatic theory. In Handbook of Child Well-Being; Ben-Arieh, A., Casas, F., Frones, I., Korbin, J.E., Eds.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2014; Volume 2, pp. 635–662. [Google Scholar]
  16. González-Carrasco, M.; Casas, F.; Malo, S.; Viñas, F.; Dinisman, T. Changes with age in subjective well-being through the adolescent years: Differences by gender. J. Happiness Stud. 2017, 18, 63–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. González-Carrasco, M.; Casas, F.; Viñas, F.; Malo, S.; Gras, M.E.; Bedin, L. What leads subjective well-being to change throughout adolescence? An exploration of potential factors. Child Indic. Res. 2017, 10, 33–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Sáez, M.; Vidiella-Martin, J.; López-Casasnovas, G. Collateral Damages of the Great Crisis in Spain. A Longitudinal Health Study; Economics Working Papers 1603; Department of Economics and Business, Universitat Pompeu Fabra: Barcelona, Spain, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  19. Sáez, M.; López-Casanova, G. Assessing the effects on health inequalities of differential exposure and differential susceptibility of air pollution and environmental noise in Barcelona, 2007–2014. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 3470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  20. Landsford, J.E.; Banati, P. (Eds.) Handbook of Adolescent Development Research and Its Impact on Global Policy; Oxford University Press: Oxford, OH, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  21. Holte, A.; Barry, M.M.; Bekkhus, M.; Borge, A.I.H.; Bowes, L.; Casas, F.; Friborg, O.; Grinde, B.; Headey, B.W.; Jozefiak, T.; et al. Psychology of child well-being. In Handbood of Child Well-Being; Ben-Arieh, A., Casas, F., Frones, I., Korbin, J.E., Eds.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2014; Volume 1, pp. 555–631. [Google Scholar]
  22. Diener, E.; Emmons, R.; Larsen, R.; Smith, H.L. The satisfaction with life scale. J. Pers. Assess. 1985, 49, 71–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Rees, G.; Main, G. (Eds.) Children’s Views on Their Lives and Well-Being in 15 Countries: An Initial Report on the Children’s Worlds Survey, 2013–2014; Children’s Worlds Project (ISCWeB): York, UK, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  24. Huebner, E.S. Initial development of the students’ life satisfaction scale. Sch. Psychol. Int. 1991, 12, 231–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Casas, F.; Sarriera, J.C.; Abs, D.; Coenders, G.; Alfaro, J.; Saforcada, E.; Tonon, G. Subjective indicators of personal well-being among adolescents. Performance and results for different scales in Latin-language speaking countries: A contribution to the international debate. Child Indic. Res. 2012, 5, 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Casas, F.; Sarriera, J.C.; Alfaro, J.; González, M.; Malo, S.; Bertran, I. Testing the Personal Well-Being Index in 12-16 year-old adolescents in 3 different countries with 2 new items. Soc. Indic. Res. 2012, 105, 461–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Cummins, R.A.; Lau, A.L.D. Personal Well-Being Index-SCHOOL Children (PWI-SC) (English), 3rd ed.; Manual 2005; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2005; Available online: http://www.deakin.edu.au/research/acqol/instruments/PWI/PWI-school.pdf (accessed on 15 May 2006).
  28. Casas, F.; González, M.; Navarro, D.; Aligué, M. Children as advisers of their researchers: Assuming a different status for children. Soc. Indic. Res. 2013, 6, 193–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Seligson, J.L.; Huebner, E.S.; Valois, R.F. Preliminary validation of the Brief Multidimensional Student’s Life Satisfaction Scale. Soc. Indic. Res. 2003, 61, 121–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Seligson, J.L.; Huebner, E.S.; Valois, R.F. An investigation of a Brief Life Satisfaction Scale with elementary school children. Soc. Indic. Res. 2005, 73, 355–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Huebner, E.S.; Seligson, J.L.; Valois, R.F.; Suldo, S.M. A review of the brief multidimensional students’ life satisfaction scale. Soc. Indic. Res. 2006, 79, 477–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Rue, H.; Martino, S.; Chopin, N. Approximate Bayesian inference for latent Gaussian models using integrated nested Laplace approximations (with discussion). J. R. Stat. Soc. B. 2009, 71, 319–392. [Google Scholar]
  33. Rue, H.; Riebler, A.; Sørbye, H.; Illian, J.B.; Simpson, D.P.; Lindgren, F.K. Bayesian computing with INLA: A review. Annu. Rev. Stat. Appl. 2017, 4, 395–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Simpson, D.P.; Rue, H.; Martins, T.G.; Riebler, A.; Sørbye, S.H. Penalising model component complexity: A principled, practical approach to constructing priors (with discussion). Stat. Sci. 2017, 32, 1–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. R Core Team, R. A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing; R Core Team: Vienna, Austria, 2020; Available online: https://www.r-project.org (accessed on 20 July 2020).
  36. R INLA Project. 2020. Available online: http://www.r-inla.org/ (accessed on 25 July 2020).
  37. Shek, D.; Liang, L.-Y. Psychosocial factors influencing individual well-being in Chinese adolescents in Honh Kong: A six-year longitudinal study. Appl. Res. Qual. Life 2018, 13, 561–584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Eagley, A.H.; Wood, W. Social role theory. In Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology; Van Lange, P.A.M., Kruglanski, A.W., Tory Higgins, E., Eds.; SAGE: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2012; Volume 2, pp. 458–476. [Google Scholar]
  39. Casas, F.; González-Carrasco, M. Subjective Well-Being decreasing with age: New research on children over 8. Child Dev. 2019, 90, 375–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Goldbeck, L.; Schmitz, T.G.; Nesier, T.; Herschbach, P.; Henrich, G. Life satisfaction decreases during adolescence. Qual. Life Res. 2007, 16, 969–979. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Boys’ and girls’ trends for OLS scores over the five year longitudinal study by time of data collection (the vertical axis represents the original scores obtained with the instrument).
Figure 1. Boys’ and girls’ trends for OLS scores over the five year longitudinal study by time of data collection (the vertical axis represents the original scores obtained with the instrument).
Ijerph 17 08249 g001
Figure 2. Boys’ and girls’ trends for OLS scores over the five year longitudinal study by cohort (the vertical axis represents the original scores obtained with the instrument).
Figure 2. Boys’ and girls’ trends for OLS scores over the five year longitudinal study by cohort (the vertical axis represents the original scores obtained with the instrument).
Ijerph 17 08249 g002
Figure 3. Boys’ and girls’ trends for HOL scores over the five year longitudinal study by time of data collection (the vertical axis represents the original scores obtained with the instrument).
Figure 3. Boys’ and girls’ trends for HOL scores over the five year longitudinal study by time of data collection (the vertical axis represents the original scores obtained with the instrument).
Ijerph 17 08249 g003
Figure 4. Boys’ and girls’ trends for HOL scores over the five year longitudinal study by cohort (the vertical axis represents the original scores obtained with the instrument).
Figure 4. Boys’ and girls’ trends for HOL scores over the five year longitudinal study by cohort (the vertical axis represents the original scores obtained with the instrument).
Ijerph 17 08249 g004
Figure 5. Boys’ and girls’ trends for SWLS/SLSS scores over the five years longitudinal study by time of data collection (the vertical axis represents the original scores obtained with the instrument).
Figure 5. Boys’ and girls’ trends for SWLS/SLSS scores over the five years longitudinal study by time of data collection (the vertical axis represents the original scores obtained with the instrument).
Ijerph 17 08249 g005
Figure 6. Boys’ and girls’ trends for SWLS/SLSS scores over the five year longitudinal study by cohort (the vertical axis represents the original scores obtained with the instrument).
Figure 6. Boys’ and girls’ trends for SWLS/SLSS scores over the five year longitudinal study by cohort (the vertical axis represents the original scores obtained with the instrument).
Ijerph 17 08249 g006
Figure 7. Boys’ and girls’ trends for PWI scores over the five year longitudinal study by time of data collection (the vertical axis represents the original scores obtained with the instrument).
Figure 7. Boys’ and girls’ trends for PWI scores over the five year longitudinal study by time of data collection (the vertical axis represents the original scores obtained with the instrument).
Ijerph 17 08249 g007
Figure 8. Boys’ and girls’ trends for PWI scores over the five year longitudinal study by cohort (the vertical axis represents the original scores obtained with the instrument).
Figure 8. Boys’ and girls’ trends for PWI scores over the five year longitudinal study by cohort (the vertical axis represents the original scores obtained with the instrument).
Ijerph 17 08249 g008
Figure 9. Boys’ and girls’ trends for BMSLSS scores over the five years longitudinal study by time of data collection (the vertical axis represents the original scores obtained with the instrument).
Figure 9. Boys’ and girls’ trends for BMSLSS scores over the five years longitudinal study by time of data collection (the vertical axis represents the original scores obtained with the instrument).
Ijerph 17 08249 g009
Figure 10. Boys’ and girls’ trends for BMSLSS scores over the five years longitudinal study by cohort (the vertical axis represents the original scores obtained with the instrument).
Figure 10. Boys’ and girls’ trends for BMSLSS scores over the five years longitudinal study by cohort (the vertical axis represents the original scores obtained with the instrument).
Ijerph 17 08249 g010
Table 1. Sample characteristics.
Table 1. Sample characteristics.
Data Collections and CohortsNumber of Years the Child Answered the QuestionnaireTotal
12345
Cohort
(year of birth)
19981613440390229
19992582692213211
20002886518639290
20011888273169233
20021176613568251
20037711213600325
2004294613200207
2005571312300211
20062230000223
Total4847555392131892180
Table 2. Number of years the child answered the questionnaire by cohort and gender.
Table 2. Number of years the child answered the questionnaire by cohort and gender.
Cohort and GenderCohortTotal
199819992000200120022003200420052006
boyNumber of years the child answered the questionnaire189159539112699221
259394549375219590359
32125191935685480249
4171035149000085
502134032000087
Total105851271311181598493991001
girlNumber of years the child answered the questionnaire18161396381831124262
275434139396027720396
31944328266878150290
422125117260000128
50112629360000102
Total1241261631021331661231181241179
TotalNumber of years the child answered the questionnaire11625281811772957223483
213482868876112461310755
34069512761136132230539
439228631350000213
50133969680000189
Total2292112902332513252072112232180
Table 3. Variations in mean Overall Life Satisfaction (OLS) scores over the five year longitudinal study.
Table 3. Variations in mean Overall Life Satisfaction (OLS) scores over the five year longitudinal study.
Coefficients0.025
Quantile
0.975
Quantile
Probability of Coefficient Differing from Zero
Intercept86.2385.4687.001.00
gender (girls)−1.18−2.22−0.140.99
Table 4. Variations in mean OLS scores by time of data collection.
Table 4. Variations in mean OLS scores by time of data collection.
GenderYear of Data CollectionMSD0.025 Quantile0.975 Quantile
Boys1st5.700.514.696.70
2nd2.590.481.653.53
3rd−1.190.42−2.00−0.37
4th−2.770.45−3.66−1.88
5th−4.320.51−5.32−3.32
Girls1st6.940.476.037.86
2nd3.660.442.794.53
3rd−0.830.39−1.59−0.07
4th−3.440.43−4.28−2.60
5th−6.330.48−7.27−5.38
Table 5. Variations in mean OLS scores by cohort.
Table 5. Variations in mean OLS scores by cohort.
GenderCohortMSD0.025 Quantile0.975 Quantile
Boys1998−7.710.97−9.62−5.81
1999−5.390.95−7.26−3.52
2000−3.100.82−4.72−1.49
2001−1.0590.79−2.610.50
2002−0.200.81−1.791.38
20033.830.832.205.46
20045.810.963.937.69
20057.831.135.6210.04
Girls1998−11.190.90−12.97−9.41
1999−8.320.84−9.97−6.68
2000−5.370.73−6.80−3.94
2001−0.540.85−2.211.11
20021.550.760.053.04
20034.530.812.956.11
20047.270.855.598.94
200512.071.0210.0614.08
Table 6. Variations in mean Happiness Taking into Account Overall Life (HOL) scores over the five year longitudinal study.
Table 6. Variations in mean Happiness Taking into Account Overall Life (HOL) scores over the five year longitudinal study.
Coefficients0.025
Quantile
0.975
Quantile
Probability of Coefficient Different from Zero
intercept85.9185.1486.691.00
gender (girls)−1.46−2.50−0.410.99
Table 7. Variations in mean HOL scores by time of data collection.
Table 7. Variations in mean HOL scores by time of data collection.
GenderYear of Data CollectionMSD0.025 Quantile0.975 Quantile
Boys1st6.430.485.4817.35
2nd3.420.442.524.27
3rd−1.200.38−1.95−0.452
4th−3.370.41−4.18−2.55
5th−5.280.48−6.21−4.31
Girls1st7.080.436.247.95
2nd3.840.413.044.66
3rd−1.220.35−1.91−0.56
4th−3.610.39−4.38−2.83
5th−6.090.45−7.00−5.21
Table 8. Variations in mean HOL scores by cohort.
Table 8. Variations in mean HOL scores by cohort.
GenderCohortMSD0.025 Quantile0.975 Quantile
Boys1998−9.470.96−11.34−7.56
1999−7.140.93−8.97−5.31
2000−3.7810.82−5.35−2.13
2001−1.430.78−2.970.09
2002−0.170.84−1.871.42
20034.260.832.675.94
20047.240.935.419.08
200510.511.158.2012.70
Girls1998−11.280.90−13.08−9.54
1999−8.220.83−9.85−6.59
2000−4.950.73−6.41−3.53
2001−1.110.83−2.720.53
20021.280.79−0.222.86
20033.400.812.375.55
20047.720.846.069.37
200512.571.0410.5614.64
Table 9. Variations in mean SWLS/SLSS scores over the five year longitudinal study.
Table 9. Variations in mean SWLS/SLSS scores over the five year longitudinal study.
Coefficients0.025
Quantile
0.975
Quantile
Probability of Coefficient Differing from Zero
Intercept78.3376.8779.791.00
gender (girls)−4.57−6.27−2.870.99
Table 10. Variations in mean SWLS/SLSS scores by time of data collection.
Table 10. Variations in mean SWLS/SLSS scores by time of data collection.
GenderYear of Data CollectionMSD0.025 Quantile0.975 Quantile
Boys1st6.450.715.067.85
2nd2.550.581.413.70
3rd−1.620.53−2.67−0.58
4th−3.020.58−4.16−1.87
5th−4.370.68−5.71−3.03
Girls1st6.320.625.097.54
2nd2.180.541.113.25
3rd−1.540.50−2.53−0.55
4th−2.310.56−3.41−1.21
5th−4.650.65−5.93−3.37
Table 11. Variations in mean SWLS/SLSS scores by cohort.
Table 11. Variations in mean SWLS/SLSS scores by cohort.
GenderCohortMSD0.025 Quantile0.975 Quantile
Boys1998−8.211.28−10.73−5.70
1999−5.981.21−8.36−3.61
2000−2.881.07−4.99−0.78
2001−1.071.06−3.151.00
2002−0.871.11−3.061.313
20033.961.051.886.03
20047.901.325.3110.50
20057.172.701.8612.47
Girls1998−8.501.22−10.90−6.11
1999−6.051.13−8.27−3.83
2000−2.891.00−4.87−0.92
2001−0.421.08−2.541.70
2002−0.061.09−2.192.08
20033.631.041.595.66
20047.451.245.019.89
20056.842.661.6112.07
Table 12. Variations in mean Personal Well-Being Index (PWI) scores over the five year longitudinal study.
Table 12. Variations in mean Personal Well-Being Index (PWI) scores over the five year longitudinal study.
Coefficients0.025
Quantile
0.975
Quantile
Probability of Coefficient
Differing from Zero
intercept85.5584.7486.351.00
gender (girls)−1.23−2.01−0.440.99
Table 13. Variations in mean PWI scores by time of data collection.
Table 13. Variations in mean PWI scores by time of data collection.
GenderYear of Data CollectionMSD0.025 Quantile0.975 Quantile
Boys1st3.740.402.964.52
2nd2.010.351.322.70
3rd−0.560.30−1.150.026
4th−2.360.33−3.02−1.71
5th−2.830.40−3.61−2.04
Girls1st5.630.364.936.33
2nd2.890.322.253.52
3rd−0.630.27−1.17−0.093
4th−2.930.31−3.54−2.32
5th−4.960.38−5.71−4.21
Table 14. Variations in mean PWI scores by cohort.
Table 14. Variations in mean PWI scores by cohort.
GenderCohortMSD0.025 Quantile0.975 Quantile
Boys1998−6.470.78−8.00−4.92
1999−4.210.76−5.72−2.73
2000−1.730.66−3.01−0.41
2001−0.870.61−2.070.31
2002−0.530.63−1.790.69
20033.400.652.154.70
20044.710.763.206.21
20055.710.963.817.58
Girls1998−9.810.74−11.28−8.36
1999−6.130.67−7.46−4.80
2000−3.650.59−4.83−2.49
2001−1.020.65−2.290.26
20020.710.59−0.441.88
20033.850.622.615.06
20046.670.685.338.03
20059.380.887.6611.13
Table 15. Variations in mean Brief Multidimensional Student’s Life Satisfaction Scale (BMSLSS) scores over the five years longitudinal study.
Table 15. Variations in mean Brief Multidimensional Student’s Life Satisfaction Scale (BMSLSS) scores over the five years longitudinal study.
Coefficients0.025
Quantile
0.975
Quantile
Probability of Coefficient
Differing from Zero
intercept84.9284.2885.551.00
gender (girls)−1.75−2.60−0.890.99
Table 16. Variations in mean BMSLSS scores by time of data collection.
Table 16. Variations in mean BMSLSS scores by time of data collection.
GenderYear of Data CollectionMSD0.025 Quantile0.975 Quantile
Boys1st6.040.375.326.77
2nd3.080.362.373.79
3rd−1.110.31−1.72−0.50
4th−3.830.34−4.50−3.16
5th−4.180.38−4.92−3.44
Girls1st7.210.336.557.86
2nd3.800.323.174.44
3rd−0.850.28−1.40−0.29
4th−3.460.32−4.08−2.84
5th−6.700.35−7.39−6.01
Table 17. Variations in mean BMSLSS scores by cohort.
Table 17. Variations in mean BMSLSS scores by cohort.
GenderCohortMSD0.025 Quantile0.975 Quantile
Boys1998−9.630.79−11.18−8.08
1999−5.840.80−7.39−4.22
2000−3.110.69−4.44−1.74
2001−1.650.67−2.98−0.36
2002−0.450.68−1.810.87
20034.280.702.945.69
20047.0690.795.518.62
20059.330.957.4411.15
Girls1998−11.270.74−12.73−9.83
1999−7.790.71−9.19−6.41
2000−4.370.61−5.59−3.18
2001−1.000.72−2.380.43
20020.580.64−0.671.85
20034.070.682.715.38
20048.070.706.689.45
200511.710.8510.0713.40
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

González-Carrasco, M.; Sáez, M.; Casas, F. Subjective Well-Being in Early Adolescence: Observations from a Five-Year Longitudinal Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 8249. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17218249

AMA Style

González-Carrasco M, Sáez M, Casas F. Subjective Well-Being in Early Adolescence: Observations from a Five-Year Longitudinal Study. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2020; 17(21):8249. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17218249

Chicago/Turabian Style

González-Carrasco, Mònica, Marc Sáez, and Ferran Casas. 2020. "Subjective Well-Being in Early Adolescence: Observations from a Five-Year Longitudinal Study" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, no. 21: 8249. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17218249

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop