Next Article in Journal
Exploring Similarities and Differences of Non-European Migrants among Forensic Patients with Schizophrenia
Next Article in Special Issue
Enhancing Self-Esteem and Body Image of Breast Cancer Women through Interventions: A Systematic Review
Previous Article in Journal
The Effect of Smoking during Pregnancy on Severity and Directionality of Externalizing and Internalizing Symptoms: A Genetically Informed Approach
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Subjective Environmental Experiences and Women’s Breastfeeding Journeys: A Survival Analysis Using an Online Survey of UK Mothers

1
Department of International Development, London School of Economics & Political Science, London WC2A 2AE, UK
2
Institute for Global Health, University College London, London NW3 2PF, UK
3
UCL Anthropology, University College London, London WC1H 0BW, UK
4
BirthRites Independent Max Planck Research Group, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, 04103 Leipzig, Germany
5
Department of Population Health, London School of Hygiene & Tropical Medicine, London WC1E 7HT, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17(21), 7903; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17217903
Submission received: 30 September 2020 / Revised: 26 October 2020 / Accepted: 26 October 2020 / Published: 28 October 2020

Abstract

:
Local physical and social environmental factors are important drivers of human health and behaviour. Environmental perception has been linked with both reproduction and parenting, but links between subjective environmental experiences and breastfeeding remain unclear. Using retrospective data from an online survey of UK mothers of children aged 0–24 months, Cox-Aalen survival models test whether negative subjective environmental experiences negatively correlated with any and exclusive breastfeeding (max n = 473). Matching predictions, hazards of stopping any breastfeeding were increased, albeit non-significantly, across the five environmental measures (HR: 1.05–1.26) Hazards for stopping exclusive breastfeeding were however (non-significantly) reduced (HR: 0.65–0.87). Score processes found no significant time-varying effects. However, estimated cumulative coefficient graphs showed that the first few weeks postpartum were most susceptible to environmental influences and that contrary to our predictions, mothers with worse subjective environmental experiences were less likely to stop breastfeeding at this time. In addition, the hazard of stopping exclusive breastfeeding declined over time for mothers who thought that littering was a problem. The predicted increased hazards of stopping breastfeeding were only evident in the later stages of any breastfeeding and only for mothers who reported littering as a problem or that people tended not to know each other. Perceived harsher physical and social environmental conditions are assumed to deter women from breastfeeding, but this may not always be the case. Women’s hazards of stopping breastfeeding change over time and there may be particular timepoints in their breastfeeding journeys where subjective environmental experiences play a role.

1. Introduction

The UK has some of the lowest breastfeeding rates in the world. Whilst many UK mothers initiate breastfeeding, continuation rates remain low [1,2]. With breastfeeding providing a myriad of health benefits for both mothers and infants, this is a key public health problem [3,4], and one that does not just fall on mothers to solve. Infant feeding decisions are complex and multifactorial, and there are many layers of influence from family attitudes and social norms to economic and policy drivers [4,5,6,7,8]. Furthermore, a growing body of research suggests that physical and social environmental factors play a key role in women’s infant feeding journeys [9,10,11,12,13,14].
Local environmental factors have well-established links with a range of health outcomes, from chronic diseases [15] and the aging process [16], to health behaviours [17], mental health outcomes [18], and social well-being [19]. Harsher environments as indexed by greater neighbourhood deprivation, higher levels of crime, exposure to environmental hazards, and other place-based stressors have also been shown to predict reproductive and parenting behaviour. Mothers in harsher environments have been reported to have earlier first births, more births and be at greater risk of preterm deliveries, small for gestational age and lower birthweight infants [13,20,21,22,23]. Further, mothers have been shown to exhibit lower maternal warmth and harsher/more controlling parenting practices in more dangerous environments (i.e., those perceived to have more violence, crime and social disorder) [24,25]. Linked to health outcomes for mother and infant, connected with women’s fertility and a key component of parenting, it is therefore not surprising that breastfeeding has also been shown to be environmentally influenced.
How environmental influences are conceptualised and measured varies across studies. The Index of Multiple Deprivation (IMD), a summary score based on several environmental domains and available at electoral ward level (or equivalently sized geographies) [26] is conceptualised as area-based deprivation, and is a commonly used metric in environment-breastfeeding research. Findings have however been mixed, with greater deprivation associated with shorter breastfeeding durations in some studies [12,13,27] but not others [28]. Possible reasons for such mixed results include the overlooking of individual subjective environmental experiences [29,30] given the focus on objective or independent neighborhood assessments [9,31,32]. Further, composite measures like the IMD combine both physical and social environmental factors into a summary score, making it difficult to discern specific pathways or mechanisms of influence [32].
Human environments are complex and comprise a multitude of threats and resources, the combination of which varies greatly across contexts. Physical aspects of the environment comprise both natural and built components [33] and include environmental hazards such as pollution [34], but also positive aspects like the availability of green spaces [35,36] and general perceptions of how “nice” an area is [9,37]. On the other hand, social environmental factors include aspects related to community cohesion and safety such as neighbour friendliness and levels of crime [14,38]. Physical and social factors are however often interrelated, with for example, physical incivilities such as littering leading to increased fear of crime [39].
Some environmental aspects are more perceivable than others. Previous research has shown that visually non-detectable levels of pollution in air and water, i.e., physical aspects of the environment, are linked with breastfeeding outcomes [10], suggesting that some environmental threats may impact the physiology of lactation directly [40]. This is in contrast to more salient and perceivable environmental cues, such as the behaviour of others, i.e., the social environment, which instead may affect breastfeeding through psychological or cognitive pathways, i.e., mothers perceiving and processing information (consciously or unconsciously) and then altering their behaviour accordingly.
Environmental perception therefore provides an important potential explanation for how environmental threats and resources are translated into behavioural changes. As already discussed, local environmental conditions have been shown to predict a range of reproductive behaviours and outcomes [13,20,21,22,23]. Other studies have suggested that environmental perception may be driving some of these associations [37,41]. For example, women who perceive their environments unfavourably are more likely to start reproducing at younger ages [13,37] and to have lower birth weight infants [13,31]. While increasing evidence suggests subjective environmental experiences influence reproductive behaviours and outcomes, the relationship between environmental perception and breastfeeding duration remains relatively underexplored. The one study we know of that has looked at this found only weak evidence for a link between subjective environmental quality and duration of any breastfeeding [9]. That study combined a mix of subjective environmental experiences into a summary score, making it difficult to identify which aspects of the physical and/or social environment may be relevant. Furthermore, only breastfeeding initiation and duration of any breastfeeding (i.e., including mixed-feeding) were considered, and relationships with exclusive breastfeeding remain untested. The evidence for links between environmental perception and breastfeeding therefore remains inconclusive. Understanding environmental influences on breastfeeding duration can guide interventions to improve breastfeeding rates. In addition, considering the wider environmental determinants of infant feeding can help to combat the neoliberal public health message of maternal responsibility and shift the focus away from the mother [42]. A focus on environmental perception specifically provides at least two avenues for intervention—addressing the environmental threat and/or the (mis)perception. Measuring separate and specific physical and social environmental factors—rather than obscuring potential effects in composite measures—is necessary in order to be able to identify which environmental factors or perceptions thereof need addressing.
This study aims to test whether subjective environmental experience, as measured by responses to five environmental perception questions relating to different aspects of the physical and social environment, is associated with duration of any and exclusive breastfeeding in a UK sample of mothers. This study therefore builds on previous environmental perception research by looking at individual environmental experience factors separately in an attempt to identify potential pathways of influence and intervention. Whilst the lack of existing research means the role of environmental perception in shaping infant feeding journeys remains unestablished, existing environmental health literature suggests that harsher environmental conditions lead to worse health outcomes, fewer health-promoting behaviours, more adverse reproductive outcomes and altered parenting practices. This leads us to predict that mothers who perceive their environments less favourably will have shorter breastfeeding durations.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Data Collection

In this study we use data from a retrospective online survey which was developed as part of a wider project on social support and maternal experience (https://osf.io/7kb5q/) and hosted on Opinio (survey platform; http://objectplanet.com/opinio/). The survey included questions on maternal characteristics, household characteristics, infant characteristics, birth experiences, support experiences and infant feeding experiences, taking 15–20 min to complete. For more information on the survey, see https://osf.io/dbtpy/ and information published in [43].
We took an opportunistic approach and recruited participants through convenience-sampling between December 2017 and February 2018. While convenience-sampling is likely to introduce recruitment bias, it is cost and time efficient [44]. Survey adverts were posted on social networking sites (Twitter and Facebook), as well as a forum-based parenting website (Netmums). The Facebook groups were diverse, including parenting groups, infant feeding support groups, and second-hand baby-item groups. We specifically targeted local Facebook groups around the UK in an attempt to diversify our sample. However, despite these efforts it must be noted our sample is not representative of the broader population of UK mothers and our analyses are therefore only exploratory in nature; any extrapolation of these results beyond this sample should be done with caution.
On the survey landing page, participants were informed that there would be some questions about infant feeding, with an explicit statement that it did not matter whether infants were breastfed or formula fed. Multiple-entries were prevented using IP-address checks. Ethical approval for the survey was obtained from the UCL Research Ethics Committee (ref: 11479/001).

2.2. Data Cleaning

In total 883 women accessed the online survey by early 2018. Of those 883, 74 (8.4%) started the survey but did not go on to fill out any further details. Of those that completed the majority of the survey, 757 (93.6%) resided in the UK, while 739 had children born in the UK (91.3%). Those not based in the UK were removed from the sample due to ineligibility. Further, six individuals were removed from the sample as their children were older than two years when the survey was taken. This left a total eligible sample of 733 individuals.

2.3. Breastfeeding Outcomes

85.4% of the eligible sample (n = 626) provided a measure of breastfeeding initiation and/or duration. Of those women, 43 reported they never initiated breastfeeding (6.9%), leaving a total maximum sample size of 583 for the duration of breastfeeding analysis. Breastfeeding duration was measured in weeks and both duration of any and of exclusive breastfeeding were available. Estimates in days were divided by 7 to give weeks and the age of the child was used for women who reported that they were still breastfeeding. For any breastfeeding, durations were available for 579 women. For exclusive breastfeeding, 560 mothers provided an estimate of at least 1 day. We removed cases where mothers breastfed but never exclusively (n = 23). Due to apparent misunderstanding of the exclusive duration questions, we restricted the sample to those who were either still breastfeeding and reported having stopped exclusive breastfeeding or those who reported any breastfeeding for longer than they exclusively breastfed, suggesting they had understood the question correctly (removing a further 141 cases). As exclusive breastfeeding is recommended to just 6 months [1,45] we also dropped cases where estimated durations were higher than 7 months (n = 49) as these were likely erroneous. This left a maximum sample size of 626, 579 and 351 for initiation, any breastfeeding and exclusive breastfeeding analyses, respectively.

2.4. Subjective Environmental Experiences

The analytical sample was restricted to those who provided a yes or no response to at least one of five environmental questions (excluding 85 individuals), which reduced the maximum sample size to 541 observations. The questions were: 1. “My area is a nice place to live” (Yes/No); 2. “Crime is a problem in my area” (No/Yes); 3. “Littering and rubbish is a problem in my area” (No/Yes); 4. “People tend to know each other in my area” (Yes/No); 5. “In general, people help each other out in my area” (Yes/No).

2.5. Covariates

Our initial plan was to include several infant and maternal characteristics known to be important for predicting breastfeeding outcomes: birthweight [46], maternal age [47], parity [27], partnership status [48] and ethnicity [49]. However, due to small sample size, low variation and a lack of bivariate associations (see Section 3.1 below), we decided to adjust only for maternal age and parity in our first set of models (presenting just descriptives for the other characteristics). In our second set of models we also controlled for maternal education, a proxy for socioeconomic status (SES) which is a key driver of breastfeeding outcomes [2,12,50,51,52,53]. GCSEs and A-levels (i.e., middle and high-school level qualifications) were combined into one category due to small cell sizes and “Other” was recoded as missing. We did not adjust for other aspects of socioeconomic status to avoid issues associated with collinearity and power reduction, but present the descriptives for annual income, education, partner’s employment and financial situation for interest. Restricting the sample to complete cases for maternal age, parity and education reduced the sample to 525 cases. Running multi-level analyses meant further restricting the data to those who had provided an answer to the region questions, thereby reducing the sample down further to 512 observations, of whom 32 reported never initiating breastfeeding, leaving a maximum usable sample size of 480 and 293 for duration of any and exclusive breastfeeding, respectively.
Of these 512 mothers, responses were coded as NA/missing if they did not answer the partner’s employment (n = 11), or birthweight (n = 9) questions, or answered “Don’t know” (n = 21), “Prefer not to say” (n = 5) or didn’t answer (n = 2) the income question, or answered “Prefer not to say” (n = 2) or didn’t answer (n = 2) the financial situation question.
Annual income was split into high earners “£50,000 and above” or low earners “Under £50,000” as it was not clear if respondents provided take-home or pre-tax annual income and this was not equivalised income (i.e., we couldn’t be confident that, for example, £20,000 was meaningfully different from £30,000). Several other covariate categories were collapsed due to small cell sizes. “Looking for work” and “Not looking for work” were collapsed into “Unemployed” for partner’s employment status and “Finding it very difficult” and “Finding it quite difficult” were collapsed into “Finding it difficult” for financial situation. All non-White ethnicities were collapsed into “Other” for ethnicity (since our sample was overwhelming White leaving little variance across other categories). Given that multiparous mothers are more confident and experienced with breastfeeding [54], we categorised parity into “1st birth” and “2nd birth or higher” (collapsing “2”, “3”, “4” and “5”). Birthweights were sense-checked and recoded where obvious mistakes had been made (i.e., unit omitted, wrong unit listed) before being categorised into low (<2500 g) and normal (≥2500 g). Most respondents did not answer the multiple birth question and so we were unable to reliably exclude multiple births from the analyses, though we expect these to be low in number.

2.6. Data Analysis

As an initial descriptive analysis, we used Pearson’s Chi-squared test with Yates’ continuity correction and Fisher’s Exact Test where there were small cell sizes to compare probabilities of initiating breastfeeding across different environmental experiences, maternal and infant characteristics and socioeconomic statuses. We used Wilcoxon Rank Sum tests (2 groups) and Kruskal Wallis tests (3+ groups) to compare median breastfeeding durations across the same variables (as distributions of both any and exclusive breastfeeding durations were right/positively-skewed). Post-hoc Dunn tests were used to test which categories had significantly different medians.
It was predicted that there would be a diminishing probability of maintaining breastfeeding over time. However, visual inspection of QQ plots and the Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests revealed that neither the duration of any (D = 0.819, p < 0.001) nor exclusive (D = 0.608, p < 0.001) breastfeeding followed the predicted parametric Weibull distribution. The distributions were actually bimodal with any breastfeeding peaking around 0–10 and 30–40 weeks and exclusive breastfeeding peaking around 0–5 and 25–30 weeks.
Cox proportional hazards models were then pursued but some of the main predictors and covariates did not satisfy the proportional hazards assumption. We therefore used Cox-Aalen models which allowed for time-varying effects (using the timecox option of the timereg package in R [55]). Time–varying effects are presented as non-standard cumulative estimates (i.e., the hazard rate as the cumulative log of the hazard rate and the effects of non-proportional hazards covariates as the cumulative effect of the covariate over time) which have different interpretations to standard de-cumulated estimates. This limitation is offset by the package’s advantages. Firstly, the timecox package enables controlling for clustering at the area-level, which is not possible with the standard Cox proportional hazards model. Secondly, timecox permits testing whether parameters have significant effects on survival outcomes (with the Supremum-test) and additionally whether these effects are time-invariant (with the Kolmogorov-Smirnov and Cramer von Mises score processes tests) under the non-proportional hazards setting [55,56,57].
We ran two sets of main models for each environmental predictor. The first set of models (M1) adjusted for maternal age and parity and for clustering at the region level and the second set (M2) additionally for education to test whether socioeconomic status confounded the relationship between subjective environmental experiences and breastfeeding outcomes. Exclusive breastfeeding survival models were capped at 6 months. Due to differing missingness on our exposure variables, analytical sample sizes for our survival analyses ranged from 409 to 473 and 250 to 290 for duration of any and exclusive breastfeeding, respectively.
We used proportionality tests in separate Cox proportional hazards models and Aalen’s additive hazards models to decide which covariates should be set as constant (i.e., proportional hazards, multiplicative effect) and which should be set as time-varying (i.e., non-proportional, additive effect) in our main Cox-Aalen models testing for the effects of the five environmental experience questions on breastfeeding duration. These diagnostic models were fully adjusted for maternal age, parity and education but excluded the environmental indicators. Overall the results of the different tests for proportionality indicated that parity should be set as constant and age should be set as time-varying for both any and exclusive breastfeeding, whilst education should be set as time-varying for any breastfeeding and constant for exclusive breastfeeding (see Table A1 in the Appendix A).

3. Results

3.1. Sample Characteristics

93.8% of the mothers in our restricted sample initiated breastfeeding (n = 480) and with the exclusions described above, duration of any and exclusive breastfeeding were determinable for 100% and 61% of these mothers, respectively. The median durations of which were 31.6 weeks (Interquartile range [IQR] 42.8 weeks) and 22.1 weeks (IQR 23.7 weeks). This highlights that our sample had very high levels of breastfeeding initiation compared to the UK average which was reported at 81% in 2010 [2]. Sample characteristics are shown in Table 1.
As a reminder, our sample was restricted to mothers who answered at least one of the subjective environmental experience questions, and provided area, age, parity and education information. Within this restricted sample, missingness on environmental information ranged from 1.6% (“My area is a nice place to live”) to 14.6% (“Crime is a problem in my area”). The majority of mothers reported favourable environmental experiences: only 4.3% thought that their area wasn’t a nice place to live, 11.5% thought that crime was a problem in their area, 20.3% thought that littering and rubbish was a problem, 20.3% thought that people didn’t tend to know each other in their area and 15% thought that people did not help each other out. It is important to note here that in some instances we had very few cases of unfavourable environmental experiences (e.g., only 22 mothers thought that their area was not a nice place to live). Results should therefore be interpreted with caution. The vast majority (88.5%) of mothers lived in England.
Almost half of the sample was aged between 30–34 years, 95.7% were White, 97.9% had partners, 61.1% had just the one child and 94.3% had normal birthweight infants. This was a high SES sample, with 81.6% educated to degree level or higher, 95.5% having partners who were employed, 54.3% having an annual household income of £50,000 and above, 44.9% “doing alright” financially and 28.1% “living comfortably”.
None of the environmental experience indicators were significantly associated with breastfeeding initiation or duration in bivariate analyses. Associations with breastfeeding initiation were however mostly in the predicted direction, with mothers with worse subjective environmental experiences having lower rates; the exception was mothers who disagreed that people tended to know each other in their area having higher initiation rates (95.9%) than those who agreed (93.6%, p = 0.470). Although there were no significant differences across environmental experience categories, average breastfeeding durations were mostly shorter for mothers with worse environmental experiences. Exceptions were mothers who agreed that crime was a problem in their area having longer durations of any breastfeeding (33.8 weeks [IQR 44.2] versus 31.0 weeks [IQR 42.4], p = 0.331) and the same average duration of exclusive breastfeeding than those who disagreed (22.1 weeks [IQR 24.6] versus 22.1 weeks [IQR 19.6], p = 0.537). Similarly, durations of exclusive breastfeeding were the same for mothers who did and didn’t think that littering and rubbish was a problem in their area (22.1 [IQR 19.6] versus 22.1 [IQR 24.6], p = 0.179).
Region was not significantly associated with breastfeeding, but there was some geographical variation with South West England having the lowest initiation rate (76.2%), whilst the West Midlands had the lowest average durations of both any (16.5 weeks [IQR 40.6]) and exclusive breastfeeding (3 weeks [IQR 40.6]). Maternal age was the only maternal or infant characteristic significantly associated with breastfeeding outcomes, and only with initiation. The proportion of respondents who initiated breastfeeding increased across age categories (p = 0.005), although the oldest age group had lower levels than those aged 35–39 years (91.7% versus 98.3%). Parity was associated with duration of exclusive breastfeeding but only at the 10% level, with mothers with two or more children exclusively breastfeeding for longer on average than those with just one child (26.6 weeks [IQR 22.1] versus 18 weeks [IQR 24.6], p = 0.083).
Education was the only socioeconomic status indicator to be significantly associated with breastfeeding outcomes. Education had a dose-response association with initiation and duration of any breastfeeding: mothers with GCSEs or AS/A-levels, graduate qualifications and postgraduate qualifications had a 89.4%, 90.4% and 98.6% chance of initiating breastfeeding, respectively (p < 0.001), and their median durations of any breastfeeding were 19.5 (IQR 36.5), 27.9 (IQR 35.9), and 38.7 (IQR 41.0) weeks, respectively (p < 0.001). Post-hoc Dunn tests showed that only mothers with GCSEs or AS/A-levels versus graduate qualifications (p < 0.001) and mothers with graduate qualifications versus postgraduate qualifications (p = 0.006) had significantly different median durations of any breastfeeding. Median durations of exclusive breastfeeding were similar for mothers with graduate (26.6 weeks [QR 23.6]) and postgraduate qualifications (26.3 weeks [IQR 22.6]), with post-hoc Dunn tests confirming that the only significant differences were between mothers with GCSEs or AS/A-levels (8.0 weeks [IQR 25.1]) and those with postgraduate qualifications (p = 0.003).

3.2. Model Results

The results of the Cox-Aalen models are shown in Table 2 and Table 3 for any and exclusive breastfeeding, respectively. These tables show results for models controlling for just age and parity (M1) and additionally education (M2), and provide hazard ratios for when environmental experiences were set as constant effects and Supremum-tests of significance and Kolmogorov-Smirnov and Cramer von Mises tests for time-varying effects when environmental experiences were set as time-varying.
M1 graphs (adjusting for maternal age and parity) and M2 graphs (adjusting additionally for education) varied little from one another, with cumulative coefficients generally moving just a little more in the negative direction after adjusting for SES. This suggests that once education differentials were accounted for, women’s hazards of stopping breastfeeding reduced only slightly. We focus on the M2 graphs here (Figure 1, Figure 2, Figure 3, Figure 4 and Figure 5) and include the M1 graphs in the Appendix A (Figure A1, Figure A2, Figure A3, Figure A4 and Figure A5). These graphs can be interpreted as solid lines below zero representing a reduced hazard of stopping breastfeeding and solid lines above zero representing an increased hazard. Flat lines represent steady impact over time, lines going downwards represent a declining hazard, lines going upwards represent an increasing hazard. When lines are near zero there is a negligible effect, and where the confidence interval band (dotted lines) includes zero, there is no significant effect of the environmental factor at that point in time.
We now discuss findings for each of the five environmental indicators in turn.

3.2.1. My Area Is a Nice Place to Live

Under the constant effect (i.e., proportional hazards) setting and controlling for maternal age and parity (M1), mothers who disagreed that their area was a nice place to live were 1.44 times as likely to stop any breastfeeding and this reduced to 1.26 times once education was controlled for (M2).
When the proportional hazards assumption was relaxed and Nice was instead set as an additive time-varying effect, we find weak evidence (i.e., at the 10% level) that this indicator had a significant effect on chances of stopping any breastfeeding both before and after adjusting for SES (M1 Supremum-test p = 0.069, M2 Supremum-test p = 0.066). The Kolmogorov-Smirnov and Cramer von Mises tests did not provide any evidence of time-varying effects. This is confirmed by Figure 1a which has confidence intervals crossing zero throughout. The general trajectory suggests that, albeit non-significantly, compared to mothers who agreed that their area was a nice place to live, those who disagreed generally had increased hazards of stopping breastfeeding but that this fluctuated over time, with reduced hazards observed in the first 5 weeks, between weeks 10 and 15, and in weeks 20–40 (4–9 months).
For exclusive breastfeeding, constant effects were non-significant and only went in the predicted direction before controlling for SES, with mothers who disagreed that their area was a nice place to live having 1.03 times the risk of stopping exclusive breastfeeding compared to those that agreed. The direction of association reversed once education was controlled for, with mothers who disagreed now having reduced chances of stopping exclusive breastfeeding (HR 0.86).
When the proportional hazards assumption was relaxed and Nice was set as a time-varying effect, whether mothers thought their area was a nice place to live significantly predicted the chances of stopping exclusively breastfeeding, both before and after controlling for SES (M1 p = 0.017, M2 p = 0.032). Figure 1b shows that there was a small window within the first week postpartum where mothers who disagreed that their area was a nice place to live had significantly reduced hazards of stopping exclusive breastfeeding. Non-significantly reduced hazards were also observed in the first 3 weeks and between 6–13.5 weeks.

3.2.2. Crime Is a Problem in My Area

Under the constant effect setting and controlling for maternal age and parity (M1), mothers who thought that crime was a problem in their area were (non-significantly) 1.15 times as likely to stop any breastfeeding and this reduced to 1.12 times once education was controlled for (M2).
Although there were no significant constant effects, when the proportional hazards assumption was relaxed, Crime was found to significantly predict the chances of stopping any breastfeeding, both before (M1 Supremum-test p = 0.006) and after (M2 Supremum-test p = 0.002) controlling for SES. However, the high p-values of the Kolmogorov-Smirnov and Cramer von Mises score processes tests for constant effects suggested that hazards did not significantly vary over time. However, Figure 2a shows that mothers who thought that crime was a problem in their area had significantly reduced hazards of stopping any breastfeeding in the first 10 weeks postpartum. Although confidence intervals cross zero, mothers who considered crime as a problem in their area generally had increased hazards of stopping breastfeeding at later time points.
For exclusive breastfeeding, although constant effects were non-significant they went in the opposite to predicted direction both before and after controlling for SES, with mothers who thought that crime was a problem in their area having reduced chances of stopping exclusive breastfeeding (M1 HR 0.91, M2 HR 0.83).
The Supremum-tests suggest that crime did not have a significant effect on exclusive breastfeeding chances when the proportional hazards assumption was relaxed and crime was set as a time-varying effect. The Kolmogorov-Smirnov and Cramer von Mises score processes tests also suggested that hazards did not significantly vary over time. However, Figure 2b shows that mothers who thought that crime was a problem in their area had significantly reduced hazards of exclusive breastfeeding between weeks 1 and 2 postpartum. Although confidence intervals cross zero, mothers who considered crime as a problem in their area generally had reduced hazards of stopping exclusive breastfeeding at most time-points.

3.2.3. Littering and Rubbish Is a Problem in My Area

Mothers who thought that littering and rubbish was a problem in their area had a slightly increased but non-significant hazard of stopping any breastfeeding when controlling for age and parity (M1 HR 1.09) and this reduced slightly after controlling for SES (M2 HR 1.05).
Litter was also not significantly associated with stopping any breastfeeding in the non-proportional hazards setting and Kolmogorov-Smirnov and Cramer von Mises score processes tests found no evidence of time-varying effects.
However, Figure 3a shows that mothers who thought that littering and rubbish was a problem had the predicted increased hazard of stopping any breastfeeding between weeks 53 and 58 (12–13 months) and from week 66 (month 15) onwards, and they were significantly more likely to stop breastfeeding from week 80 (18 months). They did however have almost as much time with reduced hazards.
Mothers who thought that littering and rubbish was a problem in their area had reduced hazards of stopping exclusive breastfeeding in the constant effects models, and this effect approached significance when SES was controlled for (M2 0.72, p = 0.061).
The Supremum-tests suggest that litter had a significant effect on exclusive breastfeeding when its effect was set as time-varying (M1 p < 0.001, M2 p = 0.002). The high p-values of the Kolmogorov-Smirnov and Cramer von Mises score processes tests suggest that hazards did not significantly vary over time but Figure 3b shows a clear time-based effect. The hazards of stopping exclusive breastfeeding declined over time, with mothers who thought that littering and rubbish was a problem having significantly reduced hazards at all time-points except between 2 and 3 weeks.

3.2.4. People Tend to Know Each Other in My Area

In the constant effects setting, mothers who reported that people tended not to know each other had non-significantly increased hazards of stopping any breastfeeding (M2 HR 1.16) which increased slightly after adjusting for SES (M2 HR 1.18).
In the time-varying setting, Supremum-tests and Kolmogorov-Smirnov and Cramer von Mises tests showed that whether mothers reported that people knew each other had no significant effect and no time-varying effect on any breastfeeding, respectively. However, inspection of Figure 4a shows that these mothers had reduced hazards of stopping any breastfeeding in the first 5 weeks (significantly so around week 2), at 6–8 weeks and between weeks 58 and 62 (13–14 months). Their hazards were otherwise generally increased, and significantly so from week 75 (17 months), compared to mothers who thought that people tended to know each other.
When the effects were set as constant, mothers who thought that people tended not to know each other had non-significantly reduced hazards of stopping exclusive breastfeeding (M1 HR 0.90, M2 HR 0.87). When effects were set as time-varying, we found no evidence of a significant or time-varying effect on exclusive breastfeeding. However, Figure 4b shows that hazards of stopping exclusive breastfeeding were generally reduced and significantly so for a few days around 1 week postpartum; hazards were only increased around weeks 3–4, 4.5–6 weeks, 10–11.5 weeks, and weeks 12–17.5.

3.2.5. In General, People Help Each Other in My Area

Whether mothers thought that people helped each other out in their area did not significantly predict chances of any breastfeeding in the constant effect setting, although associations were in the predicted direction (M1 HR 1.31, M2 HR 1.23).
Supremum-tests and Kolmogorov-Smirnov and Cramer von Mises tests showed that whether mothers thought that people helped each other had no significant effect and no time-varying effect on any breastfeeding, respectively. Figure 5a does however show that mothers who thought that people helped each other out in their area had significantly reduced hazards of stopping any breastfeeding in the first 2 weeks. Hazards started to be increased (albeit non-significantly) from around week 10, staying increased for the remainder of women’s infant feeding journeys except during weeks 58–62 (13–14 months). Although the increased hazards were non-significant, the lower confidence band approaches zero from 75 weeks (17 months).
Whether mothers thought that people helped each other out was associated with reduced hazards of stopping exclusive breastfeeding in the constant effects models, approaching significance once SES was controlled for (M2 HR 0.65, p = 0.081).
Supremum-tests and Kolmogorov-Smirnov and Cramer von Mises tests showed no significant or time-varying effects in the non-proportional hazards setting. However, Figure 5b shows hazards of stopping exclusive breastfeeding were consistently reduced for mothers who thought that people did not help each other out, with a significant reduction seen for a few days around 1 week postpartum.
Overall, these findings suggest that generally the risks of stopping any breastfeeding increase with time (but after a while) and the risks of stopping exclusive breastfeeding decrease steadily over time.

4. Discussion

Given the literature on environmental effects on parenting, reproduction and health, we would expect mothers to stop breastfeeding sooner when they perceive their environments less favourably. We found limited support for this hypothesis. Women’s hazards of stopping both any and exclusive breastfeeding varied over time, with subjective environmental experiences only being associated with breastfeeding chances at specific timepoints, and not always in the predicted direction. Whilst results were not necessarily significant according to the conventional alpha cut-off of 0.05, consistent trends highlight that real effects may exist and are important to consider [58,59].
Our findings suggest that the first few weeks postpartum were most susceptible to environmental influences and that contrary to our predictions, mothers with worse subjective environmental experiences were less likely to stop breastfeeding at this time. Mothers who thought that littering and rubbish was a problem in their area also showed a declining hazard of stopping exclusive breastfeeding over time. The predicted increased hazard of stopping breastfeeding was only evident in the later stages of any breastfeeding and only for mothers who reported littering as a problem or that people tended not to know each other. We now discuss each of these key findings in turn before considering study limitations and proposing ideas for future research.
The reduced hazards of stopping breastfeeding in the first few weeks postpartum were evident for all five of our environmental indicators to varying degrees, with mothers who thought that crime was a problem in their area having the most pronounced effects. Most crime and health research focuses on the health consequences of directly experiencing crime; victims of violent crime suffer a range of health problems. The initial acute physical injury, increased stress and increased risk of mental health problems can lead to chronic physical injury, impaired immune system functioning, increased health risk behaviour and inappropriate health care utilisation, all resulting in further increased risks of health problems [60]. Research explicitly linking crime and breastfeeding is largely limited to the effects of experiencing domestic violence; the evidence is inconclusive but suggests that many mothers who have experienced abuse may be less likely to breastfeed [52,53,54,55]. Direct experience of crime and domestic violence may have negative health impacts and reduce women’s breastfeeding chances, but what about crime perception and crime outside the home?
Just as agreement between objective and subjective measures of environmental quality tends to be only low to moderate [32], fear of crime is not just a response to high crime rates with the two tending to be only weakly correlated [39]. This apparent disentangling of subjective and objective assessments of crime seems to be driven by individual factors. For example, women tend to fear crime to a greater extent than men (and to fear different types of crime). Furthermore, vicarious fear expressed by husbands or partners may lead to restrictions on women’s activities [39]. Other factors influencing fear of crime are previous direct experience of crime, mental health, ethnicity, and age [39]. Whilst we had data on these last two factors, the vast majority of our sample of mothers were White and so fears specifically related to racially motivated crimes are unlikely to be relevant for this sample.
Regardless of the extent to which it correlates with objective crime statistics or other individual factors, fear of crime has been to shown to have its own negative health impacts [39,61]. Pleasant neighbourhoods are associated with better wellbeing and feelings of safety whereas unsafe neighbourhoods may discourage social interaction [39], potentially negatively impacting breastfeeding which tends to be more successful with adequate social support [43,62,63,64]. Furthermore, social norms discouraging breastfeeding may prevail in disadvantaged environments [65], so any social interaction that does occur may reduce breastfeeding chances.
Fear of crime can lead to avoidance behaviours including self-isolation [61] thus we can speculate that new mothers scared of crime may feel safer staying at home, and staying at home in the first few weeks postpartum may help with infant bonding and establishing successful breastfeeding [66]. Avoidance behaviours coupled with the protective effect of “nesting in” with a newborn may also explain the reduced hazards of stopping breastfeeding seen for some of the other environmental experience measures, such as whether mothers thought their area was a nice place to live or whether littering and rubbish was a problem, which both conferred reduced hazards of stopping exclusive breastfeeding early on. Of course if mothers are experiencing breastfeeding difficulties, then they may benefit from seeking social support [4], but friends, family and health professionals could make home visits if mothers did not want to leave the house, and many services are accessible on the phone or via the internet. The reduced hazards of stopping breastfeeding in the early weeks for those mothers that report that people do not tend to know or help each other in their area does however suggest that this kind of social support, whether or not provided at home, may be coming from people outside the areas where mothers live. This seems plausible given that it is common for mothers, especially those of higher SES, to live far away from their own parents [67].
Our questionnaire asked mothers whether they thought crime was a problem in their area, which perhaps may not be addressing fear of crime itself. Mothers may have thought crime was a problem without being scared to go out and we do not know which crimes they were considering as problematic. Similarly, the type and extent of littering and rubbish is unknown, but we can imagine that in this relatively high SES sample, the kinds of crimes, littering and rubbish considered to be problematic are likely to differ from those in more deprived areas. Avoidance behaviour may however only makes sense in extreme cases of environmental contamination and could be a less likely explanation for the persistently declining hazard of stopping exclusive breastfeeding seen amongst mothers who considered littering and rubbish to be a problem. What then might explain this finding?
Given that the trajectories of any and exclusive breastfeeding differ so greatly for mothers’ perceptions of littering and rubbish, with the former showing no association except in the later stages of breastfeeding and the latter showing a clear reduction in hazards over time, we can speculate that there may be important differences between mothers who breastfeed exclusively and those who combine breastfeeding with formula. The exclusive breastfeeding models remove mothers who used mixed methods of infant feeding, and so the differently shaped graphs could suggest that mixed-feeding mothers’ breastfeeding chances were either unaffected by their perception of littering, or that they were more likely to stop any breastfeeding when they perceived littering and rubbish as a problem. In other words, formula feeding may be confounding the association. This could mean that, for example, littering and rubbish may be more of a deterrent to formula feeding in environments perceived as dirty, with mothers concerned that there is nowhere clean to prepare formula. Less deprived areas, which likely correspond with being a nicer place to live, with less litter, crime etc., tend to have more “mummy cafes” with facilities for preparing formula feeds as well as being a more friendly place to breastfeed. On the other hand, in more deprived areas, beyond supermarket cafes and big chain restaurants, facilities for formula feeding are lacking. Breastfeeding may therefore be perceived as more convenient [68], although mothers may be more concerned that they have nowhere sanitary to sit whilst breastfeeding [69,70]. Another potential explanation is that the littering and rubbish question may be acting as a proxy for other aspects of the environment, which may be behind the observed associations. For example, when considered as a physical incivility, littering may be signaling antisocial behaviour, which may explain why it has been found to correlate with increased fear of crime in other studies [39]. Littering and rubbish may also correspond with other physical environmental issues such as levels of air and water pollution and together these broader environmental quality concerns may be driving women to stay home and avoid these exposures [10]. Alternatively, as breastfeeding provides antioxidative protection against some of the detrimental health impacts of prenatal environmental contaminant exposure [71,72,73,74], mothers who are aware of these benefits may make a conscious decision to continue breastfeeding to protect their infants from environmental harm. It is helpful to think through what might explain the observed associations but these are of course just speculations that go beyond the capabilities of our quantitative data; qualitative research is needed to unpack some of the underlying mechanisms and to understand how these environmental factors play out alongside sociocultural, policy and marketing drivers.
Whilst littering and rubbish was associated with reduced hazards of stopping exclusive breastfeeding, it was associated with increased hazards of stopping any breastfeeding from around week 75 (17 months). Whilst mothers are more likely to give up breastfeeding in the early weeks if they’re struggling, logically they are increasingly likely to stop breastfeeding as time goes on, as they transition to introducing their infants to complementary foods. It may just take one small push for a mother to stop breastfeeding at this stage, and perhaps a (perceived) dirty environment is enough for a wavering mother to stop. A perceived lack of neighbourhood friendliness and support (as indexed by whether mothers thought people tended to know or help each other in their area) may similarly be enough to push mothers to stop breastfeeding at the later stages. In the UK, breastfeeding outside the home is still difficult for many mothers [75,76] and negative reactions from the public are common [77], especially if the child is older [78]. Perceiving others as unkind or unhelpful may exacerbate women’s concerns over breastfeeding outside the home.

Limitations and Recommendations for Further Research

There are many factors that affect women’s infant feeding decisions. This survey was conducted to primarily explore the role of social support in shaping women’s infant feeding experiences and some of our other research using this dataset explores this further [43]. This paper presents a secondary analysis of the data exploring different drivers of infant feeding behaviour but does not preclude the role of other important sociocultural drivers and contextual factors.
Our analytical sample size combined with limited variation in exposures and outcomes means we are only likely to identify medium and large effect sizes at p ≤ 0.05. Given the relatively consistent direction of effect (within any and exclusive breastfeeding), our null results in the constant effect (proportional hazards) models may suggest a power issue. A larger sample size may therefore mitigate against the risk of a Type II error. However, as the cumulative coefficient graphs suggested, perhaps relationships between environmental factors and breastfeeding outcomes are better described by time-varying effects.
In addition, our results are unlikely to be representative. Mothers were recruited online, primarily via Facebook groups which have biased our sample towards higher SES individuals. Studies have shown that social media survey recruitment can lead to an increased proportion of middle-class participants; however, this trend is not consistent, and it can still be an effective way to enroll “hard-to-reach” populations [79], especially through tailored and targeted recruitment (for further discussion see [43]). However, the high SES bias in our sample is evident in the descriptive statistics in Table 1, where education and annual income levels are far greater than country averages for the same time period [80,81]. Our findings are therefore only exploratory and descriptive in nature and not generalizable to the broader general UK population of new mothers; larger, more representative samples are needed to verify our results. Further research could replicate this study with more representative samples with more diverse SES and environmental experiences, including in non-WEIRD (Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich and Democratic) populations [82].
We do not know what women were doing in the “Any breastfeeding” category other than that they were giving their infants some breastmilk, so future research, including using qualitative methodology, could look at whether subjective environmental experiences predict patterns of feeding behaviour within this group. It would also be interesting to explore how subjective environmental experiences map onto objective measures of environmental quality to see how accurate women’s risk perceptions are. For example, how mothers’ crime perception maps onto local crime statistics. This was not possible in the current study as we only had the region in which mothers lived; future studies could collect postcode information to match up smaller scale geographical information with women’s subjective environmental experiences. Given that SES has been shown to buffer against environmental effects on breastfeeding [9], further research could also explore how women’s subjective environmental experiences interact with socioeconomic status and other individual characteristics such as ethnicity (and prior victimization in the case of crime) to impact breastfeeding chances (we attempted to explore interactions between SES and the five environmental measures in this study but small numbers in combinatorial categories prevented the models from converging. This kind of analysis would require larger sample sizes). There is also a need for more qualitative research to explore the role of environmental perception in women’s infant feeding decisions further. Possible questions to address include: What crimes are perceived as a problem? Does littering and rubbish lead to avoidance behaviour? Do women recognise these as threatening breastfeeding? Or are there other indirect links to breastfeeding outcomes?

5. Conclusions

Our study has highlighted that even in this relatively homogenous high SES sample, women’s hazards of stopping breastfeeding were not constant over time and there were particular timepoints in their breastfeeding journeys where subjective environmental experiences may have played a role. In particular we found that contrary to our predictions, women with worse subjective environmental experiences were less likely to stop breastfeeding in the first few weeks postpartum, especially women who perceived crime as a problem in their area. We also found that women who thought that littering and rubbish was a problem in their area were progressively less and less likely to stop exclusive breastfeeding. Lastly, the increased hazards from around 75 weeks (17 months) onwards for mothers who perceived littering and rubbish as a problem or who thought that people tended not to know each other was the only evidence we found in support of our prediction that mothers who perceived their environments unfavourably would have lower breastfeeding chances.
Further research with larger, more diverse and representative samples is warranted to explore these associations further. It would also be helpful to conduct qualitative research to understand whether mothers deliberately alter their infant feeding behavior in response to environmental threats or whether other mechanisms are at play.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, L.J.B., methodology, L.J.B.; validation, L.J.B., S.M., A.E.P. and E.H.E.; formal analysis, L.J.B.; investigation, S.M., A.E.P. and E.H.E.; data curation, S.M., A.E.P., E.H.E.; writing—original draft preparation, L.J.B.; writing—review and editing, S.M., A.E.P., and E.H.E.; visualization, L.J.B.; project administration, S.M., A.E.P. and E.H.E.; funding acquisition, A.E.P. and L.J.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The original study was part-funded by the Medical Research Council as part of Abigail E. Page’s Postdoctoral Fellowship (grant number MR/P014216/1), and this secondary analysis was funded by the UK’s Economic and Social Research Council, as part of Laura J Brown’s Postdoctoral Fellowship (grant number ES/T008296/1).

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all our participants who took part in our survey, who took their time to share important information about their lives with us. We would also like to thank Susie Schaffnit and Eve Black, who tested, reviewed and provided feedback on the original survey. Thanks also to three anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

Appendix A

Table A1. Cox and Aalen model diagnostics testing for proportional hazards assumption and time invariant effects to determine whether to set covariate effects as time-varying in main models.
Table A1. Cox and Aalen model diagnostics testing for proportional hazards assumption and time invariant effects to determine whether to set covariate effects as time-varying in main models.
CovariatesAny Breastfeeding (n = 480)Exclusive Breastfeeding (n = 293)
Cox Model:
Test for PH
Aalen Model:
Tests for Time-Invariant Effects
Cox Model:
Test for PH
Aalen Model:
Tests for Time-Invariant Effects
rhoChi2p-ValueK-S TestK-S p-ValueC TestC p-ValuerhoChi2p-ValueK-S testK-S p-ValueC TestC p-Value
Maternal age (years): ref. = 16–24
25–290.1439.0100.003114.00.001365,000.0<0.0010.0040.0030.95413.50.173769.00.238
30–340.15211.4900.001112.0<0.001419,000.0<0.0010.0200.0850.77121.00.0262240.00.030
35–390.17720.470<0.001121.0<0.001438,000.0<0.0010.0270.2480.61824.20.0083310.00.007
40–440.15211.0700.001135.00.001596,000.0<0.001−0.1303.5410.06046.7<0.00118,300.0<0.001
Parity: ref. = 1st birth
2nd +−0.0430.1900.66318.40.4526500.00.3960.0270.1450.7043.50.24154.10.313
Education: ref: = GCSEs or AS/A–levels
Graduate0.17714.610<0.00143.20.07856,000.00.044−0.0140.0310.8614.40.244154.00.105
Postgrad0.1255.6900.01739.90.02960,000.00.0030.0440.4930.4834.00.28094.60.197
PH: Proportional Hazards. K-S: Kolmogorov-Smirnov. CvM: Cramer von Mises test. p-Values below 0.05 indicate evidence against the null of proportional hazards and time-invariant effects. Covariates were considered non-proportional/time-varying where at least two of the three tests provided evidence against the null for the majority of the covariate’s categories.
Figure A1. Cox-Aalen model results for “My area is a nice place to live” (No vs. Yes) for any (a) and exclusive breastfeeding (b). Main effect set as time-varying, and models adjusted for maternal age and parity (M1). Estimated cumulative coefficients (solid line) with 95% pointwise confidence intervals (dotted lines).
Figure A1. Cox-Aalen model results for “My area is a nice place to live” (No vs. Yes) for any (a) and exclusive breastfeeding (b). Main effect set as time-varying, and models adjusted for maternal age and parity (M1). Estimated cumulative coefficients (solid line) with 95% pointwise confidence intervals (dotted lines).
Ijerph 17 07903 g0a1
Figure A2. Cox-Aalen model results for “Crime is a problem in my area” (Yes vs. No) for any (a) and exclusive breastfeeding (b). Main effect set as time-varying, and models adjusted for maternal age and parity (M1). Estimated cumulative coefficients (solid line) with 95% pointwise confidence intervals (dotted lines).
Figure A2. Cox-Aalen model results for “Crime is a problem in my area” (Yes vs. No) for any (a) and exclusive breastfeeding (b). Main effect set as time-varying, and models adjusted for maternal age and parity (M1). Estimated cumulative coefficients (solid line) with 95% pointwise confidence intervals (dotted lines).
Ijerph 17 07903 g0a2
Figure A3. Cox-Aalen model results for “Littering and rubbish is a problem in my area” (Yes vs. No) for any (a) and exclusive breastfeeding (b). Main effect set as time-varying, and models adjusted for maternal age and parity (M1). Estimated cumulative coefficients (solid line) with 95% pointwise confidence intervals (dotted lines).
Figure A3. Cox-Aalen model results for “Littering and rubbish is a problem in my area” (Yes vs. No) for any (a) and exclusive breastfeeding (b). Main effect set as time-varying, and models adjusted for maternal age and parity (M1). Estimated cumulative coefficients (solid line) with 95% pointwise confidence intervals (dotted lines).
Ijerph 17 07903 g0a3
Figure A4. Cox-Aalen model results for “People tend to know each other in my area” (No vs. Yes) for any (a) and exclusive breastfeeding (b). Main effect set as time-varying, and models adjusted for maternal age and parity (M1). Estimated cumulative coefficients (solid line) with 95% pointwise confidence intervals (dotted lines).
Figure A4. Cox-Aalen model results for “People tend to know each other in my area” (No vs. Yes) for any (a) and exclusive breastfeeding (b). Main effect set as time-varying, and models adjusted for maternal age and parity (M1). Estimated cumulative coefficients (solid line) with 95% pointwise confidence intervals (dotted lines).
Ijerph 17 07903 g0a4
Figure A5. Cox-Aalen model results for “In general, people help each other out in my area” (No vs. Yes) for any (a) and exclusive breastfeeding (b). Main effect set as time-varying, and models adjusted for maternal age and parity (M1). Estimated cumulative coefficients (solid line) with 95% pointwise confidence intervals (dotted lines).
Figure A5. Cox-Aalen model results for “In general, people help each other out in my area” (No vs. Yes) for any (a) and exclusive breastfeeding (b). Main effect set as time-varying, and models adjusted for maternal age and parity (M1). Estimated cumulative coefficients (solid line) with 95% pointwise confidence intervals (dotted lines).
Ijerph 17 07903 g0a5

References

  1. Victora, C.G.; Bahl, R.; Barros, A.J.D.; França, G.V.; Horton, S.; Krasevec, J.; Murch, S.; Sankar, M.J.; Walker, N.; Rollins, N.C. Breastfeeding in the 21st century: Epidemiology, mechanisms, and lifelong effect. Lancet 2016, 387, 475–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. McAndrew, F.; Thompson, J.; Fellows, L.; Large, A.; Speed, M.; Renfrew, M.J. Infant Feeding Survey 2010; Health and Social Care Information Centre: Leeds, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  3. Binns, C.; Lee, M.; Low, W.Y. The long-term public health benefits of breastfeeding. Asia Pac. J. Public Health 2016, 28, 7–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Brown, A. Breastfeeding as a public health responsibility: A review of the evidence. J. Hum. Nutr. Diet 2017, 30, 759–770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Tomori, C.; Palmquist, A.E.L.; Quinn, E.A. Introduction: Towards multidisciplinary anthropologies of breastfeeding. In Breastfeeding: New Anthropological Approaches, 1st ed.; Tomori, C., Palmquist, A.E.L., Quinn, E.A., Eds.; Routledge: Oxford, UK; New York, NY, USA, 2017; pp. 1–25. [Google Scholar]
  6. Emmott, E.H.; Mace, R. Practical support from fathers and grandmothers is associated with lower levels of breastfeeding in the UK Millennium Cohort study. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0133547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Gartner, L.M.; Morton, J.; Lawrence, R.A.; Naylor, A.J.; O’Hare, D.; Schanler, R.J.; Eidelman, A.I.; American Academy of Pediatrics Section on Breastfeeding. Breastfeeding and the use of human milk. Pediatrics 2005, 115, 496–506. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  8. Condon, L.; Ingram, J. Increasing support for breastfeeding: What can Children’s Centres do? Health Soc. Care Community 2011, 19, 617–625. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Brown, L.J.; Sear, R. Local environmental quality positively predicts breastfeeding in the UK’s Millennium Cohort Study. Evol. Med. Public Health 2017, 2017, 120–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Brown, L.J.; Sear, R. Are mothers less likely to breastfeed in harsh environments? Physical environmental quality and breastfeeding in the Born in Bradford study. Matern. Child Nutr. 2019, 15, e12851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Quinlan, R.J. Extrinsic mortality effects on reproductive strategies in a Caribbean community. Hum. Nat. 2010, 21, 124–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Brown, A.; Raynor, P.; Benton, D.; Lee, M.D. Indices of Multiple Deprivation predict breastfeeding duration in England and Wales. Eur. J. Public Health 2009, 20, 231–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Nettle, D. Dying young and living fast: Variation in life history across English neighborhoods. Behav. Ecol. 2010, 21, 387–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Peregrino, A.B.; Watt, R.G.; Heilmann, A.; Jivraj, S. Breastfeeding practices in the United Kingdom: Is the neighbourhood context important? Matern. Child Nutr. 2018, 14, e12626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Roux, A.-V.D. Neighborhoods and health: Where are we and were do we go from here? Rev. d’Épidémiologie St. Publique 2007, 55, 13–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Park, M.; Verhoeven, J.E.; Cuijpers, P.; Iii, C.F.R.; Penninx, B.W.J.H. Where you live may make you old: The Association between perceived poor neighborhood quality and leukocyte telomere length. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0128460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Pepper, G.V.; Nettle, D. Socioeconomic disparities in health behaviour: An evolutionary perspective. In Applied Evolutionary Anthropology: Darwinian Approaches to Contemporary World Issues; Gibson, M.A., Lawson, D.W., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2014; pp. 225–243. [Google Scholar]
  18. Galea, S.; Ahern, J.; Nandi, A.; Tracy, M.; Beard, J.R.; Vlahov, D. Urban neighborhood poverty and the incidence of depression in a population-based cohort study. Ann. Epidemiol. 2007, 17, 171–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  19. Robinette, J.W.; Charles, S.T.; Mogle, J.A.; Almeida, D.M. Neighborhood cohesion and daily well-being: Results from a diary study. Soc. Sci. Med. 2013, 96, 174–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  20. Pearl, M.; Braveman, P.; Abrams, B. The relationship of neighborhood sociodemographic characteristics to birthweight among five ethnic groups in California. Am. J. Public Health 2001, 91, 1808–1824. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Schempf, A.; Strobino, D.; O’Campo, P. Neighborhood effects on birthweight: An exploration of psychosocial and behavioral pathways in Baltimore, 1995–1996. Soc. Sci. Med. 2009, 68, 100–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Agyemang, C.; Vrijkotte, T.; Droomers, M.; Van Der Wal, M.F.; Bonsel, G.; Stronks, K. The effect of neighbourhood income and deprivation on pregnancy outcomes in Amsterdam, The Netherlands. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2009, 63, 755–760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Morello-Frosch, R.; Shenassa, E.D. The environmental ‘Riskscape’ and social inequality: Implications for explaining maternal and child health disparities. Environ. Health Perspect. 2006, 114, 1150–1153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Leventhal, T.; Brooks-Gunn, J. The neighborhoods they live in: The effects of neighborhood residence on child and adolescent outcomes. Psychol. Bull. 2000, 126, 309–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Earls, F.; McGuire, J.; Shay, S. Evaluating a community intervention to reduce the risk of child abuse: Methodological strategies in conducting neighborhood surveys. Child Abus. Negl. 1994, 18, 473–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Department for Communities and Local Government. The English Indices of Deprivation. GOV.UK. 2011. Available online: https://www.gov.uk/government/collections/english-indices-of-deprivation (accessed on 27 September 2020).
  27. Oakley, L.L.; Kurinczuk, J.J.; Renfrew, M.J.; Quigley, M. Breastfeeding in England: Time trends 2005–2006 to 2012–2013 and inequalities by area profile. Matern. Child Nutr. 2014, 12, 440–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Agboado, G.; Michel, E.; Jackson, E.; Verma, A. Factors associated with breastfeeding cessation in nursing mothers in a peer support programme in Eastern Lancashire. BMC Pediatr. 2010, 10, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  29. Carter, S.; Williams, M.; Paterson, J.; Iusitini, L. Do perceptions of neighbourhood problems contribute to maternal health?: Findings from the Pacific Islands Families study. Health Place 2009, 15, 622–630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Curtis, S.; Southall, H.; Congdon, P.; Dodgeon, B. Area effects on health variation over the life-course: Analysis of the longitudinal study sample in England using new data on area of residence in childhood. Soc. Sci. Med. 2004, 58, 57–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Auger, N.; Daniel, M.; Platt, R.W.; Wu, Y.; Luo, Z.-C.; Choinière, R. Association between perceived security of the neighbourhood and small-for-gestational-age birth. Paediatr. Périnat. Epidemiol. 2008, 22, 467–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Rollings, K.A.; Wells, N.M.; Evans, G.W. Measuring physical neighborhood quality related to health. Behav. Sci. 2015, 5, 190–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. O’Campo, P.; Dunn, J.R. Rethinking Social Epidemiology: Towards a Science of Change; Springer Science & Business Media: Berlin, Germany, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  34. Erickson, A.C. Person, Place and Context: The Interaction between the Social and Physical Environment on Adverse Pregnancy Outcomes in British Columbia; University of Victoria: Victoria, BC, Canada, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  35. Hystad, P.; Davies, H.W.; Frank, L.; Van Loon, J.; Gehring, U.; Tamburic, L.; Brauer, M. Residential greenness and birth outcomes: Evaluating the influence of spatially correlated built-environment factors. Environ. Health Perspect. 2014, 122, 1095–1102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Rydin, Y.; Bleahu, A.; Davies, M.; Dávila, J.D.; Friel, S.; De Grandis, G.; Groce, N.; Hallal, P.C.; Hamilton, I.; Howden-Chapman, P.; et al. Shaping cities for health: Complexity and the planning of urban environments in the 21st century. Lancet 2012, 379, 2079–2108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Johns, S.E. Perceived environmental risk as a predictor of teenage motherhood in a British population. Health Place 2011, 17, 122–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Auger, N.; Park, A.L.; Gamache, P.; Pampalon, R.; Daniel, M. Weighing the contributions of material and social area deprivation to preterm birth. Soc. Sci. Med. 2012, 75, 1032–1037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Lorenc, T.; Petticrew, M.P.; Whitehead, M.; Neary, D.; Clayton, S.; Wright, K.; Thomson, H.; Cummins, S.; Sowden, A.; Renton, A. Fear of crime and the environment: Systematic review of UK qualitative evidence. BMC Public Health 2013, 13, 496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  40. Rosen-Carole, C.; Auinger, P.; Howard, C.R.; Brownell, E.A.; Lanphear, B.P. Low-level prenatal toxin exposures and breastfeeding duration: A prospective cohort study. Matern. Child Health J. 2017, 21, 2245–2255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Chisholm, J.S.; Quinlivan, J.A.; Petersen, R.W.; Coall, D.A. Early stress predicts age at menarche and first birth, adult attachment, and expected lifespan. Hum. Nat. 2005, 16, 233–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Geronimus, A.T.; James, S.A.; Destin, M.; Graham, L.F.; Hatzenbuehler, M.L.; Murphy, M.C.; Pearson, J.A.; Omari, A.; Thompson, J.P. Jedi public health: Co-creating an identity-safe culture to promote health equity. SSM Popul. Health 2016, 2, 105–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  43. Emmott, E.H.; Page, A.E.; Myers, S. Typologies of postnatal support and breastfeeding at two months in the UK. Soc. Sci. Med. 2020, 246, 112791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Etikan, I.; Musa, S.A.; Alkassim, R.S. Comparison of convenience sampling and purposive sampling. Am. J. Theor. Appl. Stat. 2016, 5, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Kramer, M.S.; Kakuma, R. The Optimal Duration of Exclusive Breastfeeding: A Systematic Review; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  46. Furman, L.; Minich, N.M.; Hack, M. Breastfeeding of very low birth weight infants. J. Hum. Lact. 1998, 14, 29–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Sloan, S.; Sneddon, H.; Stewart, M.; Iwaniec, D. Breast is best? Reasons why mothers decide to breastfeed or Bottlefeed their babies and factors influencing the duration of breastfeeding. Child Care Pract. 2006, 12, 283–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Kiernan, K.; Pickett, K.E. Marital status disparities in maternal smoking during pregnancy, breastfeeding and maternal depression. Soc. Sci. Med. 2006, 63, 335–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Kelly, Y.; Watt, R.G.; Nazroo, J. Racial/Ethnic differences in breastfeeding initiation and continuation in the United Kingdom and comparison with findings in the United States. Pediatrics 2006, 118, 1428–1435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  50. Awi, D.D.; Alikor, E.A.D. Barriers to timely initiation of breastfeeding among mothers of healthy full-term babies who deliver at the University of Port Harcourt Teaching Hospital. Niger. J. Clin. Pract. 2006, 9, 57–64. [Google Scholar]
  51. Diemer, M.A.; Mistry, R.S.; Wadsworth, M.E.; López, I.; Reimers, F. Best practices in conceptualizing and measuring social class in psychological research. Anal. Soc. Issues Public Policy 2012, 13, 77–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Dyson, L.; McCormick, F.; Renfrew, M.J. Interventions for promoting the initiation of breastfeeding. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2005, 2, CD001688. [Google Scholar]
  53. Arlotti, J.P.; Cottrell, B.H.; Lee, S.H.; Curtin, J.J. Breastfeeding among low-income women with and without peer support. J. Community Health Nurs. 1998, 15, 163–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Hackman, N.M.; Schaefer, E.W.; Beiler, J.S.; Rose, C.M.; Paul, I.M. Breastfeeding outcome comparison by parity. Breastfeed. Med. 2015, 10, 156–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Scheike, T.H.; Zhang, M.-J. Analyzing competing risk data using theRtimeregPackage. J. Stat. Softw. 2011, 38, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Hagar, Y.; Dukić, V. Comparison of hazard rate estimation in R. arXiv 2015, arXiv:1509.03253. [Google Scholar]
  57. Başar, E. Aalen’s additive, cox proportional hazards and the Cox-Aalen Model: Application to kidney transplant data. Sains Malays. 2017, 46, 469–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Amrhein, V.; Greenland, S.; Mcshane, B. Retire statistical significance. Nature 2019, 567, 305–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  59. Smith, R.J. P > 0.05: The incorrect interpretation of “not significant” results is a significant problem. Am. J. Phys. Anthr. 2020, 172, 521–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Resnick, H.S.; Acierno, R.; Kilpatrick, D.G. Health impact of interpersonal violence 2: Medical and mental health outcomes. Behav. Med. 1997, 23, 65–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Lorenc, T.; Clayton, S.; Neary, D.; Whitehead, M.; Petticrew, M.P.; Thomson, H.; Cummins, S.; Sowden, A.; Renton, A. Crime, fear of crime, environment, and mental health and wellbeing: Mapping review of theories and causal pathways. Health Place 2012, 18, 757–765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Kaunonen, M.; Hannula, L.; Tarkka, M.-T. A systematic review of peer support interventions for breastfeeding. J. Clin. Nurs. 2012, 21, 1943–1954. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Brown, A. What do women really want? Lessons for breastfeeding promotion and education. Breastfeed. Med. 2016, 11, 102–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  64. Hector, D.; King, L.; Webb, K.; Heywood, P. Factors affecting breastfeeding practices. Applying a conceptual framework. N. S. W. Public Health Bull. 2005, 16, 52–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Scott, J.; Mostyn, T. Women’s experiences of breastfeeding in a bottle-feeding culture. J. Hum. Lact. 2003, 19, 270–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Sandwell and West Birmingham NHS Trust. More Mums Breastfeeding Babies Despite Coronavirus. MEdia. 2020. Available online: https://www.swbh.nhs.uk/media/more-mums-breastfeeding-babies-despite-coronavirus/ (accessed on 27 September 2020).
  67. Chan, T.W.; Ermisch, J. Proximity of couples to parents: Influences of gender, labor market, and family. Demography 2015, 52, 379–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Synnott, K.; Bogue, J.; Edwards, C.A.; Scott, J.; Higgins, S.; Norin, E.; Frias, D.; Amarri, S.; Adam, R. Parental perceptions of feeding practices in five European countries: An exploratory study. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2007, 61, 946–956. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Battersby, S. Not in public please: Breastfeeding as dirty work in the UK. In Exploring the Dirty Side of Women’s Health; Kerkham, M., Ed.; Routledge: London, UK; New York, NY, USA, 2007; pp. 90–101. [Google Scholar]
  70. Williamson, I.R.; Sacranie, S.M. Nourishing body and spirit: Exploring British Muslim mothers’ constructions and experiences of breastfeeding. Divers. Equal. Health Care 2012, 9, 113–123. [Google Scholar]
  71. Guxens, M.; Ballester, F.; Espada, M.; Fernández, M.F.; Grimalt, J.; Ibarluzea, J.; Olea, N.; Rebagliato, M.; Tardón, A.; Torrent, M.; et al. Cohort profile: The INMA—INfancia y Medio Ambiente—(environment and childhood) project. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2011, 41, 930–940. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  72. Naz, S.; Page, A.; Agho, K.E. Household air pollution and under-five mortality in India (1992–2006). Environ. Health 2016, 15, 54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  73. Lertxundi, A.; Baccini, M.; Lertxundi, N.; Fano, E.; Aranbarri, A.; Martínez, M.D.; Ayerdi, M.; Álvarez, J.; Santa-Marina, L.; Dorronsoro, M.; et al. Exposure to fine particle matter, nitrogen dioxide and benzene during pregnancy and cognitive and psychomotor developments in children at 15 months of age. Environ. Int. 2015, 80, 33–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Shoji, H.; Koletzko, B. Oxidative stress and antioxidant protection in the perinatal period. Curr. Opin. Clin. Nutr. Metab. Care 2007, 10, 324–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Thomson, G.; Ebisch-Burton, K.; Flacking, R. Shame if you do—Shame if you don’t: Women’s experiences of infant feeding. Matern. Child Nutr. 2014, 11, 33–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Stewart-Knox, B.; Gardiner, K.; Wright, M. What is the problem with breast-feeding? A qualitative analysis of infant feeding perceptions. J. Hum. Nutr. Diet. 2003, 16, 265–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Grant, A. “I…don’t want to see you flashing your bits around”: Exhibitionism, othering and good motherhood in perceptions of public breastfeeding. Geoforum 2016, 71, 52–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Dowling, S.; Brown, A. An exploration of the experiences of mothers who breastfeed long-term: What are the issues and why does it matter? Breastfeed. Med. 2013, 8, 45–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  79. Topolovec-Vranic, J.; Natarajan, K. The use of social media in recruitment for medical research studies: A scoping review. J. Med. Internet Res. 2016, 18, e286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Office for National Statistics (ONS). Average Household Income, UK: Financial Year Ending 2019. 2020. Available online: https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/personalandhouseholdfinances/incomeandwealth/bulletins/householddisposableincomeandinequality/financialyearending2019 (accessed on 15 September 2020).
  81. HESA. Higher Education Student Statistics: UK, 2016/17—Qualifications Achieved. 2018. Available online: https://www.hesa.ac.uk/news/11-01-2018/sfr247-higher-education-student-statistics/qualifications (accessed on 15 September 2020).
  82. Henrich, J.; Heine, S.J.; Norenzayan, A. The weirdest people in the world? SSRN Electron. J. 2010, 33, 61–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Cox-Aalen model results for “My area is a nice place to live” (No vs. Yes) for any (a) and exclusive breastfeeding (b). Main effect set as time-varying, and models adjusted for maternal age, parity and education (M2). Estimated cumulative coefficients (solid line) with 95% pointwise confidence intervals (dotted lines).
Figure 1. Cox-Aalen model results for “My area is a nice place to live” (No vs. Yes) for any (a) and exclusive breastfeeding (b). Main effect set as time-varying, and models adjusted for maternal age, parity and education (M2). Estimated cumulative coefficients (solid line) with 95% pointwise confidence intervals (dotted lines).
Ijerph 17 07903 g001
Figure 2. Cox-Aalen model results for “Crime is a problem in my area” (Yes vs. No) for any (a) and exclusive breastfeeding (b). Main effect set as time-varying, and models adjusted for maternal age, parity and education (M2). Estimated cumulative coefficients (solid line) with 95% pointwise confidence intervals (dotted lines).
Figure 2. Cox-Aalen model results for “Crime is a problem in my area” (Yes vs. No) for any (a) and exclusive breastfeeding (b). Main effect set as time-varying, and models adjusted for maternal age, parity and education (M2). Estimated cumulative coefficients (solid line) with 95% pointwise confidence intervals (dotted lines).
Ijerph 17 07903 g002
Figure 3. Cox-Aalen model results for “Littering and rubbish is a problem in my area” (Yes vs. No) for any (a) and exclusive breastfeeding (b). Main effect set as time-varying, and models adjusted for maternal age, parity and education (M2). Estimated cumulative coefficients (solid line) with 95% pointwise confidence intervals (dotted lines).
Figure 3. Cox-Aalen model results for “Littering and rubbish is a problem in my area” (Yes vs. No) for any (a) and exclusive breastfeeding (b). Main effect set as time-varying, and models adjusted for maternal age, parity and education (M2). Estimated cumulative coefficients (solid line) with 95% pointwise confidence intervals (dotted lines).
Ijerph 17 07903 g003
Figure 4. Cox-Aalen model results for “People tend to know each other in my area” (No vs. Yes) for any (a) and exclusive breastfeeding (b). Main effect set as time-varying, and models adjusted for maternal age, parity and education (M2). Estimated cumulative coefficients (solid line) with 95% pointwise confidence intervals (dotted lines).
Figure 4. Cox-Aalen model results for “People tend to know each other in my area” (No vs. Yes) for any (a) and exclusive breastfeeding (b). Main effect set as time-varying, and models adjusted for maternal age, parity and education (M2). Estimated cumulative coefficients (solid line) with 95% pointwise confidence intervals (dotted lines).
Ijerph 17 07903 g004
Figure 5. Cox-Aalen model results for “In general, people help each other out in my area” (No vs. Yes) for any (a) and exclusive breastfeeding (b). Main effect set as time-varying, and models adjusted for maternal age, parity and education (M2). Estimated cumulative coefficients (solid line) with 95% pointwise confidence intervals (dotted lines).
Figure 5. Cox-Aalen model results for “In general, people help each other out in my area” (No vs. Yes) for any (a) and exclusive breastfeeding (b). Main effect set as time-varying, and models adjusted for maternal age, parity and education (M2). Estimated cumulative coefficients (solid line) with 95% pointwise confidence intervals (dotted lines).
Ijerph 17 07903 g005
Table 1. Characteristics of respondents and their association with breastfeeding outcomes.
Table 1. Characteristics of respondents and their association with breastfeeding outcomes.
Characteristics Breastfeeding Initiation
(n = 512)
Duration of Any Breastfeeding (weeks)
(n = 480)
Duration of Exclusive Breastfeeding (weeks)
(n = 293)
nn (%)p-Value 1nMedian (IQR)p-Value 3nMedian (IQR)p-Value 3
Subjective environmental experiences
My area is a nice place to live
Yes482453 (94.0%) 45331.7 (41.7) 27722.1 (23.7)
No2220 (90.9%)0.6382019.0 (34.3)0.1131313.3 (24.6)0.630
Missing87 (87.5%) 753.4 (49.6) 326.6 (0.0)
Crime is a problem in my area
No378355 (93.9%) 35531.0 (42.4) 21322.1 (24.6)
Yes5954 (91.5%)0.5645433.8 (44.2)0.3313722.1 (20.6)0.537
Missing7571 (94.7%) 7131.7 (44.3) 4317.7 (23.6)
Littering and rubbish is a problem in my area
No387364 (94.1%) 36431.4 (43.0) 22022.1 (24.6)
Yes10497 (93.3%)0.947 29729.4 (50.0)0.4866322.1 (19.6)0.179
Missing2119 (90.5%) 1934.0 (20.2) 1024.4 (8.3)
People tend to know each other in my area
Yes329308 (93.6%) 30834.3 (43.1) 18622.1 (24.4)
No121116 (95.9%)0.470 211627.6 (42.0)0.3137420.1 (23.6)0.882
Missing6256 (90.3%) 5632.4 (40.6) 3326.6 (21.6)
In general, people help each other out in my area
Yes361339 (93.9%) 33933.0 (41.4) 20522.1 (23.7)
No7771 (92.2%)0.6077128.0 (40.9)0.4194626.6 (24.3)0.596
Missing7470 (94.6%) 7027.2 (52.4) 4217.7 (23.3)
Area
England
East Midlands2116 (76.2%) 1627.4 (39.1) 1226.6 (17.8)
East of England3431 (91.2%) 3131.0 (27.6) 1826.6 (19.6)
South East8983 (93.3%) 8329.4 (51.1) 5117.7 (25.6)
Yorkshire &
The Humber
5754 (94.7%) 5435.5 (40.7) 3626.6 (20.6)
North East4543 (95.6%) 4326.3 (29.2) 2013.3 (25.2)
West Midlands2322 (95.7%) 2216.5 (40.6) 113.0 (16.0)
London6966 (95.7%) 6635.4 (37.0) 4624.4 (19.1)
North West6159 (96.7%) 5935.4 (46.6) 3821.1 (22.3)
South West5453 (98.1%) 5329.9 (42.7) 2619.9 (23.3)
Wales2118 (85.7%) 1833.2 (26.8) 1216.3 (22.8)
Scotland3229 (90.6%) 2930.3 (32.1) 2022.1 (23.8)
Northern Ireland66 (100.0%)0.607633.2 (44.3)0.92630.9 (13.1)0.439
Maternal and infant characteristics
Maternal age (years)
16–242420 (83.3%) 2024.4 (38.7) 96.0 (25.1)
25–299887 (88.8%) 8722.1 (44.1) 5512.0 (25.1)
30–34245232 (94.7%) 23234.5 (46.5) 14022.1 (23.9)
35–39121119 (98.3%) 11935.4 (34.6) 7626.6 (16.9)
40–442422 (91.7%)0.0052227.4 (44.5)0.1941326.6 (25.6)0.150
Parity
1st birth313292 (93.3%) 29231.0 (41.7) 17718.0 (24.6)
2nd or higher199188 (94.5%)0.726 218832.6 (44.5)0.92111626.6 (22.1)0.083
Ethnicity
White490459 (93.7%) 45931.7 (43.1) 28222.1 (23.6)
Other2221 (95.5%)1.0002129.4 (44.1)0.7101117.1 (25.4)0.724
Partnership status
Partnered501469 (93.6%) 46931.1 (43.1) 28822.1 (23.6)
Unpartnered1111 (100.0%)1.0001152.6 (41.9)0.295526.6 (25.6)0.993
Birthweight
Normal (≥2500 g)483452 (93.6%) 45231.9 (43.2) 28222.1 (23.6)
Low (<2500 g)2020 (100.0%)0.6262028.1 (33.7)0.609813.4 (25.5)0.561
Missing98 (88.9%) 813.4 (21.7) 38.9 (8.4)
Socioeconomic Status
Education
GCSEs or
AS/A-levels
9484 (89.4%) 8419.5 (36.5) 568.0 (25.1)
Graduate198179 (90.4%) 17927.9 (35.9) 10726.6 (23.6)
Postgraduate220217 (98.6%)<0.001 221738.7 (41.0)<0.001 413026.3 (22.6)0.003 4
Annual income
Under £50,000206189 (91.7%) 18931.7 (44.3) 11326.6 (23.7)
£50,000 plus278264 (95.0%)0.215 226431.0 (39.9)0.78215922.1 (23.6)0.472
Missing2827 (96.4%) 2738.9 (42.1) 2117.7 (18.6)
Financial Situation
Finding it difficult3430 (88.2%) 3022.1 (42.5) 147.4 (24.6)
Just about getting by10096 (96.0%) 9631.9 (43.8) 5826.6 (21.3)
Doing alright230216 (93.9%) 21631.0 (42.4) 13622.1 (24.6)
Living comfortably144134 (93.1%)0.40813435.4 (41.6)0.6438122.1 (22.6)0.559
Missing44 (100.0%) 417.7 (11.2) 417.7 (5.6)
Partner’s employment status
Unemployed1210 (83.3%) 1036.6 (77.6) 826.6 (20.2)
Employed489459 (93.9%)0.17545931.1 (42.9)0.44028022.1 (23.6)0.503
Missing1111 (100.0%) 1152.6 (41.9) 526.6 (25.6)
1 Fisher’s Exact Test unless otherwise specified. 2 Pearson’s Chi-squared test with Yates’ continuity correction. 3 Wilcoxon rank test used for binary variables and Kruskal-Wallis used when 3 or more categories. 4 Post-hoc Dunn tests showed only GCSEs or AS/A-levels vs. Graduate (p < 0.001) and Graduate vs. Postgraduate (p = 0.006) had significantly different any breastfeeding median durations and only GCSEs or AS/A-levels vs. Graduate (p = 0.004) and GCSEs or AS/A-levels vs. Postgraduate (p = 0.003) had significantly different exclusive breastfeeding median durations (p-values adjusted with the Benjamini-Hochberg method for multiple comparisons).
Table 2. Constant and time-varying effects of subjective environmental experience on duration of any breastfeeding.
Table 2. Constant and time-varying effects of subjective environmental experience on duration of any breastfeeding.
M1: Controlling for Age and ParityM2: Additionally Controlling for Education
Constant EffectTime-Varying EffectConstant EffectTime-Varying Effect
nHR
(95% CI)
p-ValueSup.
p-Value
K-S
p-Value
CvM
p-Value
HR
(95% CI)
p-ValueSup.
p-Value
K-S
p-Value
CvM
p-Value
Nice
(No vs. Yes)
4731.44
(0.73–2.86)
0.3510.0690.4800.6171.26
(0.62–2.53)
0.5690.0660.4550.607
Crime
(Yes vs. No)
4091.15
(0.75–1.77)
0.5510.0060.7550.6761.12
(0.72–1.74)
0.6420.0020.5900.470
Litter
(Yes vs. No)
4611.08
(0.76–1.53)
0.6930.1390.1790.1561.05
(0.74–1.51)
0.7890.1140.2560.132
Know
(No vs. Yes)
4241.16
(0.82–1.63)
0.4410.6940.3310.3641.18
(0.84–1.67)
0.3910.6310.3280.432
Help
(No vs. Yes)
4101.31
(0.89–1.95)
0.2390.5930.3870.541.23
(0.82–1.82)
0.4080.3850.5200.682
Cox-Aalen model results for duration of any breastfeeding. HR: Hazard Ratio. 95% CI: 95% Confidence Interval). Sup. p-values: Supremum-test p-values testing the significance of parameters under the non-proportional hazards setting. K-S p-values and CvM p-values: Kolmogorov-Smirnov test and Cramer von Mises test p-values for score processes testing if effects are non-proportional (H0 = constant effect). Nice: “My area is a nice place to live”. Crime: “Crime is a problem in my area”. Litter: “Littering and rubbish is a problem in my area”. Know: “People tend to know each other in my area”. Help: “In general, people help each other out in my area”.
Table 3. Constant and time-varying effects of subjective environmental experience on duration of exclusive breastfeeding.
Table 3. Constant and time-varying effects of subjective environmental experience on duration of exclusive breastfeeding.
M1: Controlling for Age and ParityM2: Additionally Controlling for Education
Constant EffectTime-Varying EffectConstant EffectTime-Varying Effect
nHR
(95% CI)
p-ValueSup.
p-Value
K-S
p-Value
CvM
p-Value
HR
(95% CI)
p-ValueSup.
p-Value
K-S
p-Value
CvM
p-Value
Nice
(No vs. Yes)
2901.03
(0.49–2.19)
0.9280.0170.5870.8510.86
(0.40–1.85)
0.6920.0320.6820.865
Crime
(Yes vs. No)
2500.91
(0.55–1.49)
0.6620.1390.4580.3690.83
(0.50–1.40)
0.4310.1310.4630.383
Litter
(Yes vs. No)
2830.78
(0.52–1.17)
0.158<0.0010.4340.2270.72
(0.47–1.09)
0.0610.0020.3740.163
Know
(No vs. Yes)
2600.90
(0.62–1.32)
0.5840.8180.3510.2470.87
(0.59–1.27)
0.4580.6920.6370.271
Help
(No vs. Yes)
2510.77
(0.48–1.23)
0.2660.1590.2360.1270.65
(0.40–1.05)
0.0810.2020.2070.146
Cox-Aalen model results for duration of exclusive breastfeeding. HR: Hazard Ratio. 95% CI: 95% Confidence Interval). Sup. p-values: Supremum-test p-values testing the significance of parameters under the non-proportional hazards setting. K-S p-values and CvM p-values: Kolmogorov-Smirnov test and Cramer von Mises test p-values for score processes testing if effects are non-proportional (H0 = constant effect). Nice: “My area is a nice place to live”. Crime: “Crime is a problem in my area”. Litter: “Littering and rubbish is a problem in my area”. Know: “People tend to know each other in my area”. Help: “In general, people help each other out in my area”.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Brown, L.J.; Myers, S.; Page, A.E.; Emmott, E.H. Subjective Environmental Experiences and Women’s Breastfeeding Journeys: A Survival Analysis Using an Online Survey of UK Mothers. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 7903. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17217903

AMA Style

Brown LJ, Myers S, Page AE, Emmott EH. Subjective Environmental Experiences and Women’s Breastfeeding Journeys: A Survival Analysis Using an Online Survey of UK Mothers. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2020; 17(21):7903. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17217903

Chicago/Turabian Style

Brown, Laura J., Sarah Myers, Abigail E. Page, and Emily H. Emmott. 2020. "Subjective Environmental Experiences and Women’s Breastfeeding Journeys: A Survival Analysis Using an Online Survey of UK Mothers" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, no. 21: 7903. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17217903

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop