Next Article in Journal
Particulate Matter Emissions of Four Different Cigarette Types of One Popular Brand: Influence of Tobacco Strength and Additives
Previous Article in Journal
How Does CEO’s Environmental Awareness Affect Technological Innovation?
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

A Cross-Sectional Description of Parental Perceptions and Practices Related to Risky Play and Independent Mobility in Children: The New Zealand State of Play Survey

1
Human Potential Centre, School of Sport and Recreation, Auckland University of Technology, Auckland 0632, New Zealand
2
Department of Pediatrics, School of Population and Public Health, University of British Columbia, British Columbia Children’s’ Hospital Research Institute, British Columbia Injury Research and Prevention Unit, Vancouver, BC V6H 3V4, Canada
3
Professor and Department Head, Occupational Therapy, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, CO 80523, USA
4
Professor of Occupational Therapy, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW 2006, Australia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16(2), 262; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16020262
Submission received: 21 November 2018 / Revised: 9 January 2019 / Accepted: 11 January 2019 / Published: 17 January 2019

Abstract

:
The potential for risky play and independent mobility to increase children’s physical activity, and enhance cognitive development and emotional wellbeing has been recognised for some time. The aim of this study was to describe the attitudes of New Zealand parents towards such risky play practices and independent mobility, the barriers preventing them from allowing their children to participate, and how often their children engaged in risky play activities. An online survey comprised mostly of validated scales and standardised questions was completed by a nationally representative sample of 2003 parents. We found that parents had neutral feelings about the risk of injury to their child through play, rather they were concerned about road safety and “stranger danger”. There was strong agreement that there are multiple benefits to be gained from exposure to risk and challenge, and that health and safety rules are too strict. However, 73% of respondents stated that their 5–12 year old child seldom or never engaged in four or more risky activities, and only 14.3% engaged in four or more often or always. While parents agree that their child is likely to benefit from risky play, they do not have the confidence to allow their children to engage in such activities. Future research should address barriers and fears when implementing strategies to facilitate risky play.

1. Introduction

Physical activity is essential for both physiological and psychological healthy development in children [1,2]. Reports suggest that, on average, New Zealand children [3] and adolescents [4] currently meet the global recommendations for moderate-vigorous physical activity (MVPA) of 60 minutes per day, five days a week [5]. However, results from the New Zealand Report Card on Physical Activity for Children and Youth indicate that, in parallel with global trends [6], these levels are decreasing [7]. Given that physical activity habits in early childhood are likely to predict those in adulthood [8,9], it is pertinent to promote strategies that encourage and increase physical activity in children.
While the benefits of play have been documented for some time [10,11], the value of play has only relatively recently been formally recognised as being vital for a child’s healthy emotional, social and intellectual development [12]. In fact, given the strong evidence that all types of play contribute to a child’s self-esteem, behaviour regulation, emotional expression, cognitive and motor skills, resilience and mental well-being, [12,13] play is identified as a child’s right in the United Nations Convention on the rights of the child [14]. Risky, outdoor play—play that is challenging, thrilling, and involves some physical risk [15]—has been cited as a potential allure to engage children in physical activity, potentially increasing MVPA [16,17] and improving psychological well-being [13], [18]. Emerging literature has identified six categories of risky play: play with great heights, play with high speed, play with dangerous tools (e.g., hammer, saw), play near dangerous elements (e.g., water, fire), rough-and-tumble play (e.g., play fighting) and play where children can disappear or get lost (in older children, this is sometimes referred to in the literature as independent mobility) [19].
Given the complex, multi-factorial nature of children’s development, and the inherent difficulty in monitoring play practices and their effects, it has been difficult to discern whether there is a causal link between engagement in risky play and long-term physical and mental well-being. Despite this, most studies indicate that substantial benefits [10,11,20] are likely to be gained, with few showing, at least in the short term, negative consequences of minor injury, verbal disputes or physical conflict resolution [15,21,22]. Moreover, it could be argued that the importance of avoiding acute physical harm (often minor) is prioritised over the prevention of long-term psychological distress [23]. There is an argument that autonomy and risk management skills are a requirement for healthy development. For example, there is an association between reduced opportunity for all aspects of risky play and increased incidence of behavioural and psychological issues in children and young adults. Furthermore, profound positive effects of free play interventions have been observed in children with ADHD [24,25]; however, these conclusions have not been demonstrated empirically.
Despite these potential benefits, opportunities to engage in risky play behaviours have declined over the last three decades [26,27,28]. In many developed nations, participation in what is considered risky play has shifted to structured, supervised, and/or indoor activities [29,30,31]. This appears to be, in part, the consequence of increasing concerns about child safety and accountability issues [32,33,34]. Such concerns have also led to increased restrictions of children’s independent mobility (roaming the neighbourhood unsupervised), active transport to/from school, and leisure activities with friends [27,31,35]. The decrease in independent mobility is a concern because not only is this a convenient way to accumulate MVPA [36], but also by learning to navigate the neighbourhood, skills such as problem solving and decision making, for example when crossing roads, are developed [37]. Additionally, when children interact with peers away from adults they are compelled to resolve their own conflicts thus fostering social competence [38]. Notably, statistics indicate that incidents of ‘stranger danger’ have not changed over time [39,40,41,42], and current advice is actually to dismiss this mantra as the overwhelming majority of abductions, which in any case are rare, are made by adults whom the child knows [43] and not strangers. Furthermore, while traffic has increased, child road fatality in New Zealand has actually dropped over the last 30 years [44].
Numerous studies indicate that caregivers, teachers, and some policy-makers acknowledge the benefits of risky play [45,46,47] and independent mobility [48,49], yet whether or not children are allowed to engage in such activities seems to be dependent on the child’s age [34], cultural norms [50,51,52], and local policy [46,53]. For instance, as children grow older, the number of risky activities in which they are permitted to participate increases [34,54]. In addition, teacher and parent willingness to encourage children’s participation in risky play appears to align with the strictness of the safety standards of regulated institutions, such as nurseries and schools [55].
In New Zealand, there has been a notable decline in children’s outdoor play and active travel [4,28], but we still have no descriptive information about the perceptions and practices related to risky play. The aim of this study was to describe how a large sample of New Zealand parents perceive risky play behaviours in children, including children’s independent mobility, as well as the level of risky play permitted by parents. We hypothesised that risky play and independent mobility would be perceived as beneficial for children, but that this perception would not necessarily be reflected in parenting practice.

2. Materials and Methods

This study reports a descriptive analysis of the results of the New Zealand State of Play Survey, a cross-sectionally implemented questionnaire. Ethical approval was obtained by Auckland University of Technology Ethics Committee (AUTEC; 15/227 The way we use to play: Promoting free play in New Zealand children. Approved 6 July 2015). This study has been reported according to the STROBE guidelines [56]. The checklist can be found in the Supplementary Materials File S1 STROBE checklist.

2.1. Participants and Setting

The Smile City Research Panel (Kantar TNS Global, Auckland, New Zealand branch), a database of individuals residing in New Zealand, was used to identify active members listed as having at least one dependent child younger than 18 years. Recruitment and data collection were carried out by an independent market research company (TNS Global) between 19–31 August 2015.

2.2. Eligibility and Inclusion Criteria

Participants were excluded from the analyses if (1) they stated in the first screening question that they were not a parent or caregiver over the age of 18 with at least one dependent child, or (2) they did not complete the survey. Recruitment and inclusion flow are depicted in Figure 1. Using TNS Global in-house methodology, 13,400 eligible panel members were randomly selected to take part. In total, 2546 (19%) questionnaires were returned, with 2003 (14.9%) meeting the minimum criteria for analysis. A total of 1573 (11.7%) respondents had children aged 5–12 who were referred to the risky play questionnaires, and 1366 respondents had children aged 9–10 about whom independent mobility questions could be answered.

2.3. Procedures

Email invitations were sent to potential participants who were asked to fill out an online survey via the Dimensions (IBM) platform using a point-and-click interface, visually and functionally similar to a paper-based questionnaire. Some questions referred to children of a certain age; parents without children in this age-range were automatically skipped through these sections of the survey.

2.4. State of Play Survey

The State of Play Survey, funded by Persil NZ, comprised several questionnaires designed to determine parents’ perceptions and practices of risky play and independent mobility (please see Supplementary Materials File S2 State of Play Questionnaire). Risky play was defined as play meeting the characteristics described by Sandseter et al. [56] (Table 1). After consultation with play experts, two further categories were included: play with loose objects (e.g., sticks, timber, tyres, tarpaulins) and ‘messy’ play [57].

2.4.1. Tolerance for Risk in Play Scale

The Tolerance for Risk in Play Scale (TRiPS) [58] was developed to ascertain parents’ tolerance for children to experience risk during play. The TRiPS is a 32-item questionnaire that asks respondents whether or not they do allow their children to engage in activities pertaining to each category in Table 1. A few questions also refer to supervision and child injury risk, everyday risk propensity and parental over protection. The survey asks parents to consider their responses to various risky scenarios with reference to their eldest child aged between 5–12 years (parents of children aged 13–18 years were automatically skipped through this part of the survey). Sample items include: “Would you let the child go head first down a slippery dip?” and “Would you let the child walk on slippery rocks close to water?” Parents answer questions either “yes” or “no”. “No” answers are scored 0 and yes answers are scored from 1–12 weighted according to their acceptability or difficulty to endorse based on a Rasch analysis of a validation study [58], with the least acceptable statements scoring higher than the most acceptable statements. It should be noted that while higher scores indicate greater risk tolerance, this scale is not strictly continuous as not all values between 0 and 184 are possible totals.

2.4.2. Risk Engagement and Protection Survey

The Risk Engagement and Protection Survey (REPS) [59] was designed to help understand the views and attitudes of parents towards protecting children from injury and allowing them to engage in risks. To date, it has only been validated for fathers [58], although is currently being assessed on mothers. Therefore, given that there is currently no equivalent scale for female caregivers, both male and female respondents were asked to answer this section of the survey. The survey contains 12 items: six relating to protection from injury and six relating to risk engagement. Participants are asked to rate how much they agree or disagree with each statement using a seven-point Likert scale anchored with “very strongly agree” and “very strongly disagree”.

2.4.3. Perception of Positive Potentiality of Outdoor Autonomy for Children Scale

An adapted version of the Perception of Positive Potentiality of Outdoor Autonomy for Children (PPOAC) scale [60] was included to establish the extent to which parents believed that roaming the neighbourhood was positive or negative for 9–10 year-olds as this is the age group in which the questionnaire has been validated in. Answers are given on a four-point Likert scale ranging from “strongly agree” to “strongly disagree”. There are nine items meaning that possible scores range from nine to 36. Sample items include: “Become more responsible” and “Encounter ill-intentioned adults”. For analysis, negative statements were reverse coded so that ‘positive’ attitudes resulted in positive score increases.

2.4.4. Extraneous Barriers to Risky Play

Participants were asked to state to what extent they agreed with statements regarding barriers to allowing children to engage in risky play. Responses were rated on a five-point Likert scale from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree”. Examples of these statements include: “Finding ways to get children active is expensive these days” and “There are too many unnecessary safety rules in New Zealand schools” (see Supplementary Materials File S2; Q6).
Two questions relating to playing in the rain were included in the survey. The first pertaining to the frequency with which a child plays in the rain was rated on a five-point scale from “never” to “always”. The second suggested eight reasons a child might not be allowed to play in the rain and respondents were asked to indicate which might influence their decision. Items covered concerns about sickness and injury, appropriate clothing and dislike for being in the rain. There was also an option for “other”.

2.4.5. Participation in Risky Play Activities

To determine whether perceptions of risky play changed depending on a child’s age, participants were asked at which age (if at all) a child should first be allowed to engage in each category of risky play. Example items include: “Climb trees at home or in their local park or recreation areas” or “Use adult tools (e.g., hammer, saws, drills) at home”. The purpose of asking parents to state the appropriate age for each play/mobility category is that it enabled a relatively graded response facilitating comparisons of perceptions among categories. They were then asked to indicate on a five-point Likert scale, how often (from never through seldom, sometimes and often to always) their eldest child aged 5–12 engages in a selection of activities with varying degrees of supervision. Six of the activities were examples of each of the risky play categories described by Sandseter [19] for example: “How often does your child use adult tools?” (see Supplementary Materials File S2; Q13). Parents were also asked how often their child rides non-motorised vehicles in three different scenarios: under the supervision of adults, with friends or alone. Two items were specifically included to gain an understanding of the child’s independent mobility in terms of roaming the neighbourhood.

2.4.6. Active Transportation and Independent Mobility

Independent mobility is a sub-category of risky play as children considered “streetwise” appear to be trusted by their parents to make responsible decisions if faced with risky situations [61]. Respondents were asked questions about how their eldest child between age 5–12 normally travels to and from school, who they travel with and, for children aged 9–10, under what circumstances, if any, they are allowed to travel to other destinations unsupervised by adults. Questions were adapted from those used in a previous report comparing the independent mobility of children in England and Germany between 1971–2010 [52]. Respondents were asked to answer “yes”, “no”, or “N/A” to six items such as “Is your child usually allowed to go out alone after dark?” and “Is your child usually allowed to travel on local buses alone (other than a school bus)?” This questionnaire has recently been validated [62].

2.5. Statistical Analysis

Cross-sectional, descriptive analyses were carried out using R V3.5.0 (R Core Team (2013). Vienna, Austria). Survey response distributions were checked for normality using visual interpretation of histograms or, if necessary, Shapiro-Wilk tests. Where the assumption of normality was violated, medians and interquartile ranges are reported. The number of people who rated each level for each item was counted for all questionnaires. In addition, the following analyses were conducted. Quartiles were calculated for TRiPS scores to create four categories of risk tolerance: risk averse, somewhat risk averse, somewhat risk tolerant, risk tolerant. The proportion of “yes” answers of each weighting was then calculated for each category. For the REPS, the number of items rated high (>6) or low (<2) out of seven by each participant was counted. Ratings for each answer in the PPOAC scale were summed to produce a total score out of 36. The number and proportion of respondents agreeing with each barrier to playing in the rain was calculated. Furthermore, the number of activities participated in at all (independent mobility), or frequently (>5) or infrequently (<2; risky play) was counted. All figures were created using R package ggplot2 [63].

3. Results

3.1. Participants

Participant characteristics are summarised in Table 2. Compared to the New Zealand population, the sample was representative in terms of number of children, ethnicity, household income and dwelling location [64].

3.2. Survey Questionnaires

Summary statistics for survey scores are presented in Table 3. Of the final sample, 1573 (78.5%) completed the validated questionnaires in full. Independent mobility information was available for 1366 participants.

3.2.1. Tolerance for Risk in Play

Median (LQ–UQ) score on the TRiPS scale was 95 (61–122) out of 184. A median of 17 (6–23) out of 30 statements were answered “yes”, indicating that, on average, parents would allow their child to engage in 55% of the included risky play activities. TRiPS total scores associated with the risk category quartiles are presented in Figure 2a and Table 3. As expected, as item weighting increased, the number of respondents agreeing with the statement decreased Figure 2b). Respondents categorised as risk averse (score ≥ 41) tended to answer more, lower-scoring questions rather than a few higher-scoring questions (Figure 2b).

3.2.2. Risk Engagement and Protection

Distribution of answers for injury prevention and risk engagement are presented in Figure 3a,b, respectively. Most parents disagreed to some extent that they were concerned about hazards in the home. Almost exactly half (50.2%) of parents agreed or strongly agreed that good supervision means knowing what their child is doing at all times. The remaining four items relating to injury prevention had a median rating of “neither agree nor disagree”. Of those who had a complete set of survey answers, only 3.3% very strongly disagreed or strongly disagreed with most (four or more) items that state that injuries should and could be avoided by intensive supervision. Similarly, only 3.4% very strongly agreed or strongly agreed with this sentiment. Most respondents (87.1%) marked most statements between three to five out of seven suggesting moderate views on avoiding injury by managing play environments.
All risk engagement statements had a median rating of “agree” demonstrating that overall, risk engagement was viewed positively. For instance, whereas only 0.7% of respondents very strongly or strongly disagreed with statements proposing that risk and challenge were beneficial, 28.6% marked most statements “strongly agree”, or “very strongly agree”. This indicates that perceptions towards risk engagement are strong and positive for a significant proportion of individuals.

3.2.3. Perception of Positive Potentiality of Outdoor Autonomy for Children (PPOAC)

The number of parents responding with each rating to the items in the PPOAC scale is presented in Figure 4. The median score in the PPOAC survey was 24 indicating that most parents perceived outdoor autonomy as “likely” to be positive. Overall, statements regarding the potential for outdoor autonomy to encourage childhood development were rated as “likely”. However, while 70.8% of parents thought it was unlikely that a child would feel disorientated, 73.2%, 56.3% and 59.9% believed that road accidents, stranger danger and seeing scary things, respectively, was likely or very likely. Interestingly, though, 74.6% of parents thought it was likely or very likely that their child would be able to find someone to help in case of trouble.

3.2.4. Extraneous Barriers to Risky Play

All 2003 participants had a complete set of responses for questions relating to perceptions of health and safety. Ratings are captured in Figure 5. More than half the respondents either “agreed” or “strongly agreed” that there are too many health and safety rules in schools (55.9%) and regarding children’s play in general (60.4%). More than three quarters (78.6%) of participants “agreed” or “strongly agreed” that regular exposure to risk develops risk management skills and 74.9% “agreed” or “strongly agreed” that exposure to actual risk enhances children’s development. Furthermore, only 24.0% “agreed” or “strongly agreed” that relaxing rules would increase the risk of serious injury.
Nearly 14% of parents said that they never let their children play in the rain (see Figure 6a). There did not appear to be strong opposition to allowing children to play in the rain, however, with 57.8% of parents always, often or sometimes allowing it. Of the provided options, the most commonly selected barriers were that rain would cause children to get sick (N = 811) and make them too cold (N = 779). Half this number of parents (N = 405) believed that their child(ren) did not like playing in the rain. Getting messy (N = 179) and not having appropriate clothing (N = 134 child; N = 52) did not emerge as primary concerns (see Figure 6b).

3.2.5. Participation in Risky Play Categories

Results showed that the older children get, the more likely parents are to allow them to participate in risky play. Most children (73.4%) were allowed to engage in messy play by age three; climb trees (67.4%), engage in rough and tumble play (60.9%), and ride non-motorised vehicles (54.1%) at age five, and use adult tools (61.9%) at home by age nine. Most children were not allowed to roam the neighbourhood unsupervised by adults (but with friends; 67.6%) until age 13, or alone (64.9%) until age 15.
Fewer than half of parents reported that their children participate in at least one of the six activities (see Figure 7) “often” or “always”. Moreover, it was more common for parents to report that their child “seldom” or “never” participates in at least five of the eight activities than for them to engage in them “often” or “always” (27.2% vs. 14.3%). However, parents were more likely to allow their children to engage in the risky play activities: climbing trees, playing with tools and loose parts, engaging in rough and tumble, riding bikes and messy play, than roaming the neighbourhood alone. Most parents allow their children to participate in most of the risky play activities at least sometimes or often or always. This was not the case for independent mobility, however, with only 14.5% of children allowed to roam the neighbourhood with friends sometimes, often or always. This proportion dropped to 6.7% if the child was alone.

3.2.6. Active Transportation and Independent Mobility

Data were available for 1366 participants who had children aged nine or ten. The mode by which and with whom children travel to school is presented in Table 4. Only 364 (26.7%) children traveled without an adult, and 118 of these (32.4%) used the bus where there is some degree of supervision from the driver. Of the 725 (53.1%) children who traveled actively, 320 (44.1%) traveled either alone (157; 21.7%) or with siblings or friends (163; 22.5%).
Fewer than half of respondents indicated that their child was allowed to roam the neighbourhood, or cross main roads alone. Fewer than one fifth were allowed to cycle on main roads and fewer than one eighth were allowed to use the bus for travel other than to school. Just 4% were allowed out alone after dark.

4. Discussion

This study represents the first observation of a large, nationally representative sample of New Zealand parents’ perceptions of risky play and the frequency with which their children engage in risky play activities. Using recently validated questionnaires, we report a rich set of results covering multiple aspects of risky play and independent mobility perceptions and practice. We found that, overall, parents perceived exposure to risk as beneficial for children’s development. However, participation in risky play activities was limited with most children only ‘sometimes’ taking part in these types of activities. As expected, as children grew older, parents were more willing to allow them to engage in risky play. With regard to health and safety, parents thought that there were too many rules and that relaxing them was unlikely to increase injuries. Interestingly, while parents were neutral about whether less supervision would increase the risk of injuries, they believed that good supervision meant knowing what their child is doing at all times. Furthermore, the dangers of unsupervised roaming were rated as likely and children aged 5–12 years were seldom or never allowed to roam the neighbourhood without adults. Fewer than half of respondents afforded their children independent mobility to roam the neighbourhood on foot, with this number decreasing exponentially when asked about using bicycles, the bus and going out after dark.
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to assess New Zealand parents’ attitudes towards risky play quantitatively using validated questionnaires. The finding that there was a dichotomy between the perceived benefits of risky play and actual participation is consistent with views of parents from Australia [23,65] who revealed in semi-structured interviews that they find it difficult to balance the importance of taking risks with managing safety. The general consensus that risk is beneficial, injury unlikely, but supervision necessary, demonstrates a lack of confidence in parents’ facilitation of risky activities. Indeed, fear of being judged to be a bad parent has been reported to be one motivator of intensive supervision [66]. We found that parents did not regard the cost of activities as a barrier. Instead, perhaps paradoxically, whereas health and safety rules were considered too strict, parents were also strongly concerned about the risk of road traffic accidents and stranger danger, suggesting that enforced safety measures do not address parent’s concerns. This is supported by qualitative studies from New Zealand where increased traffic volume is identified as one barrier to allowing children to play unsupervised or use non-motorised vehicles outside, particularly in more affluent areas [28,67]. Fear of encounters with ill-intentioned adults have also been echoed in a number of studies and may partly stem from a reduced sense of community with family units believing themselves to be more isolated from their neighbours than they used to be. Parents report that when they were young, everyone looked out for each other’s children, but that this type of neighbourhood surveillance does not exist as much anymore [28,68]. This was also found in a study from Australia, with perceived “social control” predicting independent mobility [69].
Parents’ views that there are too many health and safety rules applied to children’s play contrasts with those of early childhood educators who believe that one of the strongest drivers for restrictions on play equipment is pressure from parents [23,70]. Many examples of parents blaming teachers for minor injuries their children have sustained have been reported [45] with isolated cases appearing to dictate institutional policy [71]. Sentiments expressed in previous studies suggest that parental identity, such as the role of protector [72] and social judgement [23,34,66] are important motivators in parents’ decisions regarding their children’s freedom. Such conflicting views highlight societal pressures to be viewed as though one is caring appropriately for the children within their charge, as opposed to actually allowing what is best for the child. It is vital, therefore, that institutions of power and influence such as the media, playground inspectors and local authorities set examples and standards that consider the long-term well-being of the child rather than the acute social discomfort of the caregiver [50].
It was not possible in this study to discern the reasons why respondent parents’ children do not regularly participate in risky play. However, comments from North American [30] and Norwegian [73] parents discussing outdoor free play highlight busier school schedules (e.g., homework, music, organised sport) and increased use of technology as barriers to unstructured play. Notably, these activities were not necessarily viewed as detracting from children’s development, rather the skills gained thought of as essential for the future and preventative of troublesome behaviour [30], a sentiment also captured by a New Zealand study [28].
To date, the research pertaining to risky play practices (including outdoor, nature and unsupervised real play) has mostly been qualitative and/or has employed questionnaires developed specifically for individual studies [73,74]. Risk perception has been shown to be culturally specific and it is, therefore, difficult to compare degrees of risk tolerance. For example, parents and teachers from Norway and Canada appear to be less risk averse than those from the USA and Australia [30,51,75]. Due to its geographical topography, with the ocean and bush accessible to a large proportion of the population, in the international context New Zealand is also likely to emerge as a relatively risk tolerant society although, until now, this has been impossible to demonstrate empirically. Nonetheless, New Zealand residents still feel as though attitudes towards children’s play are restrictive. We believe this study is a valuable addition to the literature as the use of three validated questionnaires mean that the results can be compared at an international level.
While the use of validated questionnaires is a strength of this investigation, currently there are no other studies that have reported analogous results and, therefore, we have no way of comparing our findings in the wider context. In addition, using Likert scale responses, especially four- and five-point scales is restrictive when attempting to understand participant opinion. Furthermore, respondents were not always given the option to add items or activities that were not listed. Therefore, influences that they felt were important or other risky activities that their children took part in were not captured in this study, and thus there is a possibility that this report underestimates participation in risky activities. Correspondingly, while this study suggests that parents are open to the idea of risky play, it neither discerned to what extent nor provided possible solutions as to how to facilitate engagement. It would also be useful to understand what factors predict participation, and future research should attempt to identify which socio-demographic and individual characteristics influence risky play practices. It should also be noted that there are inherent limitations to online survey methodology including response rates, just 19% in this study, with the possibility that those with the strongest opinions on the topic respond.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, we found that parents believe that risky play enables numerous benefits to children’s development, but many do not allow their children to take part in such activities. Given that this type of play has great potential to improve both physical and psychological health, both pertinent issues increasingly affecting each generation of children [76,77], this topic is worthy of further investigation. Future studies should thus investigate what modifiable factors predict participation in risky play, as well as investigate the influence of cultural norms and societal pressures so that the apparent, inhibited desire of parents to allow their children more freedom may be facilitated and accepted.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/1660-4601/16/2/262/s1, Table S1: Health and safety questions, Table S2: Engagement in risky play activities.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.D. and M.B.; methodology, S.D. and C.J.; formal analysis, C.J.; data curation, Kantar TNS.; writing—original draft preparation, C.J.; writing—review and editing, S.D., J.M., M.B. and A.B.; visualization, C.J.; project administration, J.M.; funding acquisition, S.D.

Funding

This study was generously funded by Persil, a Unilever brand.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Persil for their contribution to this research. We would also like to thank the staff at Kantar TNS who were assigned to this project.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; or in the writing of the manuscript. Approval was gained from Persil to publish the results on 26 September 2018.

References

  1. Biddle, S.J.H.; Asare, M. Physical activity and mental health in children and adolescents: A review of reviews. Br. J. Sports Med. 2011, 45, 886–895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Poitras, V.J.; Gray, C.E.; Borghese, M.M.; Carson, V.; Chaput, J.-P.; Janssen, I.; Katzmarzyk, P.T.; Pate, R.R.; Gorber, S.C.; Kho, M.E.; et al. Systematic review of the relationships between objectively measured physical activity and health indicators in school-aged children and youth. Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. 2016, 41, S197–S239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Farmer, V.L.; Williams, S.M.; Mann, J.I.; Schofield, G.; McPhee, J.C.; Taylor, R.W. The effect of increasing risk and challenge in the school playground on physical activity and weight in children: A cluster randomised controlled trial (PLAY). Int. J. Obes. 2017, 41, 793–800. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Hinckson, E.; Cerin, E.; Mavoa, S.; Smith, M.; Badland, H.; Stewart, T.; Duncan, S.; Schofield, G. Associations of the perceived and objective neighborhood environment with physical activity and sedentary time in New Zealand adolescents. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2017, 14, 145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Worl Health Organisation. WHO | Global Recommendations on Physical Activity for Health; World Health Organisation: Geneva, Switzerland, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  6. Hofferth, S.L. Changes in American children’s time—1997 to 2003. Electron. Int. J. Time Use Res. 2009, 6, 26–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Maddison, R.; Marsh, S.; Hinckson, E.; Duncan, S.; Mandic, S.; Taylor, R.; Smith, M. Results from New Zealand’s 2016 Report Card on Physical Activity for Children and Youth. J. Phys. Act. Health 2016, 13, S225–S230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Telama, R. Tracking of Physical Activity from Childhood to Adulthood: A Review. Obes. Facts 2009, 2, 187–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Itoh, H.; Kitamura, F.; Hagi, N.; Mashiko, T.; Matsukawa, T.; Yokoyama, K. Leisure-time physical activity in youth as a predictor of adult leisure physical activity among Japanese workers: A cross-sectional study. Environ. Health Prev. Med. 2017, 22, 37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Jambor, T. Risk-taking needs in children: An accommodating play environment. Child. Environ. Q. 1986, 3, 22–25. [Google Scholar]
  11. Henniger, M.L. Planning for Outdoor Play. Young Child. 1994, 49, 10–15. [Google Scholar]
  12. Whitebread, D. The Importance of Play; Toy Industries of Europe: Cambridge, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  13. Gray, P. The decline of play and the rise of psychopathology in children and adolescents. Am. J. Play. 2011, 3, 443–463. [Google Scholar]
  14. United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child; United Nations: Rome, Italy, 1990.
  15. Sandseter, E.B.H.; Kennair, L.E.O. Children’s Risky Play from an Evolutionary Perspective: The Anti-Phobic Effects of Thrilling Experiences. Evol. Psychol. 2011, 9, 147470491100900220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Brussoni, M.; Gibbons, R.; Gray, C.; Ishikawa, T.; Sandseter, E.B.H.; Bienenstock, A.; Chabot, G.; Fuselli, P.; Herrington, S.; Janssen, I.; et al. What is the Relationship between Risky Outdoor Play and Health in Children? A Systematic Review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 6423–6454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Gray, C.; Gibbons, R.; Larouche, R.; Sandseter, E.B.H.; Bienenstock, A.; Brussoni, M.; Chabot, G.; Herrington, S.; Janssen, I.; Pickett, W.; et al. What Is the Relationship between Outdoor Time and Physical Activity, Sedentary Behaviour, and Physical Fitness in Children? A Systematic Review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 6455–6474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Whitebread, D. Free play and children’s mental health. Lancet Child Adolesc. Health 2017, 1, 167–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Sandseter, E.B.H. Categorising risky play-how can we identify risk-taking in children’s play? Eur. Early Child. Educ. Res. J. 2007, 15, 237–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Stine, S. Landscapes for Learning: Creating Outdoor Environments for Children and Youth; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1997; ISBN 978-0-471-16222-3. [Google Scholar]
  21. Christensen, P.; Mikkelsen, M.R. Jumping off and being careful: Children’s strategies of risk management in everyday life. Sociol. Health Illn. 2008, 30, 112–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Niehues, A.N.; Bundy, A.; Broom, A.; Tranter, P. Reframing healthy risk taking: Parents’ dilemmas and strategies to promote children’s well-being. J. Occup. Sci. 2016, 23, 449–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Little, H. Mothers’ beliefs about risk and risk-taking in children’s outdoor play. J. Adventure Educ. Outdoor Learn. 2015, 15, 24–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Wilkes, S.; Cordier, R.; Bundy, A.; Docking, K.; Munro, N. A play-based intervention for children with ADHD: A pilot study. Aust. Occup. Ther. J. 2011, 58, 231–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Barnes, G.; Wilkes-Gillan, S.; Bundy, A.; Cordier, R. The social play, social skills and parent-child relationships of children with ADHD 12months following a RCT of a play-based intervention. Aust. Occup. Ther. J. 2017, 64, 457–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Bassett, D.R.; John, D.; Conger, S.A.; Fitzhugh, E.C.; Coe, D.P. Trends in Physical Activity and Sedentary Behaviors of United States Youth. J. Phys. Act. Health 2015, 12, 1102–1111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Schoeppe, S.; Tranter, P.; Duncan, M.J.; Curtis, C.; Carver, A.; Malone, K. Australian children’s independent mobility levels: Secondary analyses of cross-sectional data between 1991 and 2012. Child. Geogr. 2016, 14, 408–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Witten, K.; Kearns, R.; Carroll, P.; Asiasiga, L.; Tava’e, N. New Zealand parents’ understandings of the intergenerational decline in children’s independent outdoor play and active travel. Child. Geogr. 2013, 11, 215–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Clements, R. An Investigation of the Status of Outdoor Play. Contemp. Issues Early Child. 2004, 5, 68–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Watchman, T.; Spencer-Cavaliere, N. Times have changed: Parent perspectives on children’s free play and sport. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2017, 32, 102–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Bhosale, J.; Duncan, S.; Schofield, G. Intergenerational change in children’s independent mobility and active transport in New Zealand children and parents. J. Transp. Health 2017, 7, 247–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Kalish, M.; Banco, L.; Burke, G.; Lapidus, G. Outdoor play: A survey of parent’s perceptions of their child’s safety. J. Trauma 2010, 69, S218–S222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Wyver, S.; Tranter, P.; Naughton, G.; Little, H.; Sandseter, E.; Bundy, A. Ten ways to restrict children’s freedom to play: The problem of surplus safety. Contemp. Issues Early Child. 2010, 11, 263–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Lee, H.; Tamminen, K.A.; Clark, A.M.; Slater, L.; Spence, J.C.; Holt, N.L. A meta-study of qualitative research examining determinants of children’s independent active free play. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2015, 12, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Esteban-Cornejo, I.; Carlson, J.A.; Conway, T.L.; Cain, K.L.; Saelens, B.E.; Frank, L.D.; Glanz, K.; Roman, C.G.; Sallis, J.F. Parental and Adolescent Perceptions of Neighborhood Safety Related to Adolescents’ Physical Activity in Their Neighborhood. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 2016, 87, 191–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Oliver, M.; Parker, K.; Witten, K.; Mavoa, S.; Badland, H.M.; Donovan, P.; Chaudhury, M.; Kearns, R. Children’s Out-of-School Independently Mobile Trips, Active Travel, and Physical Activity: A Cross-Sectional Examination from the Kids in the City Study. J. Phys. Act. Health 2016, 13, 318–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Rissotto, A.; Tonucci, F. Freedom of movement and environmental knowledge in elementary school children. J. Environ. Psychol. 2002, 22, 65–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Larouche, R.; Mammen, G.; Rowe, D.A.; Faulkner, G. Effectiveness of active school transport interventions: A systematic review and update. BMC Public Health 2018, 18, 206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Sedlak, A.J.; Mettenburg, J.; Basena, M.; Petta, I.; McPherson, K.; Greene, A.; Li, S. Fourth National Incidence Study of Child Abuse and Neglect (NIS–4): Report to Congress; Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families: Washington, DC, USA, 2010.
  40. Tomison, A.M. Update on Child Sexual Abuse; Australian Institute of Family Studies: Southbank, Victoria, Australia, 1995.
  41. Ronken, C.; Johnston, H. Child Sexual Assault: Facts and Stats; Bravehearts: Gold Coast, Queensland, Australia, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  42. OECD Family Database Child homicide in New Zealand: How Do the Numbers Compare Internationally? Available online: http://www.stuff.co.nz/national/crime/74207543/child-homicide-in-new-zealand-how-do-the-numbers-compare-internationally (accessed on 12 June 2018).
  43. New Zealand Police Child Protection Guidelines. Available online: http://www.police.govt.nz/advice/personal-and-community-advice/school-portal/information-and-guidelines/child-protection (accessed on 23 August 2018).
  44. Ministry of Transport Annual Number of Road Deaths Historical Information. Available online: https://www.transport.govt.nz/resources/road-safety-resources/road-deaths/annual-number-of-road-deaths-historical-information/ (accessed on 12 June 2018).
  45. Bundy, A.C.; Luckett, T.; Tranter, P.J.; Naughton, G.A.; Wyver, S.R.; Ragen, J.; Spies, G. The Risk is that there is “no risk”: A simple innovative intervention to increase children’s activity levels. Int. J. Early Years Educ. 2009, 17, 33–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Ball, D.J.; Great Britain; Health and Safety Executive; Middlesex University. Playgrounds—Risks, Benefits and Choices; HSE Books: Sudbury, ON, Canada, 2002; ISBN 978-0-7176-2340-2.
  47. Emberson, H.I. An Exploration of the Nature of Physically Active Play in New Zealand Early Childhood Education. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  48. Veitch, J.; Bagley, S.; Ball, K.; Salmon, J. Where do children usually play? A qualitative study of parents’ perceptions of influences on children’s active free-play. Health Place 2006, 12, 383–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. United Nations. Political Declaration of the High-Level Meeting of the General Assembly on the Prevention and Control of Non-Communicable Diseases; United Nations: New York, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  50. Little, H.; Wyver, S.; Gibson, F. The influence of play context and adult attitudes on young children’s physical risk-taking during outdoor play. Eur. Early Child. Educ. Res. J. 2011, 19, 113–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Carver, A.; Watson, B.; Shaw, B.; Hillman, M. A comparison study of children’s independent mobility in England and Australia. Child. Geogr. 2013, 11, 461–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Shaw, B.; Watson, B.; Frauendienst, B.; Redecker, A.; Jones, T.; Hillman, M. Children’s Independent Mobility: A Comparative Study in England and Germany (1971–2010); Policy Studies Institute: London, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  53. Health & Safety Executive. Managing Risk in Play Provision: A Position Statement; Play Safety Forum; Play England: London, UK, 2008; p. 4.
  54. Mitra, R.; Faulkner, G.E.; Buliung, R.N.; Stone, M.R. Do parental perceptions of the neighbourhood environment influence children’s independent mobility? Evidence from Toronto, Canada, Do parental perceptions of the neighbourhood environment influence children’s independent mobility? Evidence from Toronto, Canada. Urban Stud. 2014, 51, 3401–3419. [Google Scholar]
  55. Stephenson, A. Physical Risk-taking: Dangerous or endangered? Early Years 2003, 23, 35–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. STROBE Statement: Home. Available online: https://www.strobe-statement.org/index.php?id=strobe-home (accessed on 7 January 2019).
  57. Bates, B.; Stone, M.R. Measures of outdoor play and independent mobility in children and youth: A methodological review. J. Sci. Med. Sport 2015, 18, 545–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Hill, A.; Bundy, A.C. Reliability and validity of a new instrument to measure tolerance of everyday risk for children. Child Care Health Dev. 2014, 40, 68–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Olsen, L.L.; Ishikawa, T.; Mâsse, L.C.; Chan, G.; Brussoni, M. Risk Engagement and Protection Survey (REPS): Developing and validating a survey tool on fathers’ attitudes towards child injury protection and risk engagement. Inj. Prev. 2018, 24, 106–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Prezza, M.; Alparone, F.R.; Cristallo, C.; Luigi, S. Parental perception of social risk and of positive potentiality of outdoor autonomy for children: The development of two instruments. J. Environ. Psychol. 2005, 25, 437–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Jenkins, N.E. ‘You can’t wrap them up in cotton wool!’ Constructing risk in young people’s access to outdoor play. Health Risk Soc. 2006, 8, 379–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Larouche, R.; Eryuzlu, S.; Livock, H.; Leduc, G.; Faulkner, G.; Trudeau, F.; Tremblay, M.S. Test-retest reliability and convergent validity of measures of children’s travel behaviours and independent mobility. J. Transp. Health 2017, 6, 105–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Wickham, H. GGPlot 2; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  64. 2013 Census QuickStats about Culture and Identity; Statistics New Zealand: Wellington, New Zealand, 2014; Available online: https://www.nbr.co.nz/sites/default/files/2013%20Census%20QuickStats%20about%20culture%20and%20identity.pdf (accessed on 17 July 2018).
  65. McFarland, L.; Laird, S.G. Parents’ and Early Childhood Educators’ Attitudes and Practices in Relation to Children’s Outdoor Risky Play. Early Child. Educ. J. 2018, 46, 159–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Brussoni, M.; Olsen, L.L.; Pike, I.; Sleet, D.A. Risky Play and Children’s Safety: Balancing Priorities for Optimal Child Development. Int. J. Environ. Res Public Health 2012, 9, 3134–3148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Carroll, P.; Witten, K.; Kearns, R.; Donovan, P. Kids in the City: Children’s Use and Experiences of Urban Neighbourhoods in Auckland, New Zealand. J. Urban Des. 2015, 20, 417–436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Walters, S.R.; Duncan, S.; McPhee, J.; Atkins, D.; Millar, S.-K. ‘Real Play Families’: A New Zealand case study. Int. J. Play. 2018, 7, 97–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Foster, S.; Villanueva, K.; Wood, L.; Christian, H.; Giles-Corti, B. The impact of parents’ fear of strangers and perceptions of informal social control on children’s independent mobility. Health Place 2014, 26, 60–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Sandseter, E.B.H.; Sando, O.J. “We Don’t Allow Children to Climb Trees” How a Focus on Safety Affects Norwegian Children’s Play in Early-Childhood Education and Care Settings. Am. J. Play 2016, 8, 178–200. [Google Scholar]
  71. Riazi, N.A.; Faulkner, G. Examining Independent Mobility: A Socioecological Approach. In Children’s Active Transportation; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2018; pp. 77–87. [Google Scholar]
  72. Creighton, G.; Brussoni, M.; Oliffe, J.; Olsen, L. “It’s Good for the Kids”: Fathers Consider Risk and Protection in Their Own and Their Children’s Lives. J. Fam. Issues 2017, 38, 1043–1065. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Skar, M.; Wold, L.C.; Gundersen, V.; O’Brien, L. Why do children not play in nearby nature? Results from a Norwegian survey. J. Adventure Educ. Outdoor Learn. 2016, 16, 239–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Cevher-Kalburan, N.; Ivrendi, A. Risky Play and Parenting Styles. J Child Fam Stud 2016, 25, 355–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Little, H.; Sandseter, E.B.H.; Wyver, S. Early Childhood Teachers’ Beliefs about Children’s Risky Play in Australia and Norway. Contemp. Issues Early Child. 2012, 13, 300–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Ministry of Health. N. Z. Health Survey; Ministry of Health: Auckland, New Zealand, 2017.
  77. Mental Health Commission. Child and Youth Mental Health and Addiction; Te Kaitataki Oranga: Wellington, New Zealand, 2011; p. 34.
Figure 1. Participant recruitment and inclusion.
Figure 1. Participant recruitment and inclusion.
Ijerph 16 00262 g001
Figure 2. Results from the Tolerance for Risk in Play Scale (TRiPS) (a) Scores associated with each quartile (N = 398 in each group) and (b) Relationship between number of yes answers and item score for each risk category (N = 1573).
Figure 2. Results from the Tolerance for Risk in Play Scale (TRiPS) (a) Scores associated with each quartile (N = 398 in each group) and (b) Relationship between number of yes answers and item score for each risk category (N = 1573).
Ijerph 16 00262 g002
Figure 3. Risk Engagement and Protection Survey (REPS) responses to (a) injury prevention and (b) risk engagement questions.
Figure 3. Risk Engagement and Protection Survey (REPS) responses to (a) injury prevention and (b) risk engagement questions.
Ijerph 16 00262 g003
Figure 4. Responses to items from the Perception of Positive Potentiality of Outdoor Autonomy for Children (PPOAC) scale. (a) Negative statements and (b) positive statements.
Figure 4. Responses to items from the Perception of Positive Potentiality of Outdoor Autonomy for Children (PPOAC) scale. (a) Negative statements and (b) positive statements.
Ijerph 16 00262 g004
Figure 5. Parents attitudes to health and safety rules (n = 2003). H&S—health and safety.
Figure 5. Parents attitudes to health and safety rules (n = 2003). H&S—health and safety.
Ijerph 16 00262 g005
Figure 6. (a) Number of parents who allow their children to play in the rain (N = 1573) and (b) proportion of respondents agreeing with the suggested barriers. Respondents could mark as many options as they wished. All “yes” answers were summed (total = 3001) and the number per barrier calculated.
Figure 6. (a) Number of parents who allow their children to play in the rain (N = 1573) and (b) proportion of respondents agreeing with the suggested barriers. Respondents could mark as many options as they wished. All “yes” answers were summed (total = 3001) and the number per barrier calculated.
Ijerph 16 00262 g006aIjerph 16 00262 g006b
Figure 7. Number of risky play activities engaged in infrequently (seldom or never), sometimes and frequently (often or always) by children aged 5–12 (N = 1573). The total number of participants responding with each frequency rating was calculated across all activities and expressed as a percentage according to the number of activities that received that rating. For example, 234 respondents reported that their child seldom or never engaged in four of the eight activities.
Figure 7. Number of risky play activities engaged in infrequently (seldom or never), sometimes and frequently (often or always) by children aged 5–12 (N = 1573). The total number of participants responding with each frequency rating was calculated across all activities and expressed as a percentage according to the number of activities that received that rating. For example, 234 respondents reported that their child seldom or never engaged in four of the eight activities.
Ijerph 16 00262 g007
Table 1. Definitions of risky play used in the State of Play Survey.
Table 1. Definitions of risky play used in the State of Play Survey.
CategoryRiskExamples
Great heightsDanger of injury from fallingClimbing, jumping, balancing, hanging, swinging
High speedUncontrolled speed and pace that may lead to collisionSwinging, sliding/sledging, or non-motorised vehicles
Adult toolsPotential for injury or woundsKnives, saws, axes, drills, ropes
Dangerous elementsRisk of injury from falling into or from somethingTrees, cliffs, water, fire
Rough and tumbleChildren may harm each otherPlay-fighting, wrestling, fencing with sticks
Disappear or get lostChildren are unsupervised, alone or lostRoaming neighbourhood with friends or alone, exploring
Loose parts Danger of injury from sharp or heavy objects. Use of dirty objectsTyres, sticks, timber, tarpaulins
Messy play Illness from unsanitary environmentsPainting, play in mud, dirt, sand, water
Adapted from Sandseter et al. (2011), Added to broaden the concept of risky play [57].
Table 2. Demographic characteristics of respondents (N = 2003).
Table 2. Demographic characteristics of respondents (N = 2003).
CharacteristicN (%)
Gender
Male629 (31.4)
Female1374 (68.6)
Parent age (years)
Under 30192 (9.6)
30–39631 (31.5)
40–49799 (39.9)
Over 49381 (19.0)
Number of children
One675 (33.7)
Two823 (41.1)
Three349 (17.4)
Four or more157 (7.7)
Eldest child’s age
0–453 (2.6)
5–8299 (14.9)
9–12512 (25.6)
13–16540 (27.0)
16+599 (29.9)
Ethnicity
New Zealand European1412 (70.5)
New Zealand Maori234 (11.7)
Pacific Island114 (5.7)
Asian146 (7.3)
Annual household income
Less than $40.000373 (18.6)
$40.000–$100.000855 (42.7)
More than $100.000431 (21.5)
Location
Large city939 (46.9)
Small city457 (22.8)
Town377 (18.8)
Small town102 (5.1)
Rural122 (6.1)
Table 3. Survey summary statistics (n = 1573).
Table 3. Survey summary statistics (n = 1573).
Questionnaire (Score Range)Median (LQ–UQ)
Tolerance of risk in play scale (0–184)95 (61–122)
Risk averse (0–61)41 (24–51)
Somewhat risk averse (62–95)79 (71–88)
Somewhat risk tolerant (96–122)111 (103–118)
Risk tolerant (123–184)138 (130–153)
Risk engagement and perception survey
Injury Prevention (6–42)24 (21–27)
Risk engagement (6–42)30 (29–34)
Perception of positive potentiality of outdoor autonomy (9–36) 25 (23–26)
Possible scores once reverse coded for negative statements. Higher scores assigned to positive answer indicating lower fear.
Table 4. Children’s travel to school and independent mobility (N = 1366).
Table 4. Children’s travel to school and independent mobility (N = 1366).
N (%)
Mode:
 Active (e.g., walk, bike, skateboard)725 (53.1)
 Passive (e.g., car, bus)641 (46.9)
Accompaniment :
 Alone361 (26.4)
 Friends387 (28.3)
 Siblings509 (37.3)
 Adult1002 (73.4)
Roaming the neighbourhood:
 Destinations within walking distance587 (43.0)
 Crossing main roads605 (44.3)
 Out alone after dark55 (4.0)
Cycling on main roads259 (19.0)
Using buses (not for school)181 (13.3)
Respondents could choose more than one option and therefore this total is >1366.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Jelleyman, C.; McPhee, J.; Brussoni, M.; Bundy, A.; Duncan, S. A Cross-Sectional Description of Parental Perceptions and Practices Related to Risky Play and Independent Mobility in Children: The New Zealand State of Play Survey. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 262. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16020262

AMA Style

Jelleyman C, McPhee J, Brussoni M, Bundy A, Duncan S. A Cross-Sectional Description of Parental Perceptions and Practices Related to Risky Play and Independent Mobility in Children: The New Zealand State of Play Survey. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2019; 16(2):262. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16020262

Chicago/Turabian Style

Jelleyman, Charlotte, Julia McPhee, Mariana Brussoni, Anita Bundy, and Scott Duncan. 2019. "A Cross-Sectional Description of Parental Perceptions and Practices Related to Risky Play and Independent Mobility in Children: The New Zealand State of Play Survey" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 16, no. 2: 262. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16020262

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop