Next Article in Journal
The Factorial Validity of the Norwegian Version of the Multicomponent Training Distress Scale (MTDS-N)
Next Article in Special Issue
Prevalence and Determinants of Bad Sleep Perception among Italian Children and Adolescents
Previous Article in Journal
Improving the Quality of Care in Care Homes Using the Quality Improvement Collaborative Approach: Lessons Learnt from Six Projects Conducted in the UK and The Netherlands
Previous Article in Special Issue
Patient Reported Outcome Measures of Sleep Quality in Fibromyalgia: A COSMIN Systematic Review
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Role of Sleep Quality, Trait Anxiety and Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis Measures in Cognitive Abilities of Healthy Individuals

by
Javier Labad
1,2,3,
Neus Salvat-Pujol
4,5,6,
Antonio Armario
3,7,
Ángel Cabezas
8,
Aida de Arriba-Arnau
5,6,
Roser Nadal
3,7,
Lourdes Martorell
3,8,
Mikel Urretavizcaya
3,5,6,9,
José Antonio Monreal
2,3,4,
José Manuel Crespo
3,5,6,9,
Elisabet Vilella
3,8,
Diego José Palao
2,3,4,
José Manuel Menchón
3,5,6,9 and
Virginia Soria
3,5,6,9,*
1
Consorci Sanitari del Maresme, 08340 Mataró, Spain
2
Institut d’Investigació i Innovació Parc Taulí (I3PT), 08208 Sabadell, Spain
3
Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Carlos III Health Institute, 28029 Madrid, Spain
4
Department of Mental Health, Parc Taulí Hospital Universitari, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, 08208 Sabadell, Spain
5
Department of Psychiatry, Bellvitge University Hospital, 08907 L’Hospitalet de Llobregat, Spain
6
Neurosciences Group—Psychiatry and Mental Health, Biomedical Research Institute (IDIBELL), 08908 L’Hospitalet de Llobregat, Spain
7
Institut de Neurociències, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, 08193 Bellaterra, Spain
8
Hospital Universitari Institut Pere Mata, Institut d’Investigació Sanitària Pere Virgili (IISPV), Universitat Rovira i Virgili, 43206 Reus, Spain
9
Department of Clinical Sciences, School of Medicine, Universitat de Barcelona, 08907 L’Hospitalet de Llobregat, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17(20), 7600; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17207600
Submission received: 23 August 2020 / Revised: 15 October 2020 / Accepted: 16 October 2020 / Published: 19 October 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Sleep Quality Research)

Abstract

:
Sleep plays a crucial role in cognitive processes. Sleep and wake memory consolidation seem to be regulated by glucocorticoids, pointing out the potential role of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis in the relationship between sleep quality and cognitive abilities. Trait anxiety is another factor that is likely to moderate the relationship between sleep and cognition, because poorer sleep quality and subtle HPA axis abnormalities have been reported in people with high trait anxiety. The current study aimed to explore whether HPA axis activity or trait anxiety moderate the relationship between sleep quality and cognitive abilities in healthy individuals. We studied 203 healthy individuals. We measured verbal and visual memory, working memory, processing speed, attention and executive function. Sleep quality was assessed with the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index. Trait anxiety was assessed with the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. HPA axis measures included the cortisol awakening response (CAR), diurnal cortisol slope and cortisol levels during the day. Multiple linear regression analyses explored the relationship between sleep quality and cognition and tested potential moderating effects by HPA axis measures and trait anxiety. Poor sleep quality was associated with poorer performance in memory, processing speed and executive function tasks. In people with poorer sleep quality, a blunted CAR was associated with poorer verbal and visual memory and executive functions, and higher cortisol levels during the day were associated with poorer processing speed. Trait anxiety was a moderator of visual memory and executive functioning. These results suggest that subtle abnormalities in the HPA axis and higher trait anxiety contribute to the relationship between lower sleep quality and poorer cognitive functioning in healthy individuals.

1. Introduction

Both poorer sleep quality and longer duration of sleep have been associated with impaired cognitive performance in middle-age and older adults [1,2,3]. Prospective studies have demonstrated that sleep quality disturbances are risk factors of cognitive decline [4,5,6]. Individuals with poor sleep quality are more likely to experience depressive symptoms and state and trait anxiety, as well as a negative cognitive bias (enhanced recognition of negative images) and decreased sustained attention to non-emotional stimuli [7]. The relationship between sleep problems and anxiety or depressive symptoms is thought to be bidirectional [8]. Trait anxiety, which is closely related to neuroticism and refers to the stable tendency to experience negative emotions, is a risk factor for major depression [9] and impairs the processing efficiency of manipulating working memory contents [10]. The neurobiological mechanisms linking impaired cognition and sleep quality are complex. Whether sleep, particularly rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, plays a role in neuroplasticity and memory processes is a matter of debate [11]. Some authors have suggested that during REM, new associative links are formed between memory traces already stored in the neocortex by means of enhancing the cortical plasticity involved in procedural and emotional memory [12,13]. However, other authors argue that REM sleep serves no role in the processing or consolidation of memory [14]. Sleep deprivation has also been associated with impairments in memory recall depending on the emotional content of memories, as positive and neutral memories are more susceptible to disruption by sleep loss than memories for negatively valanced material [15]. The deleterious effects of sleep deprivation on cognition appear to emerge from the interaction between the task environment and specific components of cognitive functioning [16].
There is evidence that specific neural networks that are important for memory consolidation (e.g., hippocampal and parahippocampal regions concerning visuospatial learning) are reactivated during slow-wave sleep [17]. The amygdala–hippocampus–medial prefrontal cortex network involved in emotional processing, fear, memory, and valence consolidation shows the strongest activity during REM sleep, whereas the hippocampus–medial prefrontal cortex network is more active during non-REM sleep [18]. Some authors have suggested that sleep might impact the qualitative reorganization of memory [19]: schema formation (extraction of rules that can then be generalized to novel situations) and integration (integration of recent and remote memories, relational memory and the emergence of false memories) might primarily benefit from slow-wave sleep, whereas the disintegration of a schema (disbanding existing schemas to allow “outside the box thinking” and creativity) might be facilitated by REM sleep.
Changes in synaptic strength, the primary mechanism mediating learning and memory, are thought to be influenced by the wake/sleep cycle, and a core function of sleep is to renormalize overall synaptic strength increased by waking [20]. Animal studies have demonstrated that sleep deprivation is associated with a decrease of dendritic spine numbers in hippocampal area CA1 and that recovery sleep normalizes these structural alterations [21].
Another evidence of the relevant role of sleep on cognitive processes is that sleep disorders can impair cognitive functioning. Working memory deficits have been reported in people suffering from restless leg syndrome [22] or obstructive sleep apnea [23]. However, other studies exploring different memory components in people with obstructive sleep apnea suggest that cognitive impairments affect episodic, procedural and working memory, but that these impairments are not uniform [24]. These findings underscore the need for extensive cognitive testing when exploring associations between sleep and cognition.
A potential biological mechanism explaining the role of sleep quality on cognitive impairments could be the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis. The HPA axis is a physiological system whose activation—with the consequent release of adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) from the pituitary gland and cortisol from the adrenal gland—is triggered by stress. In humans, a marked circadian rhythm in circulating levels of cortisol is observed, with higher levels during the morning and lower levels during the evening [25]. The cortisol awakening response (CAR) is a distinct facet of the circadian cortisol rhythm, which shows a physiological increase of cortisol within the first hour after awakening [26]. Cognitive impairment has been related to HPA axis dysregulation [27], including higher baseline cortisol levels [28], a blunted cortisol awakening response (CAR) and a more flattened diurnal cortisol slope [29]. Previous studies have suggested that people with poor quality sleep show increased stress reactivity of the HPA axis to physical and psychosocial stressors [30]. A recent study [31] including postmenopausal women reported different associations between sleep quality indices measured with the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) and HPA axis measures. Longer sleep latency was associated with a reduced CAR, whereas poorer subjective sleep quality, shorter sleep duration and reduced sleep efficiency were associated with a sharper cortisol decline later in the day. Other studies [32] conducted in early adolescents that measured sleep duration with actigraphy also reported associations between sleep duration and the magnitude of the CAR, with a sex-specific association, suggesting poorer sleep quality in boys was associated with lower CAR.
There is a bidirectional link between sleep and HPA axis activity. Sleep onset has an inhibitory effect on the HPA axis, which has been mostly attributed to slow-wave sleep [33]. On the other hand, acute administration of exogenous cortisol-releasing hormone (CRH) to humans is associated with sleep changes, including increased intermittent wakefulness during the total night and reduction in sleep efficiency [34]. Moreover, acute glucocorticoid administration promotes sleep intensity and suppresses REM sleep in humans [35]. Although HPA axis activity may moderate the relationship between sleep quality and cognitive performance, no previous studies have explored this issue. This issue is an important topic to be studied because sleep and wake memory consolidation seem to be regulated by glucocorticoids, with low levels of cortisol possibly facilitating the dialogue between the hippocampus and the neocortex [36].
Trait anxiety, which is closely related to neuroticism, refers to the stable tendency to experience negative emotions. This personality trait has been associated with poorer sleep quality [37] and with HPA axis abnormalities such as higher morning cortisol levels [38,39], an enhanced CAR [40], or a more flattened diurnal cortisol slope [40,41,42]. For these reasons, it is also important to explore whether trait anxiety moderates the relationship between sleep quality and cognitive abilities.
Thus, the main aim of our study was to explore whether HPA axis activity or trait anxiety moderate the relationship between sleep quality and cognitive abilities in healthy individuals. Our hypothesis was that HPA axis measures (a blunted CAR, a more flattened cortisol diurnal slope and increased cortisol levels during the day) would be associated with decreased cognitive performance, mainly in memory domains, in those individuals with poorer sleep quality.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sample

The sample consisted of 203 healthy individuals who were recruited from the general community through advertisements. All participants had no past or current history of psychiatric disorders (assessed in a semi-structured interview by an experienced psychiatrist) and a score below 7 on the 28-item Spanish adaptation of the Goldberg General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-28) [43]. The exclusion criteria were as follows: age less than 18 years, a diagnosis of psychiatric disorders including substance abuse or dependence (except nicotine), intellectual disability, neurological disorders, severe medical conditions, pregnancy or puerperium, receiving current psychopharmacological treatment (including benzodiazepines), and therapy with corticoids or contraceptive pill use in the previous three months. All participants were recruited in two provinces from Catalonia, Spain (Barcelona and Tarragona) and participated as healthy control groups in other studies exploring the role of the HPA axis on cognition in patients with major depression [44], obsessive-compulsive disorder [45] or psychotic disorders [46]. All healthy participants were recruited by consecutive sampling from the community by advertisements. A previous screening contact by email or phone call was conducted by a clinical researcher who ensured that participants did not meet any exclusion criteria before scheduling the first appointment at the hospital. The research protocol was approved by the local ethics committees of the two institutions where recruitment occurred (Bellvitge University Hospital [Ref: PR81/10] and Hospital Universitari Institut Pere Mata [Ref: 10-06-17/6proj1]). All participants provided written informed consent after having received a full explanation of the study. All procedures contributing to this work complied with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975 (revised in 2013).

2.2. Clinical Assessment

Sociodemographic variables and smoking habits were assessed using a semi-structured interview administered by an experienced clinician. Weight and height were measured in all participants to calculate body mass index using the formula weight (kg)/height (m2).
The Spanish version of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) [47,48] was used for assessing state and trait anxiety symptoms. The Spanish version of the STAI includes two subscales (state anxiety and trait anxiety) of 20 items that were adapted and validated to Spanish from the form X of the STAI. Each item is scored from 0–3, with a range of scores for each subscale from 0–60. The reliability of the scale in our sample was very good for both STAI state anxiety (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.87) and STAI trait anxiety (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.87).
Sleep quality was evaluated with the PSQI [49], a self-report questionnaire that assesses sleep quality over the previous month. The PSQI includes 19 self-rated questions and five questions rated by the bed partner or roommate (if available). Only the self-rated questions are included in the scoring. They measure seven different components of sleep: subjective sleep quality, sleep latency, sleep duration, habitual sleep efficiency, sleep disturbances, use of sleep medication and daytime dysfunction. The global PSQI score is calculated by adding the seven component scores, providing an overall score ranging from 0–21, where lower scores denote a healthier sleep quality. A cut-off score of 5 has been used for defining people with poor sleep quality (PSQI ≥ 5) [50]. Of all 203 participants, 22 had missing data on at least one item that did not allow us to calculate the global PSQI score. The reliability of the scale in our sample was considered adequate (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.69). All participants completed the PSQI on the same date as the clinical assessment at the hospital facilities. As most of them came alone to the visit, information on questions rated by the bed partner or roommate (item 10 subitems) were not available.

2.3. Neuropsychological Assessment

The following neuropsychological tests were administered to all participants by a neuropsychologist to assess different cognitive domains (see Box S1 from Supplementary Material for additional information on each test): (1) verbal learning and memory: Hopkins Verbal Learning Test Revised (HVLT-R); (2) visual learning and memory: Brief Visuospatial Memory Test Revised (BVMT-R) and the Rey Complex Figure Test (RCFT), which includes copy, immediate recall and delayed recall sub-scores; (3) working memory: Corsi Block-Tapping Test (CBTT) for non-verbal working memory, and Letter-Number Span (LNS) to assess verbal working memory; (4) processing speed: Trail Making Test Part A (TMT-A), Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia—Symbol Coding (BACS-SC), and category fluency (animal naming); (5) sustained attention/vigilance: Continuous Performance Test–Identical Pairs (CPT-IP); (6) selective attention/inhibition: Stroop test (direct sub-scores for words, W; colors, C; words-colors, WC; interference); (7) reasoning and problem solving: Neuropsychological Assessment Battery® Mazes (NAB-Mazes); and (8) executive control: Trail Making Test Part B (TMT-B). For all but two of the cognitive tests, higher scores reflect better cognitive performance. The exceptions are the TMT-A and TMT-B, where the outcome measure is the number of seconds needed to perform the task; thus, higher scores reflect poorer cognitive performance.
Neuropsychological assessment was conducted in the morning between 9 a.m. and 12 p.m.

2.4. HPA Axis Measures

Saliva was collected from all participants using Salivette® tubes (Sarstedt AG & Co., Nümbrecht, Germany) for cortisol determination. Five home-collected saliva samples were obtained at the following times on the same day: awakening (T1), 30′ post awakening (T2), 60′ post awakening (T3), 10 a.m. (T4), and 11 p.m. (T5). Eating, drinking, smoking or brushing teeth in the previous 15 min was not allowed. For those home-collected samples, participants were instructed to collect all samples during a regular day, avoiding stressful situations and intense physical activity. Of all premenopausal women (N = 69), information on the date of the last menstrual period was available for 55% of them. We calculated the difference (in days) between the dates from the last menstrual period and the HPA axis assessment to define follicular and luteal phases as described elsewhere [45]. Exploratory analyses comparing cortisol concentrations by menstrual cycle status in the subsample of women with available data were performed.
Saliva samples were processed at the Biobank of the Institut de Investigació Sanitària Pere Virgili (IISPV). After centrifugation of the Salivette tubes at 3000 rpm for 5 min, the saliva was aliquoted and frozen at −20 °C until salivary cortisol was tested in singleton using a high-sensitivity commercial chemiluminescence immunoassay (IBL, Hamburg, Germany). The intra-assay and inter-assay coefficients of variation were under 8%. The sensitivity of the assay was 0.08 nmol/L.
Three HPA axis measures were considered: CAR, diurnal cortisol slope and cortisol levels over the day.

2.4.1. CAR

The CAR is a physiological response that consists of a rise in cortisol levels following morning awakening [51] and represents a distinct component of the cortisol circadian cycle, with characteristics unrelated to those of cortisol secretion throughout the rest of the day. The CAR was calculated using the area under the curve with respect to the increase (AUCi) with the trapezoid formula as suggested by Pruessner et al. [52]. Three cortisol concentrations (T1, T2, T3) were used for the calculation of the CAR with the AUCi. We also estimated the consistency of the cortisol rise after awakening as described previously [53]. Responder rates were defined as an increase of salivary cortisol levels of at least 2.5 nmol/l above baseline levels (awakening time).

2.4.2. Diurnal Cortisol Slope

The HPA axis follows a marked circadian variation, with greater cortisol secretion in the morning than the evening in humans [54]. The diurnal cortisol slope reflects the rate of decline in cortisol levels across the day, from morning to evening. We calculated the slope using the T4 and T5 samples. The slope was calculated with and without transformed cortisol concentrations. In the tables, the raw slopes are displayed; however, when parametric tests and multivariate analyses were performed, slopes were calculated using transformed cortisol values (see Section 2.5).

2.4.3. Cortisol Levels over the Day

The cortisol levels over the day (from awakening to 11 p.m.) were calculated using the area under the curve with respect to the ground (AUCg) with a trapezoid formula [52]. Although we transformed the cortisol values with a power transformation, we did not use transformed values in the calculation of cortisol levels throughout the day. This approach was preferred because the cited power transformation sometimes yields negative values for very low cortisol values (that would be the case for most evening cortisol levels), which could affect the interpretation of cortisol levels using the trapezoid formula.

2.5. Statistical Analyses

SPSS version 21.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA) was used for the statistical analyses. Cortisol values were transformed to approximate a normal distribution, as suggested by recent expert consensus guidelines [51]. The following power transformation was used: X’ = (X0.26−1)/0.26, as described by Miller and Plessow [55].
For descriptive purposes, we compared clinical variables between people with or without poor sleep quality taking into account the cut-off point of 5 in the PSQI. The chi-square test was used to compare categorical data among groups. T-test was used to compare continuous data. The statistical significance level was set at p < 0.05 (bilateral).
A univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used for exploring gender and age differences in sleep quality, anxiety measures and HPA axis variables. Each continuous variable was used as the dependent variable. Gender and age group were considered independent factors. We also conducted a stratified exploratory analysis by gender and two specific age ranges: young adulthood (between 18–40 years) and middle adulthood (between 41–65 years). The association between psychometric scales, HPA axis measures and cognitive tasks was tested with Pearson’s correlations.
Multiple linear regression analyses were performed to explore the relationship between sleep quality (as a continuous measure) and cognition while testing the moderating effects of HPA axis measures and trait anxiety. All final equations were adjusted for covariates (age, gender, education level, BMI and smoking (cigarettes/day)) and potential interactions between HPA axis measures or trait anxiety and sleep quality. Cognitive tasks were considered the dependent variables. As two cognitive measures (TMT A and B) were skewed, we used natural log transformation to normalize these variables before conducting the multiple linear regression analyses. Hierarchical regressions were performed considering three models, and all independent variables were included in different steps with the enter procedure. The first model (sleep quality, unadjusted) included only the PSQI total score as the independent variable. The second model (sleep quality + HPA axis measures) included the PSQI total score and the three main HPA axis measures (CAR, cortisol diurnal slope and cortisol levels over the day), as well as cortisol levels at awakening because this measure needs to be adjusted in analyses to consider the CAR, as per the recent consensus [51]. We verified the multicollinearity of HPA axis measures with the variance inflation factor (VIF), and for all equations, VIF scores were below 2, suggesting that there was no multicollinearity. The third and final model included sleep quality scores, HPA axis measures, covariates and significant interactions. Interactions between HPA axis measures or trait anxiety and sleep quality were included with the forward procedure. Thus, only significant interactions entered the final equation. We did not adjust for state anxiety in the multivariate analyses because state anxiety and trait anxiety STAI scores in our sample were highly correlated (r = 0.59, p < 0.001). We aimed to avoid problems of collinearity, as trait anxiety was considered a covariate in all multiple linear regression analyses.

3. Results

3.1. Univariate Analyses

The sociodemographic and psychometric data of the sample by sleep quality groups are described in Table 1. There were no significant differences in sociodemographic variables, BMI, smoking habits or alcohol consumption. In relation to anxiety scores, those individuals with poorer sleep quality reported more trait anxiety but not more state anxiety. PSQI total scores did not differ between smokers and non-smokers, nor by participants from different alcohol groups. Regarding specific PSQI items related to potential causes of sleep problems (item 5), the prevalence of significant problems (defined as the presence of the condition related to each item during three or more times per week) were: (5a) “cannot get to sleep within 30 minutes” (11.2%); (5b) “wake up in the middle of the night or early morning” (19.3%); (5c) “have to get up to use the bathroom” (23.5%); (5d) “cannot breath comfortably” (1.6%), (5e) “cough or snore loudly” (8.6%), (5f) “feel too cold” (1.1%), (5g) “feel too hot” (10.8%), (5h) “have bad dreams” (1.1%), (5i) “have pain” (4.3%).
Cortisol measures between sleep quality groups were similar between people with good and poor sleep quality, with no significant differences in the CAR, the diurnal cortisol slope or cortisol levels over the day (Table 2). Smokers had a greater CAR when compared to non-smokers (mean [standard deviation]: 50.6 [61.6] vs. 25.1 [59.0], p = 0.041). No significant differences were found in other HPA axis measures between smokers and non-smokers. HPA axis measures did not differ between participants with different alcohol intake patterns (none, occasional, daily). There were no significant differences in cortisol levels nor any HPA axis measure between premenopausal women in the follicular or luteal phase. The responder rates for the CAR in the overall sample were 70.3%.
Participants with poorer sleep quality had lower scores in several cognitive tasks dealing with verbal learning, visual learning, processing speed, attention and executive function (Table 3).

3.2. Exploratory Analysis by Gender and Age Range

We also explored the role of gender and age range (young adulthood versus middle adulthood) in sleep quality, anxiety and cortisol measures (Table S1). An older age was associated with poorer sleep quality, lower cortisol levels at 60′ post awakening, a more blunted CAR and lower cortisol levels over the day (Table S1; Figure S1). Sleep quality was not influenced by gender. However, women showed higher cortisol levels at awakening. A gender-by-age interaction was found in trait and state anxiety parameters, indicating that middle-aged women reported more state and trait anxiety than middle-aged men, but this gender-specific pattern was not observed in young individuals.
An exploratory heat map of correlation analyses exploring the relationship between sleep quality, anxiety, HPA axis measures and cognition in each subgroup by age and gender are described in Figures S2 and S3. Heat maps suggest that trait anxiety is more clearly associated with cognitive outcomes than state anxiety or even sleep quality. When comparing age and gender subgroups, stronger associations were found in the subgroup of middle-aged men. As shown in Figure S3, trait anxiety was associated with poorer cognitive functioning in most tasks, mainly visual memory, processing speed and executive function.

3.3. Multiple Linear Regression Analyses

To explore the association between sleep quality, HPA axis measures and trait anxiety with cognitive tasks while adjusting for potential confounders, independent multiple linear regression analyses were performed (Table 4; full multiple linear regression analyses are shown in Tables S2–S4).
Poor sleep quality was associated with most memory tasks (Table S2) after adjusting for HPA axis measures. However, when adjusting for clinical covariates and potential interactions, significant interactions between PSQI total scores and the CAR were found in three cognitive tasks dealing with verbal memory (HVLT-R), visual memory (BVMT-R) and working memory (LNS). These interactions suggest that there is a different pattern in the relationship between the CAR and cognitive tasks depending on reported sleep quality. In those participants with poorer sleep quality, an increased CAR was associated with better cognitive functioning. We also found that the relationship between sleep quality and cognitive performance in the BVMT-R task was moderated by trait anxiety (Table S2).
Of all processing speed tasks, sleep quality interacted with two HPA axis measures in the TMT-A (Table S3). As the TMT-A is measured in seconds, positive β coefficients reflect poorer performance on this task. A positive interaction between PSQI and cortisol levels over the day was found, suggesting that cortisol levels over the day negatively impact performance in this processing speed task, particularly in participants with poorer sleep quality. In contrast, a negative interaction between PSQI and CAR was found, which suggests that in people with poorer sleep quality, a blunted CAR was associated with poorer cognitive functioning on this task.
Finally, in those cognitive tasks dealing with attention and executive functions (Table S4), sleep quality interacted with cortisol levels over the day in two Stroop tasks (words-colors and interference scores). In participants with poorer sleep quality, higher cortisol levels during the day were associated with lower scores on both cognitive tasks, indicating poorer cognitive performance. Trait anxiety moderated the relationship between sleep quality and the TMT-B, a measure of cognitive flexibility.

4. Discussion

In the present study, which aimed to explore whether HPA axis activity or trait anxiety act as moderators of the relationship between sleep quality and cognitive abilities in healthy individuals, we found different roles for HPA axis measures and trait anxiety. Two HPA axis measures were moderators of the association between poor sleep quality and cognitive impairment. For instance, the CAR had a moderating effect on verbal learning, visual learning and working memory tasks, whereas cortisol levels over the day had a moderating effect on executive functioning tasks and processing speed. In general, in patients with poorer sleep quality, pathological HPA axis activity (blunted CAR, increased cortisol levels during the day) was associated with poorer cognitive functioning. Trait anxiety showed moderating effects in the relationship between sleep quality and cognition for cognitive tasks dealing with visual memory (BVMT-R) and another task that measures cognitive flexibility and has visual search demands (TMT-B).
The results regarding the role of trait anxiety on visual cognitive tasks are in accordance with previous studies that have also reported associations between poor quality sleep and poorer cognitive performance in visual memory tasks [2] and the TMT-B [56]. Some studies have explored the relationship between trait anxiety, sleep quality and the ability to remember the extinction of conditioned fear, reporting associations between poor quality sleep and poor extinction and between poor quality sleep and trait anxiety [12]. However, this last study did not support a link between trait anxiety and poor extinction. In another study exploring the impact of trait anxiety on visual search and rapid serial visual presentation tasks [57], high-anxiety individuals allocated attention to the target less efficiently and had reduced suppression of distractors than did low-anxiety individuals. In our sample, trait anxiety was more clearly associated with impaired cognition in middle-aged men. Other studies in the literature have found gender differences in the relationship between trait anxiety and cognition, with positive correlations between neuroticism scores and verbal memory, general memory and delayed recall scores of the working memory test in healthy females [58]. Visual processing speed is also affected by sleep deprivation, which results in reduced information processing capacity [59]. Since both poor quality sleep and trait anxiety might negatively affect visual learning, common neural substrates explaining the association between sleep quality and trait anxiety may be involved in visual memory processes. In this line, the most replicated findings by structural neuroimaging studies have been negative associations between trait anxiety and grey matter volume in the prefrontal cortex, including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex and orbitofrontal cortex [60]. These brain regions, which have been implicated in sleep quality [61,62], are also critical for visual processing and learning [63].
In relation to the potential link between sleep quality and HPA axis activity on cognition, it is noteworthy that we found moderating effects of HPA axis measures for several cognitive domains that are thought to be related to hippocampus and prefrontal cortex functioning. The hippocampus, a brain area that is particularly sensitive to prolonged increases in glucocorticoid levels brought on by prolonged stress, is involved in the consolidation of declarative memory processes [27] and participates in the regulation of the HPA axis by means of negative feedback. The hippocampus is also thought to play a role in the CAR, as subjects with hippocampal damage show a blunted CAR [64]. Interestingly, the hippocampus is a key region implicated in sleep processes. Recent animal studies have compared the occurrence of sleep and sleep stages in electroencephalogram recordings of cortical activity and local field potential recordings from the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus [65]. Although slow-wave sleep congruently occurred in signals covering neocortical and hippocampal activity, REM sleep often started substantially earlier in the hippocampus than in neocortical networks. The mechanisms mediating this dissociation between hippocampal and neocortical networks are unknown. However, these findings might be relevant for the understanding of some functions, such as memory formation, that have been associated with the stage of REM sleep [11,65].
The other findings of the associations among poor sleep quality—which include cortisol levels during the day and poorer functioning in two executive functioning tasks (Stroop word-colors and interference scores) and a processing speed task (TMT-A)—point to a role of the prefrontal cortex in the relationship between sleep quality and the HPA axis. First, previous studies have related higher diurnal salivary cortisol levels with a smaller prefrontal cortex surface area in elderly men and women [66], which fits well with the potential neurotoxic effects of glucocorticoids on hippocampal neurogenesis and prefrontal cortical synaptic plasticity [67]. Second, the prefrontal cortex has been found to be involved in the Stroop task, as meta-analyses of neuroimaging studies suggest that the response to the Stroop task is highly left lateralized, most prominently in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and inferior frontal regions [68]. Our findings suggest that increased cortisol levels over the day might be detrimental for some cognitive abilities related to the prefrontal cortex, particularly in those individuals who report poorer sleep quality.
In our study, all the HPA axis measures (CAR, diurnal cortisol slope and cortisol levels over the day), were not associated with poorer sleep quality. Regarding diurnal cortisol secretion patterns, previous studies have shown a flatter diurnal cortisol slope in individuals with shorter sleep [69,70] and poorer sleep quality [70,71,72]. There are mixed results in the literature regarding the CAR, with some studies reporting no relationship between sleep quality and the CAR in adolescents [73], others reporting a blunted CAR in middle-aged adults with poorer sleep quality [72] and others suggesting that poor sleep quality is associated with a blunted CAR only in police officers that are inactive or insufficiently active during their leisure time [74]. Our study is in line with the idea that CAR variation is not affected by sleep quality [75].
Although our findings suggest that HPA axis and trait anxiety moderate the relationship between HPA axis activity and memory processes, we need to underscore that we cannot infer causality due to the cross-sectional design of our study. Longitudinal studies have suggested that both sleep quality [76] and HPA axis dysregulation [77,78] are predictors of cognitive decline. However, no prospective studies have explored the interplay between sleep quality and HPA axis activity in relation to the risk of cognitive impairment over time. Future studies are needed to replicate our cross-sectional findings and to test potential mediating effects with longitudinal data. This approach would allow the exploration of whether sleep quality modifies HPA axis activity, which in turn contributes to a poorer cognitive outcome, or, on the contrary, whether a dysregulated HPA axis impairs sleep quality, leading to a poorer cognitive outcome. Prospective studies would also allow the study of potential neurobiological mechanisms involved in this process, including the determination of brain changes in regions of interest (hippocampus and prefrontal cortex). For instance, previous studies have reported greater brain atrophy and cognitive decline in healthy older adults with short sleep duration [79]. Whether these brain changes might be mediated by HPA axis activity would be interesting to explore and is particularly important because chronic stress and the associated increase in serum levels of glucocorticoids have been hypothesized to have neurotoxic effects on the brain and to negatively impact cognition [77]. Chronic stress causes remodeling of dendrites and synaptic connections in many brain regions, including not only the hippocampus but also the amygdala and medial prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex [80].
Our study focused on the interactions between sleep quality, trait anxiety and HPA axis measures on cognitive abilities. However, other potential biological mechanisms explaining the role of poorer sleep quality on cognitive impairments include age-related changes in the hypothalamic suprachiasmatic nucleus or other important elements of the circadian timing system [81], a decline in the white matter microstructure of frontal-subcortical tracts [82], and hypoxemia and sleep fragmentation resulting from obstructive sleep apnea [83].
A limitation of our study was the assessment of sleep quality with a self-report questionnaire without the inclusion of objective measures for assessing sleep such as polysomnography or actigraphy. Nevertheless, the PSQI has been widely used in research and clinical practice, providing information on a respondent’s sleep quality [50]. Although an objective assessment of sleep might be better for exploring the quantitative aspects of sleep (i.e., sleep duration, sleep latency and number of arousals), sleep quality involves purely subjective (e.g., self-perceived) aspects such as “depth” or “restfulness” of sleep, as the assessment of sleep quality is a complex phenomenon that is difficult to measure and define objectively [50]. The PSQI was administered the same day as the cognitive testing. Therefore, sleep quality reports of each participant reflect the subjective perception of the month before the cognitive testing. We did not quantify the sleep duration or sleep quality of the day before the cognitive assessment, which might be considered a limitation. Information on the five questions rated by the partner or roommate in the PSQI were not assessed. Therefore, indirect information on potential conditions that might contribute to sleep difficulties such as restless legs syndrome or obstructive sleep apnea was not available. Moreover, only 49.5% of the sample had a roommate or bed partner. Caffeine intake of participants was not registered, and we could not adjust multivariate analyses for this variable. People with subjective complaints about sleep quality assessed with the PSQI have been suggested to have an increased risk for subthreshold levels of depressive symptoms [84]. However, the sample of our study was composed of mentally healthy individuals without a past or current history of psychiatric disorders who were interviewed by a psychiatrist and scored less than 7 points in the GHQ-28. Another limitation of our study was measuring the CAR only one day. As the original protocol also included the assessment of the dexamethasone suppression test [44,45,46], all participants took 0.25 mg of dexamethasone at 11 p.m. and therefore we dismissed the possibility of collecting further CAR sampling the next day. In the calculation of the CAR, we used the AUC with respect to the increase, as previous guidelines recommend using measures of change in cortisol levels that capture the dynamic of post-awakening cortisol changes (e.g., AUCi). Moreover, as the variable for cortisol levels during the day was calculated with the AUC with respect to the ground and included cortisol levels from awakening to 11 p.m., we preferred to only use the CAR as AUCi for avoiding multicollinearity problems with cortisol concentrations over the day.
Some study strengths need to be underscored. First, our study is the first to explore the interplay of distinct HPA axis measures and sleep quality on cognitive abilities in a sample of healthy individuals. Second, a detailed cognitive battery was included, which allowed the assessment of different cognitive domains including memory, executive functioning, processing speed and attention. The decision for selecting specific cognitive tests was based on the original projects that included patients with psychotic disorders and mood disorders, along with healthy individuals as a control group. Some tests that were specifically designed for patients with schizophrenia (e.g., BACS-SC) are included in the 10 cognitive tests of the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, which was initially considered the gold standard for assessing cognition in patients with schizophrenia [85]. However, over the last few years it has been widely used in patients with mood disorders [86,87]. Third, we included a sample of people without mental illnesses with a wide range of ages, including both young and middle-aged adults. Finally, we tested moderating effects of HPA axis measures and trait anxiety when exploring the relationship between sleep quality and cognition, and we also controlled for other potential confounders that might affect sleep quality such as age, gender, education level, BMI and smoking habits.

5. Conclusions

Our study suggests that the role of sleep quality on cognitive abilities in healthy individuals depends on personality traits (trait anxiety) and HPA axis activity. The CAR, cortisol levels during the day and trait anxiety moderate the relationship between sleep quality and executive function and visual and working memory abilities. In people with poorer sleep quality, HPA axis alterations (blunted CAR and increased cortisol levels over the day) and high trait anxiety were associated with poorer performance in cognitive domains mediated by the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/1660-4601/17/20/7600/s1 Table S1. Sleep quality, anxiety and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis measures by gender and age subgroups. Table S2. Multiple linear regression analyses dealing with verbal, visual and working memory tasks. Table S3. Multiple linear regression analyses dealing with processing speed tasks. Table S4. Multiple linear regression analyses dealing with attention/vigilance and executive function tasks. Figure S1. Mean cortisol levels in healthy individuals by age and gender subgroups. Figure S2. Heat map of Pearson’s correlations between sleep quality, anxiety, HPA axis measures and cognitive tasks in young healthy individuals. Figure S3. Heat map of Pearson’s correlations between sleep quality, anxiety, HPA axis measures and cognitive tasks in middle-aged healthy individuals. Box S1. Neuropsychological tests and cognitive domains.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.L. and V.S.; methodology, J.L, V.S., R.N., A.A., J.M.M., E.V.; software, J.L.; formal analysis, J.L., V.S., R.N., A.A.; investigation, J.L., V.S., N.S.-P., Á.C., A.d.A.-A., L.M., M.U., J.A.M., J.M.C.; resources, N.S.-P, Á.C., A.d.A.-A., L.M., M.U., J.A.M., J.M.C.; data curation, J.L.; writing—original draft preparation, J.L.; writing—review and editing, J.L., N.S.-P., A.A., Á.C., A.d.A.-A., R.N., L.M., M.U., J.A.M., J.M.C., E.V., D.J.P., J.M.M., and V.S.; visualization, J.L., R.N. and V.S.; supervision, V.S.; project administration, J.L. and V.S. funding acquisition, J.L., V.S. and J.M.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported in part by grants from the Carlos III Health Institute through the Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities (PI10/01753, PI10/01607, PI15/01386 and PI15/00662), the European Regional Development Fund (ERDF) “A way to build Europe”, the Fundació La Marató de TV3 (092230/092231), CIBERSAM, and the Catalan Agency for the Management of University and Research Grants (AGAUR 2017 SGR 1247 and AGAUR 2017 SGR 632). Dr. Labad received an Intensification of the Research Activity Grant by the Health Department of the Generalitat de Catalunya (SLT006/17/00012) during 2018 and 2019 and by the Instituto de Salud Carlos III during 2020 and 2021 (INT19/00071). Samples from patients included in this study were processed and preserved by the Biobank IISPV and the Biobank HUB-ICO-IDIBELL, integrated in the Spanish National Biobanks Network (PT17/0015/0024) and Xarxa Banc de Tumors. The funders had no role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Acknowledgments

The authors are very grateful to all of the study participants, as well as to the staff from Bellvitge University Hospital and Institut Pere Mata, who helped collect the samples used in this study. We would also like to thank the staff of Biopsychology at the Department of Psychology of the Technische Universität Dresden for analyzing salivary cortisol samples and the technicians from the Biobank IISPV of Reus and the Biobank HUB-ICO-IDIBELL.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflict of interest regarding this work.

References

  1. Lo, J.C.; Groeger, J.A.; Cheng, G.H.; Dijk, D.-J.; Chee, M.W. Self-reported sleep duration and cognitive performance in older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med. 2016, 17, 87–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Rana, B.K.; Panizzon, M.S.; Franz, C.E.; Spoon, K.M.; Jacobson, K.C.; Xian, H.; Ancoli-Israel, S.; Lyons, M.; Kremen, W.S. Association of Sleep Quality on Memory-Related Executive Functions in Middle Age. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2017, 24, 67–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Waller, K.L.; Mortensen, E.L.; Avlund, K.; Osler, M.; Fagerlund, B.; Lauritzen, M.; Jennum, P. Subjective sleep quality and daytime sleepiness in late midlife and their association with age-related changes in cognition. Sleep Med. 2016, 17, 165–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Johar, H.; Kawan, R.; Thwing, R.E.; Karl-Heinz, L.; Emeny, R.T.; Ladwig, K.-H. Impaired Sleep Predicts Cognitive Decline in Old People: Findings from the Prospective KORA Age Study. Sleep 2016, 39, 217–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Kim, H.-B.; Myung, S.-K.; Lee, S.-M.; Park, Y.C. Longer Duration of Sleep and Risk of Cognitive Decline: A Meta-Analysis of Observational Studies. Neuroepidemiology 2016, 47, 171–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Potvin, O.; Lorrain, D.; Forget, H.; Dubé, M.; Grenier, S.; Préville, M.; Hudon, C. Sleep Quality and 1-Year Incident Cognitive Impairment in Community-Dwelling Older Adults. Sleep 2012, 35, 491–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Gobin, C.M.; Banks, J.B.; Fins, A.I.; Tartar, J.L. Poor sleep quality is associated with a negative cognitive bias and decreased sustained attention. J. Sleep Res. 2015, 24, 535–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Alvaro, P.K.; Roberts, R.M.; Harris, J.K. A Systematic Review Assessing Bidirectionality between Sleep Disturbances, Anxiety, and Depression. Sleep 2013, 36, 1059–1068. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Grupe, D.W.; Nitschke, J.B. Uncertainty and anticipation in anxiety: An integrated neurobiological and psychological perspective. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2013, 14, 488–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Basten, U.; Stelzel, C.; Fiebach, C.J. Trait anxiety and the neural efficiency of manipulation in working memory. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2012, 12, 571–588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Hobson, J.A.; Pace-Schott, E.F. The cognitive neuroscience of sleep: Neuronal systems, consciousness and learning. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002, 3, 679–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Pace-Schott, E.F.; Germain, A.; Milad, M.R. Effects of sleep on memory for conditioned fear and fear extinction. Psychol. Bull. 2015, 141, 835–857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Stickgold, R.; Hobson, J.A.; Fosse, R.; Fosse, M. Sleep, Learning, and Dreams: Off-line Memory Reprocessing. Science 2001, 294, 1052–1057. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Vertes, R.; Eastman, K.E. The case against memory consolidation in REM sleep. Behav. Brain Sci. 2000, 23, 867–876. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Killgore, W.D.S. Effects of sleep deprivation on cognition. Prog. Brain Res. 2010, 185, 105–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Jackson, M.L.; Gunzelmann, G.; Whitney, P.; Hinson, J.M.; Belenky, G.; Rabat, A.; Van Dongen, H.P.A. Deconstructing and reconstructing cognitive performance in sleep deprivation. Sleep Med. Rev. 2012, 17, 215–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Chambers, A.M. The role of sleep in cognitive processing: Focusing on memory consolidation. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 2017, 8, e1433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Genzel, L.; Spoormaker, V.; Konrad, B.; Dresler, M. The role of rapid eye movement sleep for amygdala-related memory processing. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2015, 122, 110–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Landmann, N.; Kuhn, M.; Piosczyk, H.; Feige, B.; Baglioni, C.; Spiegelhalder, K.; Frase, L.; Riemann, D.; Sterr, A.; Nissen, C. The reorganisation of memory during sleep. Sleep Med. Rev. 2014, 18, 531–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. De Vivo, L.; Bellesi, M.; Marshall, W.; Bushong, E.A.; Ellisman, M.H.; Tononi, G.; Cirelli, C. Ultrastructural evidence for synaptic scaling across the wake/sleep cycle. Science 2017, 355, 507–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Havekes, R.; Park, A.J.; Tudor, J.C.; Luczak, V.G.; Hansen, R.T.; Ferri, S.L.; Bruinenberg, V.M.; Poplawski, S.G.; Day, J.P.; Aton, S.J.; et al. Sleep deprivation causes memory deficits by negatively impacting neuronal connectivity in hippocampal area CA1. eLife 2016, 5, e13424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  22. Kim, S.M.; Choi, J.W.; Lee, C.; Lee, B.U.; Koo, Y.S.; Kim, K.H.; Jung, K.-Y. Working memory deficit in patients with restless legs syndrome: An event-related potential study. Sleep Med. 2014, 15, 808–815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Thomas, R.J.; Rosen, B.R.; Stern, C.E.; Weiss, J.W.; Kwong, K.K. Functional imaging of working memory in obstructive sleep-disordered breathing. J. Appl. Physiol. 2005, 98, 2226–2234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Naëgelé, B.; Launois, S.H.; Mazza, S.; Feuerstein, C.; Pépin, J.-L.; Lévy, P. Which Memory Processes are Affected in Patients with Obstructive Sleep Apnea? An Evaluation of 3 Types of Memory. Sleep 2006, 29, 533–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Nicolaides, N.C.; Charmandari, E.; Chrousos, G.P.; Kino, T. Circadian endocrine rhythms: The hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and its actions. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2014, 1318, 71–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Clow, A.; Hucklebridge, F.; Stalder, T.; Evans, P.; Thorn, L. The cortisol awakening response: More than a measure of HPA axis function. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2010, 35, 97–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Lupien, S.; Fiocco, A.; Wan, N.; Maheu, F.; Lord, C.; Schramek, T.; Tu, M.T. Stress hormones and human memory function across the lifespan. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2005, 30, 225–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Lee, B.K.; Glass, T.A.; McAtee, M.J.; Wand, G.S.; Bandeen-Roche, K.; Bolla, K.I.; Schwartz, B.S. Associations of Salivary Cortisol with Cognitive Function in the Baltimore Memory Study. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2007, 64, 810–818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Evans, P.; Fredhoi, C.; Loveday, C.; Hucklebridge, F.; Aitchison, E.; Forte, D.; Clow, A. The diurnal cortisol cycle and cognitive performance in the healthy old. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2011, 79, 371–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Van Dalfsen, J.H.; Markus, C.R. The influence of sleep on human hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis reactivity: A systematic review. Sleep Med. Rev. 2018, 39, 187–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Huang, T.; Poole, E.M.; Vetter, C.; Rexrode, K.M.; Kubzansky, L.D.; Schernhammer, E.; Rohleder, N.; Hu, F.B.; Redline, S.; Tworoger, S.S. Habitual sleep quality and diurnal rhythms of salivary cortisol and dehydroepiandrosterone in postmenopausal women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2017, 84, 172–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Pesonen, A.-K.; Martikainen, S.; Kajantie, E.; Heinonen, K.; Wehkalampi, K.; Lahti, J.; Strandberg, T.; Räikkönen, K. The associations between adolescent sleep, diurnal cortisol patterns and cortisol reactivity to dexamethasone suppression test. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2014, 49, 150–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Balbo, M.; Leproult, R.; Van Cauter, E. Impact of Sleep and Its Disturbances on Hypothalamo-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis Activity. Int. J. Endocrinol. 2010, 2010, 759234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Schüssler, P.; Kluge, M.; Gamringer, W.; Wetter, T.C.; Yassouridis, A.; Uhr, M.; Rupprecht, R.; Steiger, A. Corticotropin-releasing hormone induces depression-like changes of sleep electroencephalogram in healthy women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2016, 74, 302–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Chrousos, G.; Vgontzas, A.N.; Kritikou, I. HPA Axis and Sleep; Endotext: South Dartmouth, MA, USA, 2000. Available online: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK279071/ (accessed on 3 March 2020).
  36. Feld, G.B.; Born, J. Neurochemical mechanisms for memory processing during sleep: Basic findings in humans and neuropsychiatric implications. Neuropsychopharmacology 2019, 45, 31–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Stephan, Y.; Sutin, A.R.; Bayard, S.; Križan, Z.; Terracciano, A. Personality and sleep quality: Evidence from four prospective studies. Health Psychol. 2018, 37, 271–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Polk, D.E.; Cohen, S.; Doyle, W.J.; Skoner, D.P.; Kirschbaum, C. State and trait affect as predictors of salivary cortisol in healthy adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2005, 30, 261–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Portella, M.J.; Harmer, C.J.; Flint, J.; Cowen, P.; Goodwin, G.M. Enhanced Early Morning Salivary Cortisol in Neuroticism. Am. J. Psychiatry 2005, 162, 807–809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Montoliu, T.; Hidalgo, V.; Salvador, A. Personality and Hypothalamic–Pituitary–Adrenal Axis in Older Men and Women. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 983. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Doane, L.D.; Franz, C.E.; Prom-Wormley, E.C.; Eaves, L.J.; Mendoza, S.P.; Hellhammer, D.H.; Lupien, S.; Xian, H.; Lyons, M.J.; Kremen, W.S.; et al. Negative emotionality, depressive symptoms and cortisol diurnal rhythms: Analysis of a community sample of middle-aged males. Horm. Behav. 2011, 60, 202–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Hauner, K.K.; Adam, E.K.; Mineka, S.; Doane, L.D.; DeSantis, A.S.; Zinbarg, R.; Craske, M.; Griffith, J.W. Neuroticism and introversion are associated with salivary cortisol patterns in adolescents. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2008, 33, 1344–1356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Lobo, A.; Pérez-Echeverría, M.J.; Artal, J. Validity of the scaled version of the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-28) in a Spanish population. Psychol. Med. 1986, 16, 135–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Salvat-Pujol, N.; Labad, J.; Urretavizcaya, M.; De Arriba-Arnau, A.; Segalàs, C.; Real, E.; Ferrer, A.; Crespo, J.M.; Jiménez-Murcia, S.; Soriano-Mas, C.; et al. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity and cognition in major depression: The role of remission status. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2017, 76, 38–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Labad, J.; Soria, V.; Salvat-Pujol, N.; Segalàs, C.; Real, E.; Urretavizcaya, M.; De Arriba-Arnau, A.; Ferrer, À.; Crespo, J.M.; Jiménez-Murcia, S.; et al. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity in the comorbidity between obsessive-compulsive disorder and major depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2018, 93, 20–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Labad, J.; Gutiérrez-Zotes, A.; Creus, M.; Montalvo, I.; Cabezas, Á.; Solé, M.; Ortega, L.; Algora, M.J.; Sánchez-Gistau, V.; Vilella, E. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis measures and cognitive abilities in early psychosis: Are there sex differences? Psychoneuroendocrinology 2016, 72, 54–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Spielberger, C. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI); Consulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA, USA, 1983; pp. 4–26. [Google Scholar]
  48. Buela-Casal, G.; Guillén-Riquelme, A.; Seisdedos Cubero, N. Cuestionario de Ansiedad Estado-Rasgo. Manual, 9th ed.; TEA Ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 2015; pp. 7–37. [Google Scholar]
  49. Buysse, D.J.; Reynolds, C.F.; Monk, T.H.; Berman, S.R.; Kupfer, D.J. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989, 28, 193–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Mollayeva, T.; Thurairajah, P.; Burton, K.; Mollayeva, S.; Shapiro, C.M.; Colantonio, A. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index as a screening tool for sleep dysfunction in clinical and non-clinical samples: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med. Rev. 2016, 25, 52–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Stalder, T.; Kirschbaum, C.; Kudielka, B.M.; Adam, E.K.; Pruessner, J.C.; Wüst, S.; Dockray, S.; Smyth, N.; Evans, P.; Hellhammer, D.H.; et al. Assessment of the cortisol awakening response: Expert consensus guidelines. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2016, 63, 414–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Pruessner, J.C.; Kirschbaum, C.; Meinlschmid, G.; Hellhammer, D.H. Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2003, 28, 916–931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Wüst, S.; Wolf, J.; Hellhammer, D.H.; Federenko, I.; Schommer, N.; Kirschbaum, C. The cortisol awakening response—Normal values and confounds. Noise Health 2000, 2, 79–88. [Google Scholar]
  54. Posener, J. Diurnal variation of plasma cortisol and homovanillic acid in healthy subjects. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1996, 21, 33–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Miller, R.; Plessow, F. Transformation techniques for cross-sectional and longitudinal endocrine data: Application to salivary cortisol concentrations. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2013, 38, 941–946. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Nebes, R.D.; Buysse, D.J.; Halligan, E.M.; Houck, P.R.; Monk, T.H. Self-Reported Sleep Quality Predicts Poor Cognitive Performance in Healthy Older Adults. J. Gerontol. Ser. B 2009, 64, 180–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Tsai, Y.-C.; Lu, H.-J.; Chang, C.-F.; Liang, W.-K.; Muggleton, N.G.; Juan, C.-H. Electrophysiological and behavioral evidence reveals the effects of trait anxiety on contingent attentional capture. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2017, 17, 973–983. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Arbune, A.; Lin, S.-H.; Chen, K.C.; Lee, I.H.; Chen, P.S.; Yang, Y.K. Positive association between neuroticism and working memory in healthy female volunteers. Int. J. Psychiatry Clin. Pract. 2014, 19, 75–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Kong, D.; Asplund, C.L.; Chee, M.W. Sleep deprivation reduces the rate of rapid picture processing. NeuroImage 2014, 91, 169–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Montag, C.; Reuter, M.; Jurkiewicz, M.; Markett, S.; Panksepp, J. Imaging the structure of the human anxious brain: A review of findings from neuroscientific personality psychology. Rev. Neurosci. 2013, 24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Takeuchi, H.; Taki, Y.; Nouchi, R.; Yokoyama, R.; Kotozaki, Y.; Nakagawa, S.; Sekiguchi, A.; Iizuka, K.; Yamamoto, Y.; Hanawa, S.; et al. Shorter sleep duration and better sleep quality are associated with greater tissue density in the brain. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 5833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. McKinnon, A.C.; Lagopoulos, J.; Terpening, Z.; Grunstein, R.; Hickie, I.B.; Batchelor, J.; Lewis, S.J.G.; Duffy, S.; Shine, J.M.; Naismith, S.L. Sleep disturbance in mild cognitive impairment is associated with alterations in the brain’s default mode network. Behav. Neurosci. 2016, 130, 305–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Bar, M. A Cortical Mechanism for Triggering Top-Down Facilitation in Visual Object Recognition. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2003, 15, 600–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Buchanan, T.W.; Kern, S.; Allen, J.S.; Tranel, D.; Kirschbaum, C. Circadian regulation of cortisol after hippocampal damage in humans. Biol. Psychiatry 2004, 56, 651–656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Duran, E.; Oyanedel, C.N.; Niethard, N.; Inostroza, M.; Born, J. Sleep stage dynamics in neocortex and hippocampus. Sleep 2018, 41, zsy060. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Stomby, A.; Boraxbekk, C.-J.; Lundquist, A.; Nordin, A.; Nilsson, L.-G.; Adolfsson, R.; Nyberg, L.; Olsson, C.-J.; Adolfsson, A.N. Higher diurnal salivary cortisol levels are related to smaller prefrontal cortex surface area in elderly men and women. Eur. J. Endocrinol. 2016, 175, 117–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  67. Wolkowitz, O.M.; Burke, H.; Epel, E.S.; Reus, V.I. Glucocorticoids: Mood, memory, and mechanisms. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2009, 1179, 19–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Nee, D.E.; Wager, T.D.; Jonides, J. Interference resolution: Insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging tasks. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2007, 7, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Abell, J.; Shipley, M.J.; Ferrie, J.E.; Kivimäki, M.; Kumari, M. Recurrent short sleep, chronic insomnia symptoms and salivary cortisol: A 10-year follow-up in the Whitehall II study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2016, 68, 91–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  70. Castro-Diehl, C.; Roux, A.V.D.; Redline, S.; Seeman, T.; Shrager, S.E.; Shea, S. Association of Sleep Duration and Quality with Alterations in the Hypothalamic-Pituitary Adrenocortical Axis: The Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis (MESA). J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2015, 100, 3149–3158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Hansen, Å.M.; Thomsen, J.F.; Kaergaard, A.; Kolstad, H.A.; Kaerlev, L.; Mors, O.; Rugulies, R.; Bonde, J.P.; Andersen, J.H.; Mikkelsen, S. Salivary cortisol and sleep problems among civil servants. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2012, 37, 1086–1095. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Lasikiewicz, N.; Hendrickx, H.; Talbot, D.; Dye, L. Exploration of basal diurnal salivary cortisol profiles in middle-aged adults: Associations with sleep quality and metabolic parameters. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2008, 33, 143–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Van Lenten, S.A.; Doane, L.D. Examining multiple sleep behaviors and diurnal salivary cortisol and alpha-amylase: Within- and between-person associations. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2016, 68, 100–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  74. Fekedulegn, D.; Innes, K.; Andrew, M.E.; Tinney-Zara, C.A.; Charles, L.E.; Allison, P.; Violanti, J.M.; Knox, S.S. Sleep quality and the cortisol awakening response (CAR) among law enforcement officers: The moderating role of leisure time physical activity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2018, 95, 158–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Stalder, T.; Hucklebridge, F.; Evans, P.; Clow, A. Use of a single case study design to examine state variation in the cortisol awakening response: Relationship with time of awakening. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2009, 34, 607–614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. For the Osteoporotic Fractures in Men (MrOS) Study Group; Blackwell, T.; Yaffe, K.; Laffan, A.; Ancoli-Israel, S.; Redline, S.; Ensrud, K.E.; Song, Y.; Stone, K.L. Associations of Objectively and Subjectively Measured Sleep Quality with Subsequent Cognitive Decline in Older Community-Dwelling Men: The MrOS Sleep Study. Sleep 2014, 37, 655–663. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Beluche, I.; Carrière, I.; Ritchie, K.; Ancelin, M.-L. A prospective study of diurnal cortisol and cognitive function in community-dwelling elderly people. Psychol. Med. 2009, 40, 1039–1049. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  78. Lupien, S.; Lecours, A.; Lussier, I.; Schwartz, G.; Nair, N.; Meaney, M. Basal cortisol levels and cognitive deficits in human aging. J. Neurosci. 1994, 14, 2893–2903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  79. Lo, J.C.; Loh, K.K.; Zheng, H.; Sim, S.K.; Chee, M.W. Sleep Duration and Age-Related Changes in Brain Structure and Cognitive Performance. Sleep 2014, 37, 821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. McEwen, B.S.; Nasca, C.; Gray, J.D. Stress Effects on Neuronal Structure: Hippocampus, Amygdala, and Prefrontal Cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology 2015, 41, 3–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  81. Van Someren, E.J. Circadian Rhythms and Sleep in Human Aging. Chronobiol. Int. 2000, 17, 233–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Sexton, C.E.; Zsoldos, E.; Filippini, N.; Griffanti, L.; Winkler, A.M.; Mahmood, A.; Allan, C.L.; Topiwala, A.; Kyle, S.D.; Spiegelhalder, K.; et al. Associations between self-reported sleep quality and white matter in community-dwelling older adults: A prospective cohort study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2017, 38, 5465–5473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  83. Malhotra, R.K.; Desai, A.K. Healthy Brain Aging: What Has Sleep Got to Do with It? Clin. Geriatr. Med. 2010, 26, 45–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Maglione, J.E.; Ancoli-Israel, S.; Peters, K.W.; Paudel, M.L.; Yaffe, K.; Ensrud, K.E.; Stone, K.L. Subjective and Objective Sleep Disturbance and Longitudinal Risk of Depression in a Cohort of Older Women. Sleep 2014, 37, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  85. Nuechterlein, K.H.; Green, M.F.; Kern, R.S.; Baade, L.E.; Barch, D.M.; Cohen, J.D.; Essock, S.; Fenton, W.S.; Frese, F.J.; Gold, J.M.; et al. The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, Part 1: Test Selection, Reliability, and Validity. Am. J. Psychiatry 2008, 165, 203–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  86. Van Rheenen, T.E.; Rossell, S.L. An empirical evaluation of the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2013, 16, 318–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Mohn, C.; Rund, B.R. Neurocognitive profile in major depressive disorders: Relationship to symptom level and subjective memory complaints. BMC Psychiatry 2016, 16, 108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Table 1. Sociodemographic and clinical data of the sample by sleep quality groups.
Table 1. Sociodemographic and clinical data of the sample by sleep quality groups.
Total
N = 203
Good Sleep Quality (PSQI < 5)
N = 108
Poor Sleep Quality
(PSQI ≥ 5)
N = 73
p Value
Female gender102 (56.4%)60 (55.6%)42 (57.5%)0.792
Age40.8 (17.7)39.6 (17.6)42.6 (17.8)0.272
Education level (years of study)13.3 (3.7)13.7 (3.7)12.7 (3.5)0.055
Smoking33 (18.4%)17 (15.9%)16 (22.2%)0.284
Cigarettes/day (in smokers)11.8 (10.0)11.2 (10.4)12.5 (9.9)0.710
Alcohol intake
 No99 (48.8%)50 (46.3%)39 (53.4%)0.340
 Occasional76 (37.4%)44 (40.7%)22 (30.1%)
 Daily28 (13.8%)14 (13.0%)12 (16.4%)
BMI (kg/m2)24.8 (4.8)24.3 (4.1)25.5 (5.6)0.130
Working status
 Active (student or worker)122 (67.4%)75 (69.4%)47 (64.4%)0.770
 Unemployed29 (16%)16 (14.8%)13 (17.8%)
 Retired30 (16.6%)17 (15.7%)13 (17.8%)
Civil status
 Single64 (35.4%)39 (36.1%)25 (34.2%)0.892
 Married or with stable couple95 (52.5%)57 (52.8%)38 (52.1%)
 Separated or divorced14 (7.7%)7 (6.5%)7 (9.6%)
 Widow8 (4.4%)5 (4.6%)3 (4.1%)
Living
 Alone51 (28.2%)28 (25.9%)23 (31.5%)0.547
 With origin family37 (20.4%)25 (23.1%)12 (16.4%)
 With own family or couple77 (42.5%)47 (43.5%)30 (41.1%)
 With others (friends, other family)16 (8.8%)8 (7.4%)8 (11.0%)
Psychometric instruments
 PSQI total score4.6 (2.9)2.7 (1.1)7.3 (2.6)<0.001
 STAI-State anxiety11.2 (7.2)10.8 (6.6)11.9 (7.9)0.303
 STAI-Trait anxiety13.9 (8.3)12.0 (6.7)16.9 (9.6)<0.001
Data are mean (standard deviation) or N (%). Abbreviations: PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; BMI, body mass index; STAI, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory.
Table 2. HPA axis measures by sleep quality groups.
Table 2. HPA axis measures by sleep quality groups.
Total
N = 203
Good Sleep Quality
(PSQI < 5)
N = 108
Poor Sleep Quality
(PSQI ≥ 5)
N = 73
p Value
Cortisol values (nmol/L)
 Cortisol at awakening16.8 (10.5)17.8 (11.0)15.5 (9.6)0.090
 Cortisol 30’ post awakening25.0 (13.5)26.1 (13.7)23.5 (13.2)0.212
 Cortisol 60’ post awakening20.6 (12.5)21.3 (13.1)19.7 (11.4)0.439
 Cortisol at 10 a.m.12.9 (10.3)12.9 (11.0)12.7 (9.3)0.984
 Cortisol at 11 p.m.3.4 (4.1)3.4 (5.0)3.3 (2.4)0.296
HPA axis measures
 CAR (AUCi)31.1 (60.7)31.0 (57.9)31.0 (65.9)0.996
 Diurnal cortisol slope−0.73 (0.69)−0.72 (0.68)−0.73 (0.69)0.450
 Cortisol levels during the day (AUCg)8593.2 (5944.5)8725.4 (6678.8)8409.3 (4780.0)0.737
Data are mean (standard deviation). Abbreviations: HPA, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal; PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; CAR, cortisol awakening response, AUCi, area under the curve calculated with respect to the increase; AUCg, area under the curve calculated with respect to the ground.
Table 3. Cognitive assessment by sleep quality groups.
Table 3. Cognitive assessment by sleep quality groups.
Total
N = 203
Good Sleep Quality
(PSQI < 5)
N = 108
Poor Sleep Quality
(PSQI ≥ 5)
N = 73
p Value
Verbal learning and memory
 HVLT-R25.8 (5.0)26.6 (5.1)24.5 (4.7)0.007
Visual learning and memory
 BVMT-R25.2 (7.1)26.3 (6.9)23.7 (7.1)0.016
 RCFT-copy32.6 (5.6)32.7 (5.7)32.4 (5.3)0.664
 RCFT-immediate recall19.9 (7.1)20.8 (7.1)18.7 (6.9)0.055
 RCFT-delayed recall20.2 (7.1)21.1 (7.2)18.9 (6.9)0.041
Working memory
 CBTT (nonverbal)15.7 (3.8)16.1 (4.0)15.2 (3.4)0.138
 LNS (verbal)14.1 (3.2)14.4 (3.0)13.7 (3.4)0.196
Processing speed
 TMT-A (seconds)36.0 (20.8)33.9 (16.8)29.0 (25.3)0.104
 BACS-SC54.8 (15.1)55.5 (15.4)53.7 (14.7)0.440
 Category fluency24.5 (5.9)24.6 (5.8)24.5 (6.1)0.983
 Stroop direct W105.9 (16.6)108.7 (17.0)102.0 (15.4)0.008
 Stroop direct C71.9 (11.8)72.7 (11.9)70.6 (11.6)0.246
Attention/vigilance
 CPT-IP2.66 (0.72)2.8 (0.7)2.5 (0.7)0.022
Executive function
 TMT-B (seconds)69.1 (41.3)66.1 (43.2)73.5 (38.2)0.085
 NAB-mazes18.2 (6.8)18.9 (6.8)17.0 (6.5)0.061
 Stroop direct WC47.2 (12.2)48.6 (11.6)45.3 (12.9)0.077
 Stroop direct interference4.6 (8.8)5.2 (8.0)3.8 (9.8)0.291
Data are mean (standard deviation). Abbreviations: HVLT-R, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised; BVMT-R, Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised; RCFT, Rey Complex Figure Test; CBTT, Corsi Block-Tapping Test; LNS, Letter Number Span; TMT-A, Trail Making Test part A; BACS-SC, Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia-Symbol Coding; W, words; C, colors; CPT-IP, Continuous Performance Test-Identical Pairs; TMT-B, Trail Making Test part B; NAB-Mazes, Neuropsychological Assessment Battery-Mazes; WC, words-colors. TMT-A and TMT-B raw scores are shown. p values calculated upon natural log-transformed variables. As both tests are measured in seconds, higher scores reflect poorer cognitive performance.
Table 4. Results of the multiple linear regression analyses exploring the relationship between sleep quality, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis measures, trait anxiety and cognitive functioning in healthy individuals.
Table 4. Results of the multiple linear regression analyses exploring the relationship between sleep quality, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis measures, trait anxiety and cognitive functioning in healthy individuals.
Sleep Quality (PSQI)CAR (AUCi)Diurnal Cortisol SlopeCortisol Levels during the Day (AUCg)STAI-TSignificant Interactions
βpβpβpβpβp
Verbal learning and memory
 HVLT-R−0.220.007−0.150.263−0.060.442−0.110.188−0.070.370PSQI × CAR (β = 0.26, p = 0.043)
Visual learning and memory
 BVMT-R−0.430.004−0.310.032−0.080.315−0.040.645−0.410.009PSQI × CAR (β = 0.29, p = 0.037); PSQI × STAI-T (β = 0.49, p = 0.034)
 RCFT-copy0.030.7430.000.979−0.020.793−0.020.866−0.130.162NS
 RCFT-immediate recall−0.080.298−0.040.697−0.160.050−0.070.403−0.170.039NS
 RCFT-delayed recall−0.100.188−0.020.823−0.100.203−0.070.391−0.150.047NS
Working memory
 CBTT (nonverbal)0.030.699−0.010.885−0.090.284−0.120.193−0.020.812NS
 LNS (verbal)−0.140.113−0.370.018−0.110.2080.000.995−0.120.157NS
Processing speed
 TMT-A −0.260.0280.250.0510.040.543−0.260.0300.080.247PSQI × AUCg all day (β = 0.51, p = 0.001); PSQI × CAR (β = −0.28, p = 0.025)
 BACS-SC0.050.371−0.040.5360.000.993−0.030.592−0.100.091NS
 Category fluency0.070.386−0.100.303−0.150.091−0.030.739−0.110.193NS
 Stroop direct W−0.090.2900.060.5730.030.7340.070.465−0.040.660NS
 Stroop direct C0.040.626−0.050.653−0.040.6770.020.866−0.160.080NS
Attention/vigilance
 CPT-IP−0.160.0510.030.7970.030.7580.020.7910.010.920NS
Executive function
 TMT-B 0.310.0390.060.6870.040.530−0.070.7740.440.004PSQI × STAI-T (β = −0.55, p = 0.017)
 NAB-mazes−0.040.5280.020.768−0.090.2110.040.641−0.140.038NS
 Stroop direct WC0.340.027−0.090.400−0.150.1150.360.019−0.090.312PSQI × CAR (β = −0.52, p = 0.007)
 Stroop direct interference0.310.049−0.130.243−0.170.0840.310.047−0.030.704PSQI × CAR (β = −0.47, p = 0.017)
Abbreviations: PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; CAR, cortisol awakening response; AUCi, area under the curve calculated with respect to the increase; AUCg, area under the curve calculated with respect to the ground; STAI-T, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory trait anxiety subscore; HVLT-R, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised; BVMT-R, Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised; RCFT, Rey Complex Figure Test; CBTT, Corsi Block-Tapping Test; LNS, Letter Number Span; TMT-A, Trail Making Test part A; BACS-SC, Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia-Symbol Coding; CPT-IP, Continuous Performance Test-Identical Pairs; TMT-B, Trail Making Test part B; NAB-Mazes, Neuropsychological Assessment Battery-Mazes; NS, not significant. TMT-A and TMT-B and measured in seconds. For these two cognitive tests, higher scores indicate poorer cognitive performance.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Labad, J.; Salvat-Pujol, N.; Armario, A.; Cabezas, Á.; de Arriba-Arnau, A.; Nadal, R.; Martorell, L.; Urretavizcaya, M.; Monreal, J.A.; Crespo, J.M.; et al. The Role of Sleep Quality, Trait Anxiety and Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis Measures in Cognitive Abilities of Healthy Individuals. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 7600. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17207600

AMA Style

Labad J, Salvat-Pujol N, Armario A, Cabezas Á, de Arriba-Arnau A, Nadal R, Martorell L, Urretavizcaya M, Monreal JA, Crespo JM, et al. The Role of Sleep Quality, Trait Anxiety and Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis Measures in Cognitive Abilities of Healthy Individuals. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2020; 17(20):7600. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17207600

Chicago/Turabian Style

Labad, Javier, Neus Salvat-Pujol, Antonio Armario, Ángel Cabezas, Aida de Arriba-Arnau, Roser Nadal, Lourdes Martorell, Mikel Urretavizcaya, José Antonio Monreal, José Manuel Crespo, and et al. 2020. "The Role of Sleep Quality, Trait Anxiety and Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis Measures in Cognitive Abilities of Healthy Individuals" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, no. 20: 7600. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17207600

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop