Next Article in Journal
Precision Joint RF Measurement of Inter-Satellite Range and Time Difference and Scalable Clock Synchronization for Multi-Microsatellite Formations
Next Article in Special Issue
A Supervised Machine Learning Approach to Classify Brain Morphology of Professional Visual Artists versus Non-Artists
Previous Article in Journal
Error Analysis of Heterodyne Interferometry Based on One Single-Mode Polarization-Maintaining Fiber
Previous Article in Special Issue
Exposure to Multicultural Context Affects Neural Response to Out-Group Faces: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Age-Related Changes in Episodic Processing of Scenes: A Functional Activation and Connectivity Study

by
Makoto Miyakoshi
1,2,3,
Josephine Astrid Archer
4,
Chiao-Yi Wu
5,
Toshiharu Nakai
3,6,7 and
Shen-Hsing Annabel Chen
4,8,9,*
1
Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, OH 45229, USA
2
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cincinnati College of Medicine, Cincinnati, OH 45267, USA
3
Department of Gerontechnology, National Center for Geriatrics and Gerontology, Ohbu 474-8511, Aichi, Japan
4
School of Social Sciences, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore 639818, Singapore
5
Centre for Research in Child Development, National Institute of Education, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore 637616, Singapore
6
Department of Dental Radiology, Graduate School of Dentistry, Osaka University, Suita 565-0871, Osaka, Japan
7
Institute of NeuroImaging & Informatics, Ohbu 474-8511, Aichi, Japan
8
Centre for Research and Development in Learning, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore 637335, Singapore
9
Lee Kong Chian School of Medicine, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore 308232, Singapore
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sensors 2023, 23(8), 4107; https://doi.org/10.3390/s23084107
Submission received: 4 February 2023 / Revised: 5 April 2023 / Accepted: 15 April 2023 / Published: 19 April 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Brain Activity Monitoring and Measurement)

Abstract

:
The posterior-to-anterior shift in aging (PASA) effect is seen as a compensatory model that enables older adults to meet increased cognitive demands to perform comparably as their young counterparts. However, empirical support for the PASA effect investigating age-related changes in the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), hippocampus, and parahippocampus has yet to be established. 33 older adults and 48 young adults were administered tasks sensitive to novelty and relational processing of indoor/outdoor scenes in a 3-Tesla MRI scanner. Functional activation and connectivity analyses were applied to examine the age-related changes on the IFG, hippocampus, and parahippocampus among low/high-performing older adults and young adults. Significant parahippocampal activation was generally found in both older (high-performing) and young adults for novelty and relational processing of scenes. Younger adults had significantly greater IFG and parahippocampal activation than older adults, and greater parahippocampal activation compared to low-performing older adults for relational processing—providing partial support for the PASA model. Observations of significant functional connectivity within the medial temporal lobe and greater negative left IFG-right hippocampus/parahippocampus functional connectivity for young compared to low-performing older adults for relational processing also supports the PASA effect partially.

1. Introduction

Aging is generally associated with poorer memory encoding [1] and retrieval [2]. However, there are individual differences in this aging effect, and high-functioning older adults can show comparable memory performance as their young counterparts [3,4]. Clarifying the mechanism of the aging effect on memory functions and the sources of the individual differences is important to find an effective intervention to maintain QOL of the older people.
Findings from human neuroimaging studies using positron emission tomography (PET) and functional MRI (fMRI) showed that when older adults performed episodic encoding and retrieval tasks, their brain activation shows a general pattern of activation decrease in the posterior part of the brain combined with activation increase in the anterior part of the brain [5,6]. Based on this observation, the posterior-to-anterior shift in aging (PASA) model was proposed [7]. Results from a functional connectivity study also supported this view [8]. The widely accepted view of the functional role of the PASA effect today is active compensation [9]. There is empirical evidence that the posterior to anterior shift was associated with increased cognitive demands [10] and the PASA effect benefitted older adults in episodic encoding [6].
The conventional PASA model has been focusing on frontal and occipital brain regions. Here, we claim that the PASA model may be extended to the relation between the hippocampal formation and the frontal regions, particularly the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG). We found it suggestive of the extended PASA model that an age-related increase in bilateral IFG activation coupled with a decrease in bilateral hippocampal and parahippocampal activation for novelty processing of the scenes (i.e., experimentally defined as watching novel pictures minus repeating pictures; see [11]) and relational processing of the scenes (watching novel pictures minus scrambled pictures) [12,13,14]. Being motivated by these findings, we designed a study to test the central hypothesis of whether such hippocampal-frontal (i.e., temporal-to-frontal) shift in aging should offer any compensatory effect for the older adults during novelty and relational processing of scenes. We employed an established paradigm developed to examine the novelty and relational processing using indoor/outdoor scenes [11,15], a part of which was published elsewhere [15]. In conducting the analysis, we controlled age-related structural atrophy in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) [16], hippocampus [14], and parahippocampus [17]. We evaluated whether age-related changes in the BOLD signals reflect the PASA effect within bilateral IFG, hippocampi, and parahippocampi as regions of interest (ROIs) in comparison between healthy young and older adults. The older adults were further separated into two subgroups, high and low performers, to study the compensatory role of the PASA model.

2. Method

2.1. Participants

A total of 102 older and young adults were separately recruited in this study by the Clinical Brain Lab (CBL) in Singapore and National Center for Geriatrics and Gerontology (NCGG) in Japan. Twenty-five older adults (12 females, 3 left-handed, mean age = 67.8, SD = 7.6) with a mean mini-mental state exam (MMSE) [18] score of 28.9 (SD = 1.1, range 26–30) and 25 young adults (12 females, 2 left-handed, mean age = 23.2, SD = 2.2) with a mean MMSE score of 29.9 (SD = 0.3, range 29–30) were recruited by CBL. Twenty-six older adults (13 females, 2 left-handed, mean age = 66.7, SD = 3.1) with a mean mini mental status examination (MMSE) score of 28.3 (SD = 1.9, range 23–30) and 26 young adults (14 females, 1 left-handed and 1 ambidextrous, mean age = 21.7, SD = 1.6) with a mean MMSE score of 29.2 (SD = 0.9, range 27–30) were recruited by NCGG. The Edinburgh Handedness Inventory was used to determine participants’ handedness [19]. All participants were screened to exclude any psychiatric, neurologic, and metabolic conditions (e.g., diabetes and hypertension). Participants with excess head motion (>2 mm translation, >3 degrees rotation), poor response accuracy (ACC) (<60% ACC during scan and post-scan), and low MMSE score (MMSE score ≤ 26) were excluded from the analyses. Both CBL and NCGG datasets were combined and analyzed for age-related changes in functional activation, while only the NCGG dataset was analyzed for age-related changes in functional connectivity for novelty and relational processing of scenes.
The older adults were further categorized into low and high performers based on their memory performance ACC for novelty and relational processing of scenes. Unlike Cabeza et al. [3], we did not administer standardized cognitive tests other than MMSE. Hence, we categorized older adults into older low and older high performers using the mean ACC of young adults on our task, with a 2 standard deviation (SD) buffer, as the cut-off in the respective NCGG and CBL datasets. Thus, older adults were considered as older high performers if their ACC fell within 2 SD from the mean ACC of their young counterparts, while the older low performers had ACC beyond 2 SD from the mean ACC of their young counterparts in the respective datasets. Older high performers had comparable ACC as young performers, while older low performers showed significantly poorer ACC than older high and young performers.
In total, 33 older adults (19 females, 3 left-handed, mean age = 66.2, SD = 5.0) with a mean MMSE score of 29.0 (SD = 0.9, range 27–30), and 48 young adults (26 females, 2 left-handed and 1 ambidextrous, mean age = 22.4, SD = 2.1) with a mean MMSE score of 29.5 (SD = 0.8, range 27–30), were analyzed for age-related changes in functional activation related to novelty and relational processing of scenes. Among the older adults, there were 9 older low performers (7 females, 2 left-handed, mean age = 69.6, SD = 5.8) with a mean MMSE score of 28.7 (SD = 1.1, range 27–30) and 24 older high performers (12 females, 1 left-handed, mean age = 64.9, SD = 4.1) with a mean MMSE score of 29.1 (SD = 0.8, range 28–30). Although the older low performers were significantly older than older high performers, t(31) = 2.58, p < 0.05; the MMSE score for older low performers and older high low performers did not differ significantly, t(31) = 1.32, p > 0.05.
For age-related changes in functional connectivity related to novelty and relational processing of scenes, a total of 17 older adults (10 females, 1 left-handed, mean age = 66.1, SD = 3.4) with a mean MMSE score of 28.8 (SD = 1.0, range 27–30), and 25 young adults (14 females, 1 ambidextrous, mean age = 21.7, SD = 1.7) with a mean MMSE score of 29.2 (SD = 0.9, range 27–30), from the NCGG dataset were analyzed. Among the older adults, there were 6 older low performers (4 females, 1 left-handed, mean age = 67.2, SD = 2.5) with a mean MMSE score of 28.7 (SD = 1.4, range 27–30) and 11 older high performers (6 females, 0 left-handed, mean age = 65.5, SD = 3.8) with a mean MMSE score of 28.8 (SD = 0.9, range 28–30). These older low performers did not differ significantly in an age when compared to older high performers, t(15) = 0.92, p > 0.05; and the MMSE score for older low performers and older high low performers did not differ significantly, t(15) = 0.28, p > 0.05.
Written informed consent, which had been approved by both the ethics committees of NCGG, Japan, and Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration, was obtained from all participants.

2.2. Task

The investigation of episodic encoding in terms of stimulus novelty and relational processing was adapted from Binder et al.’s protocols [11]. These used an indoor/outdoor scene discrimination task, sensitive to novelty and relational processing. There were three conditions in this indoor/outdoor scene discrimination task, namely Novel Pictures condition (N), Repeating Pictures condition (R), and Scrambled Pictures condition (S). For both Novel Pictures and Repeating Pictures conditions, participants were asked to discriminate whether each non-scrambled picture was an indoor or outdoor scene. In the Scrambled Pictures condition, participants were asked to discriminate whether the left and right halves of each scrambled picture were identical.
Colored digital indoor/outdoor scenes (containing no words or people), re-sized to 600 × 600 pixels, subtended 22.5° of horizontal and vertical visual angle with a scanner, were used for stimulus presentation in the Novel Pictures and Repeating Pictures conditions. For the Novel Pictures condition, a total of 40 unique pictures (20 indoor and 20 outdoor scenes) were presented to each participant who had to discriminate whether the picture was an indoor scene with a button by the index finger or an outdoor scene with another button by the middle finger. In the Repeating Pictures condition, participants were presented with two unique pictures (one indoor and one outdoor scene) repeatedly for a total of 20 times. They pressed a button assigned to either index or middle finger as above to discriminate the indoor/outdoor scenes.
For the Scrambled Pictures condition, pictures of indoor/outdoor scenes were divided into halves, defined by a red line that bisected each picture into two equal hemifields. Pictures of indoor/outdoor scenes were scrambled into pixelated mosaics by re-arranging 20-pixel square segments. The 600 × 300 pixelated halves of these scrambled indoor/outdoor scenes were either duplicated to produce scrambled pictures with identical halves or combined with another retiling of the same picture of the indoor/outdoor scene to produce scrambled pictures with non-identical halves. A total of 40 unique scrambled pictures (20 scrambled pictures with identical halves and 20 scrambled pictures with non-identical halves) were presented to each participant who had to discriminate whether both halves of the scrambled picture were identical with a button press by the index finger, or non-identical with the middle finger.
The stimuli presentation for the indoor/outdoor scenes discrimination task was blocked by condition, in the order of Novel Pictures, followed by Repeating Pictures, then Scrambled Pictures condition. Each block consisted of eight trials. For each trial, a stimulus was presented against a black background for 2500 ms, followed by 500 ms of inter-stimulus interval. Participants had to complete a total of five cycles per run which lasted for 240 s (see Figure 1). Practice trials were given before the scan to familiarize participants with the task. Behavioral performance in terms of accuracy rate (ACC) and reaction time (RT) for the indoor/outdoor scene discrimination task were measured for all three conditions during the scan.
A post-scan recognition test measuring participants’ ACC on non-scrambled and scrambled scenes was administered (while participants were still in the scanner) to participants in the CBL dataset. This post-scan recognition test consisted of Non-scrambled Pictures and Scrambled Pictures conditions. For the Non-scrambled Pictures condition, 15 old (presented previously during the scan) and 15 new (not presented previously during the scan) non-scrambled scenes were presented to each participant who had to recognize whether they had seen the non-scrambled pictures before during the scan, via button pressing. For the Scrambled Pictures condition, 15 old (presented previously during the scan) and 15 new (not presented previously during the scan) scrambled scenes were presented to each participant who had to recognize whether they had seen the scrambled pictures before during the scan, via button pressing. This post-scan recognition test would serve as a manipulation check that stimulus novelty (novelty encoding) and relational processing (relational encoding) of scenes were previously elicited by the indoor/outdoor scenes discrimination task.
As outlined by Binder et al. [11], a subtraction analysis of the Novel Pictures minus Repeating Pictures (N > R) contrast would resemble novelty processing of scenes, while the Novel Pictures minus Scrambled Pictures (N > S) contrast would emphasize relational processing of scenes. Both novelty and relational processing were incidental in nature as the participants were not explicitly instructed to remember any stimuli; neither were participants in the CBL dataset informed of a post-scan recognition test.

2.3. Image Acquisition and Preprocesses

Structural and functional scans were acquired using a 3.0-Tesla MRI scanner (MAGNETOM Trio, Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) in both centers, as participants underwent the indoor/outdoor scenes discrimination task. VisuaStimDigital for MRI (Resonance Technology Inc., CA, United States) was used for the presentation of stimuli.
Functional imaging acquisition followed a gradient-echo echo-planar sequence for both datasets: CBL (TE, 30 ms; TR, 3000 ms; FOV, 192 mm; matrix, 64 × 64; slice thickness, 3 mm; axial slices, 39; gap between slices 0.75 mm); NCGG (TE, 24 ms; TR, 2000 ms; FOV, 192 mm; matrix, 64 × 64; slice thickness, 3 mm; axial slices, 39; gap between slices 0.75 mm). A total of 120 and 180 consecutive image volumes were acquired per scan per participant for the CBL and NCGG datasets, respectively. Participants’ T1-weighted anatomical reference image was used for localization purposes functional images were acquired at different sites, and inter-scanner variability was controlled by submitting the signal-to-fluctuation-noise-ratios (SFNRs) [20] extracted from each participant as covariates in the group-level imaging analyses [21].
Preprocessing and functional activation analyses were performed using statistical parametric mapping [22] in SPM8 (The FIL Methods Group, Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, Institute of Neurology, UCL), with MATLAB 7.9 (The MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA). For each participant, the T1-weighted anatomical image and functional images were re-oriented to have the anterior and posterior commissures on the same plane, using the T1-weighted image as the image template.
Preprocessing of each participant’s functional images followed the DARTEL (i.e., Diffeomorphic Anatomical Registration using Exponentiated Lie algebra) pipeline [23], consisting of realignment, slice-timing correction, and structural-to-functional co-registration. DARTEL was used to improve inter-subject structural-to-functional co-registration [24]. Using the group template and the individual participant’s flow field image created from DARTEL, each participant’s T1-weighted anatomical image, functional images, and gray matter probability map were normalized to Montreal Neurological Institute space (MNI; Montréal, QC, Canada).
The gray matter probability maps generated from DARTEL enabled us to control for any age-related structural atrophy [25], by submitting them as covariates in the functional activation analyses. For functional connectivity analyses, intra-cranial brain volumes were submitted as covariates to control for age-related structural differences.
Functional images were later smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm full width at half maximum (FWHM). To prevent attenuation of SFNR caused by smoothing, a separate copy of unsmoothed functional images for each participant was saved for SFNR extraction.

2.4. Design for Statistical Tests

2.4.1. Behavioral Data

For behavioral analyses, a 2 (Age: Older, Young) × 3 (Condition: Novel Pictures, Repeating Pictures, Scrambled Pictures) mixed design analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted on participants’ ACC and RT. In addition, a 3 (Performance: Older Low Performers, Older High Performers, Young Performers) × 3 (Condition: Novel Pictures, Repeating Pictures, Scrambled Pictures) ANOVA was conducted on participants’ ACC and RT. For the CBL dataset, a 2 (Age: Older, Young) × 2 (Condition: Non-scrambled Pictures, Scrambled Pictures) mixed design ANOVA was conducted on post-scan ACC. This served as a manipulation check that the incidental encoding in terms of novelty and relational processing were indeed elicited. The Greenhouse–Geisser correction was used whenever necessary.
Post-hoc multiple pair-wise comparisons, with Bonferroni correction, were carried out when a significant interaction or main effect was found, with p < 0.05. As a significant age effect on ACC was found, participants’ ACCs from the respective conditions were entered as covariates in the group-level imaging analyses.

2.4.2. Imaging Data

Functional activation analyses. For functional activation analyses, general linear model analyses were separately conducted for N > R and N > S contrasts using SPM8, to generate subject-level brain activation maps for novelty and relational processing of scenes, respectively. The subject-level brain activation maps were subsequently submitted for group-level random effects analyses, while controlling for gray matter probability, SFNRs, and ACCs, in biological parametric mapping (BPM) [26]. The significant level for cluster-level activation was set at p < 0.05 (FDR-corrected), with cluster size k ≥ 20 voxels.
Functional connectivity analyses. ROI-to-ROI functional connectivity analyses were conducted using the functional connectivity toolbox (CONN) [27,28]. As the scanning parameters used for NCGG (TR = 2000 ms) and CBL (TR = 3000 ms) datasets were different, we could not merge both datasets in CONN. Thus, we selected the NCGG dataset to be used in CONN for functional connectivity analyses, as the NCGG dataset allowed a less disproportional splitting of the older adults into low and high performers compared to the CBL dataset. In the Setup phase, individual subjects’ intracranial brain volume and ACCs were submitted as second-level covariates for ROI-to-ROI functional connectivity analysis, with left and right IFG, hippocampus, and parahippocampus as a priori ROIs. Next, in the Denoising phase, a bandpass filter of 0.0085 to 0.16 Hz was applied. Subject-level brain activation results were then generated in the First-level Analyses phase with left and right IFG, hippocampus, and parahippocampus as a priori ROIs. Subsequently, the subject-level brain activation results were submitted for group-level ROI-to-ROI functional connectivity analyses, while controlling for intracranial brain volume and ACCs. Group-level ROI-to-ROI functional connectivity focused on left and right IFG, hippocampus, and parahippocampus were generated, with N > R and N > S contrasts for novelty and relational processing of scenes, respectively, at p < 0.05 (FDR-corrected).
Regions of interest. Left and right IFG, hippocampus, and parahippocampus masks were created using MARINA software [29], and the left–right inversion of the masks were later corrected using the SPM toolbox, MarsBaR [30]. These left and right IFG, hippocampus, and parahippocampus masks would serve as ROIs in the ROI-to-ROI functional connectivity analyses for novelty and relational processing of scenes.

3. Results

3.1. Behavioral Data

A 2 (Age: Older, Young) × 3 (Condition: Novel Pictures, Repeating Pictures, Scrambled Pictures) ANOVA on ACC revealed significant age × condition interaction effect, F(2, 79) = 8.04, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.09, and a significant main effect of condition, F(2, 79) = 47.66, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.38. Post-hoc analyses showed that the older adults on average performed significantly poorer than young adults for the Scrambled Pictures condition (90.69% vs. 94.50%), p < 0.05. On average, the Scrambled Pictures condition had significantly poorer performance than Novel Pictures or Repeating Pictures condition for both older (90.39% vs. 97.09%, 90.39% vs. 98.09%, respectively) and young adults (94.50% vs. 97.02%, 94.50% vs. 97.92%, respectively), p < 0.05 (see Figure 2a). Other pairwise comparisons did not reveal significant differences in ACC.
A similar 2 × 3 ANOVA on RT showed significant age × condition interaction effect, F(2, 79) = 14.50, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.89, and significant main effects of age and condition, F(1, 79) = 19.87, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.20 and F(2, 79) = 644.76, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.89, respectively. Post-hoc analyses showed that older adults on average performed significantly slower than young adults for the Novel Pictures condition (894.35 ms vs. 778.60 ms), Repeating Pictures condition (701.82 ms vs. 651.67 ms), and Scrambled Pictures condition (1360.81 ms vs. 1135.64 ms), p < 0.05. The Scrambled Pictures condition, on average, had the slowest performance followed by the Novel Pictures condition then the Repeating Pictures condition, for both older and young adults, p < 0.05 (see Figure 2b).
Upon separating the older adults into high and low performers, a 3 (Performance: Older Low Performers, Older High Performers, Young Performers) × 3 (Condition: Novel Pictures, Repeating Pictures, Scrambled Pictures) ANOVA on ACC revealed significant performance × condition interaction effect, F(3, 79) = 9.92, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.20, and significant main effects of performance and condition, F(2, 79) = 17.18, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.31 and F(2, 79) = 65.29, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.46, respectively. Post-hoc analyses showed that older low performers on average performed significantly poorer than both older high performers and young performers for the Repeating Pictures condition (94.44% vs. 99.38%, 94.44% vs. 97.81%, respectively) and Scrambled Pictures condition (81.05% vs. 93.72%, 81.05% vs. 94.32%, respectively), p < 0.05 (see Figure 3a). Both older high performers and young performers did not differ significantly in their performance for all three conditions, p > 0.05.
A similar 3 × 3 ANOVA on RT demonstrated a significant performance × condition interaction effect, F(3, 79) = 7.18, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.16, and significant main effects of performance and condition, F(2, 79) = 10.62, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.21 and F(2, 79) = 465.01, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.86, respectively. Post-hoc analyses showed that both older low performers and older high performers on average performed significantly slower than young adults for the Novel Pictures condition (942.31 ms vs. 778.60 ms, 876.37 ms vs. 778.60 ms, respectively) and Scrambled Pictures condition (1400.59 ms vs. 1135.64 ms, 1345.89 ms vs. 1135.64 ms, respectively), p < 0.05 (see Figure 3b). No significant difference in RT was found in other pair-wise comparisons across the performance.
A 2 (Age: Older, Young) × 2 (Condition: Non-scrambled Pictures, Scrambled Pictures) mixed design ANOVA, conducted on post-scan ACC for the CBL dataset, revealed significant age × condition interaction effect, F(1, 37) = 4.47, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.11, and significant main effects of age and condition, F(1, 37) = 6.11, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.14 and F(1, 37) = 140.69, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.79, respectively. Post-hoc analyses showed that the older adults on average performed poorer than young adults on post-scan ACC for the Non-scrambled Pictures condition (72.38% vs. 80.65%), p < 0.05, but not for the Scrambled Pictures condition, p > 0.05. On average, the Scrambled Pictures condition had poorer performance than the Non-scrambled Pictures condition for both older (72.38% vs. 56.00%) and young adults (80.65% vs. 57.17%), p < 0.05 (see Figure 4). It is clear that the Age × ACC interaction is dominated by the simple effect of Non-scrambled Pictures rather than the Scrambled Pictures between the Older and Young adults, which indicate the task’s sensitivity to separate the two Age groups according to the scene encoding but not to geometric pattern encoding.
As a significant age effect of ACC was found, the ACCs measured for all conditions were submitted as non-imaging covariates during group-level imaging analyses for novelty processing of scenes.

3.2. Functional Activation Analyses

Novelty processing of scenes. Results of supra-threshold cluster-level activations, at p < 0.05 (FDR-corrected) with k ≥ 20 for N > R contrast representing novelty processing of scenes, are shown in Table 1. Participants’ gray matter probability, SFNR value, and ACC in both Novel Pictures and Repeating Pictures conditions were controlled as covariates in group-level analyses. Significant cluster-level activations related to novelty processing of scenes for older adults (n = 33) were observed in the right superior frontal, precentral and postcentral gyri, left superior temporal, middle temporal, and fusiform gyri, bilateral parahippocampal gyrus, and right cerebellar tonsil (see Figure 5a). Among the older adults, only the older high performers (n = 24) showed significant activations in the right middle frontal, precentral, postcentral, and superior temporal gyri, left middle temporal and fusiform gyri, right hippocampus, left parahippocampal gyrus, and right cerebellum Lobule 8/9 (see Figure 5b). No significant activations were found for older low performers. In contrast, significant cluster-level activations related to novelty processing of scenes for young adults (n = 48) were observed in the right middle frontal and fusiform gyri, right hippocampus, and left parahippocampal gyrus (see Figure 5c). Group-level comparisons among the different age or performance groups for novelty processing of scenes did not reveal significant differences in functional activations.
Relational processing of scenes. Results of supra-threshold cluster-level activations, at p < 0.05 (FDR-corrected) with k ≥ 20 for N > S contrast representing novelty processing of scenes, are shown in Table 2. Participants’ gray matter probability, SFNR value, and ACC in both Novel Pictures and Scrambled Pictures conditions were controlled as covariates in group-level analyses. Significant cluster-level activations related to novelty processing of scenes for older adults (n = 33) were observed in the left superior frontal gyrus, bilateral superior temporal gyrus, left middle temporal and angular gyri, right middle temporal gyrus, bilateral insula, left cingulate gyrus and anterior cingulate, bilateral posterior cingulate, left hippocampus, and bilateral parahippocampus gyrus (see Figure 6a). Among the older adults, only the older high performers (n = 24) showed significant activations in the left superior frontal gyrus, bilateral superior temporal gyrus, left middle temporal gyrus, right inferior temporal gyrus, left angular and superior occipital gyri, bilateral insula, left cingulate gyrus, bilateral anterior and posterior cingulate, and bilateral parahippocampal gyri (see Figure 6b). No significant activations were found for older low performers. In contrast, significant cluster-level activations related to relational processing of scenes for young adults (n = 48) were observed in the right inferior frontal gyrus, left middle temporal gyrus, posterior cingulate and parahippocampal gyrus, bilateral cerebellum Crus II, right cerebellum Crus I, and right cerebellum Lobule 9 (see Figure 6c). BA, Brodmann Area.
Age-related differences in functional activation have also been found for the relational processing of scenes. Young adults versus older adults (n = 81) revealed significantly greater activations in the bilateral superior frontal and middle frontal gyri, left inferior frontal gyrus, bilateral medial frontal gyrus, left precentral gyrus, right inferior parietal lobule, bilateral precuneus, right superior temporal gyrus, bilateral middle temporal gyrus, cuneus, and lingual gyrus, left cingulate gyrus, right anterior cingulate, left posterior cingulate, bilateral parahippocampal gyrus, left fusiform gyrus, right cerebellum Crus I, right cerebellum Lobule 7b, left cerebellum Crus II, right cerebellum Lobule CrusI/8/9 (see Figure 6d). In particular, when compared against older low performers (n = 57), young performers showed significantly greater activations in the bilateral parahippocampal gyrus (see Figure 6e). BA, Brodmann Area.

3.3. Functional Connectivity Analyses

Novelty processing of scenes. ROI-to-ROI functional connectivity analyses were conducted with a priori ROIs, namely left and right IFG, hippocampus, and parahippocampus, at p < 0.05 (FDR-corrected), for N > R contrast representing novelty processing of scenes. Participants’ intracranial brain volume and ACC in both Novel Pictures and Repeating Pictures conditions were controlled as covariates in group-level analyses. However, no significant functional connectivity was found among these ROIs.
Relational processing of scenes. ROI-to-ROI functional connectivity analyses were conducted with a priori ROIs, namely left and right IFG, hippocampus, and parahippocampus at p < 0.05 (FDR-corrected) for N > S contrast representing relational processing of scenes. Results are shown in Table 3. Participants’ intracranial brain volume and ACC in both Novel Pictures and Scrambled Pictures conditions were controlled as covariates in group-level analyses. Significant positive left parahippocampus–right parahippocampus functional connectivity was found for older low performers (n = 6) (see Figure 7a). In contrast, young performers (n = 25) revealed significant positive left hippocampus–right hippocampus functional connectivity (see Figure 7b). In terms of group comparisons, marginal significance was found between young and older low performers for the relational processing of scenes. In particular, young performers demonstrated greater negative left IFG–right hippocampus and left IFG–right parahippocampus functional connectivity than older low performers (see Figure 7c). It is noteworthy that the age-related increase, not decrease, of the functional connectivity was found in the low-performing older adults, a pattern predicted by the PASA model.

4. Discussion

We predicted that the PASA model may be extended to the relation between the hippocampal formation and the frontal regions, particularly the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG). Our prediction was partially supported by the results from the present study: In the relational processing, age-related changes in activations and functional connectivity between IFG (i.e., top-down modulation) and hippocampal/parahippocampal structures (i.e., bottom-up processing) were identified to be performance-related for the old adults (see Young > Old-Low adults in Table 2 and Table 3). The detail will be discussed below.

4.1. The Extended PASA Model between IFG and Hippocampus/Parahippocampus

During the relational task, the low-performing older adults showed less parahippocampal activations but more functional connectivity between the hippocampal/parahippocampal and IFG compared with the young adults. These differences were not found when comparing the high-performing older adults with the young adults. We conclude that the parahippocampal modulation found in the low-performing older adults is in line with the posterior aspect of the PASA model, namely the decrease in activations in the aging brain. We also conjecture this parahippocampal alteration in low-performing older adults may be interpreted in relation to compensation because positive prefrontal-hippocampus functional connectivity is usually associated with better memory performance [31]. However, it does not seem to provide convincing support for the compensation view. We will revisit this problem later.
Some of the past literature has shown activations in the IFG, hippocampus, and parahippocampus were found for both older and young adults during novelty and relational processing of scenes [11,13,14,32]. Our results are consistent with these findings. Furthermore, the intact parahippocampal activation in high-performing older adults and the lack of it in low-performing older adults in our results validated the view that the parahippocampus subserves the encoding process [13,33] as well as the associative processing [34] of scenes.

4.2. The Frontal Aspect of the PASA Model Was Not Replicated

In the current study, we did not observe the activation increase in the IFG in the older adults compared with the young adults, although this aging-related increase in the anterior brain is predicted as a part of the PASA effect. Even after separating the older adults into high- and low-performance subgroups, the predicted age-related increase was not found. Thus, our results did not replicate the previous studies [12,13,14,35].
We observed that young but not older adults showed significant activation in the IFG during the relational processing task. The role of IFG, an area in the PFC, during this task, is to maintain the encoded information in a top-down manner [36]. The IFG activation observed in our study may suggest that the young adults could maintain the encoded information better than the older adults. This is consistent with the fact that functional connectivity between PFC and MTL is required for successful memory encoding [36,37,38,39]. One possible explanation for not observing the expected activation increase in IFG in the older adults is that the task design for the encoding process did not work as intended. The detail of this interpretation will be discussed in the Limitation section. Another possibility is that the anterior aspect of the PASA model is less robust than the posterior aspect of the PASA model.

4.3. Compensation vs. Dedifferentiation

Although the current consensus of the field gravitates toward the compensation view when interpreting the functional role of the PASA model [9], the alternative view is that the age-related increase in brain activation is due to a loss of functional specificities. This concept is called de-differentiation, which is conceptually the reversal process of functional differentiation that occurs during the developmental period [40]. According to this view, the PASA model is a representation of functional degrading that does not necessarily have a compensatory effect. The results from the current study cannot provide direct evidence to support either view because the group difference between high- and low-performing older adults did not show significant differences. The increase of the prefrontal-hippocampus functional connectivity found in the low-performing older adults may be interpreted as a trace of the compensation because usually it is associated with better memory performance [31], but from the behavioral results it is no longer helping. To summarize, the current study did not yield convincing evidence that supports either the compensation view or the differentiation view.

4.4. Cerebellar Participation

We observed cerebellum activation in both novelty and relational processing. This is consistent with past studies showing cerebellar involvement in episodic encoding [41,42,43,44]. We found right inferior cerebellar activity only in older adults and not young adults for novelty processing. In contrast, greater bilateral superior and right inferior cerebellum activation was observed in young adults for relational processing. It has been suggested that the right superior cerebellum is associated with encoding success for semantic information of relational processing and the bilateral superior cerebellum with non-semantic information of letters [43]. Specifically, right Crus 1 was found to be associated with the retrieval of episodic memory [41]. Our findings provided further evidence for the involvement of the cerebellum in episodic memory.

4.5. Other Thoughts on the Analysis Method

We demonstrated in this study that task performance during fMRI can adequately separate the older adults into low and high performers. Conventionally, this separation would require separate standardized cognitive tests. Our approach ensures that we examine the actual difference directly related to the task without relying on the assumption that individuals who scored better on cognitive tests should perform better during the fMRI task. The current approach offers an opportunity for the experimenters of past studies to further examine their findings based on participants’ fMRI task performance.

4.6. Limitations and Future Directions

The older adults in the CBL dataset showed more difficulty in recognizing non-scrambled novel scenes during the post-scan recognition test. This suggests they were less engaged during the novelty-related processing when recognizing indoor/outdoor scenes inside the MRI scanner. The lack of the expected results may be related to poor engagement during the task. When we interpret the results from the current study, this limitation should be kept in mind.
We did not control for the number of low- and high-performing older adults when separating them into the two subgroups. As a result, we had more high performers than low performers, which may have reduced the statistical power to detect the group difference. Furthermore, this subgrouping is inherently affected by age [45]. In this study, low-performing older adults were older than the high-performing older adults. However, no significant difference in the MMSE score was found between low and high-performing older adults, justifying that both performance groups are within normal cognitive limits. Hence neither age nor MMSE score were submitted as covariates in our analyses.
We did not delineate the hippocampus into its anterior and posterior regions. Studies by Ta et al. [11,14] have shown functional specialization within the hippocampus, with the anterior hippocampus responsible for the relational processing of scenes and the posterior hippocampus as the neural correlate for the novelty processing of scenes. Age-related vulnerability to structural atrophy and activation was also demonstrated, with the anterior hippocampus found to be more prone to age-related changes [14]. It remains unclear how the anterior-posterior hippocampal difference may interact with the current results.
Finally, for future studies, it would be worthwhile to examine if the PASA model and compensatory view of age-related changes in functional activation and connectivity are supported for encoding other types of stimuli, such as words, faces, or abstract patterns.

5. Conclusions

The present study provided partial support for the PASA model for scene encoding. In the relational processing, age-related changes in activations and functional connectivity between IFG (i.e., top-down modulation) and hippocampal/parahippocampal structures (i.e., bottom-up processing) were identified to be performance-related for the old adults. However, the expected activation increase in IFG in older adults was not observed. Thus, the posterior aspect of the PASA model was supported, but the anterior aspect of it was not directly supported by the BOLD signal increase but instead indicated by the increase of parahippocampal-IFG functional connectivity. The current study did not provide direct support for the compensatory view of the PASA model.

Author Contributions

M.M. developed the task stimuli, collected the data at NCGG, and wrote the revised manuscript. J.A.A. advised on data analyses and the initial write-up of the draft. C.-Y.W. advised on the data analysis and conducted the data collection at CBL. T.N. supervised the research at NCGG and was involved in reviewing the manuscript. S.-H.A.C. conceptualized, supervised the overall research, and was involved in reviewing and editing the manuscript drafts. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research is supported by the Ministry of Education, MOE2019-T2-1-019 (S), Singapore and Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) under Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research [KAKENHI #15H03104 and 19H04025].

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the ethics committees of the National Center for Geriatrics and Gerontology, Japan, and Nanyang Technological University, Singapore.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data is available upon request from the authors.

Acknowledgments

We would like to acknowledge Dayton Leow for contributing to the data analysis and writing the initial draft of the manuscript as part of his final year project, and the support from Nanyang Technological University under the Undergraduate Research Experience on Campus (URECA) program.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

  1. Cabeza, R.; Grady, C.L.; Nyberg, L.; McIntosh, A.R.; Tulving, E.; Kapur, S.; Jennings, J.M.; Houle, S.; Craik, F.I. Age-related differences in neural activity during memory encoding and retrieval: A positron emission tomography study. J. Neurosci. 1997, 17, 391–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Madden, D.J.; Turkington, T.G.; Provenzale, J.M.; Denny, L.L.; Hawk, T.C.; Gottlob, L.R.; Coleman, R.E. Adult age differences in the functional neuroanatomy of verbal recognition memory. Hum. Brain Mapp. 1999, 7, 115–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Cabeza, R.; Anderson, N.D.; Locantore, J.K.; McIntosh, A.R. Aging gracefully: Compensatory brain activity in high-performing older adults. Neuroimage 2002, 17, 1394–1402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Manenti, R.; Cotelli, M.; Miniussi, C. Successful physiological aging and episodic memory: A brain stimulation study. Behav. Brain Res. 2011, 216, 153–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Daselaar, S.M.; Fleck, M.S.; Dobbins, I.G.; Madden, D.J.; Cabeza, R. Effects of healthy aging on hippocampal and rhinal memory functions: An event-related fMRI study. Cereb. Cortex 2006, 16, 1771–1782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Grady, C.L.; Maisog, J.M.; Horwitz, B.; Ungerleider, L.G.; Mentis, M.J.; Salerno, J.A.; Pietrini, P.; Wagner, E.; Haxby, J.V. Age-related changes in cortical blood flow activation during visual processing of faces and location. J. Neurosci. 1994, 14, 1450–1462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Davis, S.W.; Dennis, N.A.; Daselaar, S.M.; Fleck, M.S.; Cabeza, R. Que PASA? The posterior-anterior shift in aging. Cereb. Cortex 2008, 18, 1201–1209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Sala-Llonch, R.; Junqué, C.; Arenaza-Urquijo, E.M.; Vidal-Piñeiro, D.; Valls-Pedret, C.; Palacios, E.M.; Domènech, S.; Salvà, A.; Bargalló, N.; Bartrés-Faz, D. Changes in whole-brain functional networks and memory performance in aging. Neurobiol. Aging 2014, 35, 2193–2202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Myrum, C. Is PASA passé?: Rethinking compensatory mechanisms in cognitive aging. J. Neurosci. 2019, 39, 786–787. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Phillips, L.H.; Andrés, P. The cognitive neuroscience of aging: New findings on compensation and connectivity. Cortex 2010, 46, 421–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Binder, J.R.; Bellgowan, P.S.F.; Hammeke, T.A.; Possing, E.T.; Frost, J.A. A comparison of two FMRI protocols for eliciting hippocampal activation. Epilepsia 2005, 46, 1061–1070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Brewer, J.B.; Zhao, Z.; Desmond, J.E.; Glover, G.H.; Gabrieli, J.D. Making memories: Brain activity that predicts how well visual experience will be remembered. Science 1998, 281, 1185–1187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Gutchess, A.H.; Welsh, R.C.; Hedden, T.; Bangert, A.; Minear, M.; Liu, L.L.; Park, D.C. Aging and the neural correlates of successful picture encoding: Frontal activations compensate for decreased medial-temporal activity. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2005, 17, 84–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Ta, A.T.; Huang, S.-E.; Chiu, M.-J.; Hua, M.-S.; Tseng, W.-Y.I.; Chen, S.-H.A.; Qiu, A. Age-related vulnerabilities along the hippocampal longitudinal axis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2012, 33, 2415–2427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Miyakoshi, M.; Chen, S.-H.A.; Matsuo, K.; Wu, C.-Y.; Suzuki, A.; Nakai, T. Extensive stimulus repetition leads older adults to show delayed functional magnetic resonance imaging adaptation. Brain Imaging Behav. 2012, 6, 357–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Raz, N.; Gunning-Dixon, F.M.; Head, D.; Dupuis, J.H.; Acker, J.D. Neuroanatomical correlates of cognitive aging: Evidence from structural magnetic resonance imaging. Neuropsychology 1998, 12, 95–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Echávarri, C.; Aalten, P.; Uylings, H.B.M.; Jacobs, H.I.L.; Visser, P.J.; Gronenschild, E.H.B.M.; Verhey, F.R.J.; Burgmans, S. Atrophy in the parahippocampal gyrus as an early biomarker of Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Struct. Funct. 2011, 215, 265–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Folstein, M.F.; Folstein, S.E.; McHugh, P.R. Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 1975, 12, 189–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Oldfield, R.C. The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 1971, 9, 97–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Friedman, L.; Glover, G.H.; Fbirn Consortium. Reducing interscanner variability of activation in a multicenter fMRI study: Controlling for signal-to-fluctuation-noise-ratio (SFNR) differences. Neuroimage 2006, 33, 471–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Giannelli, M.; Diciotti, S.; Tessa, C.; Mascalchi, M. Effect of echo spacing and readout bandwidth on basic performances of EPI-fMRI acquisition sequences implemented on two 1.5 T MR scanner systems. Med. Phys. 2010, 37, 303–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Friston, K.J.; Holmes, A.P.; Worsley, K.J.; Poline, J.P.; Frith, C.D.; Frackowiak, R.S.J. Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach. Hum. Brain Mapp. 1994, 2, 189–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Ashburner, J. A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage 2007, 38, 95–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Bergouignan, L.; Chupin, M.; Czechowska, Y.; Kinkingnéhun, S.; Lemogne, C.; Le Bastard, G.; Lepage, M.; Garnero, L.; Colliot, O.; Fossati, P. Can voxel based morphometry, manual segmentation and automated segmentation equally detect hippocampal volume differences in acute depression? Neuroimage 2009, 45, 29–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Peelle, J.E.; Cusack, R.; Henson, R.N.A. Adjusting for global effects in voxel-based morphometry: Gray matter decline in normal aging. Neuroimage 2012, 60, 1503–1516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Casanova, R.; Srikanth, R.; Baer, A.; Laurienti, P.J.; Burdette, J.H.; Hayasaka, S.; Flowers, L.; Wood, F.; Maldjian, J.A. Biological parametric mapping: A statistical toolbox for multimodality brain image analysis. Neuroimage 2007, 34, 137–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Chai, X.J.; Castañón, A.N.; Ongür, D.; Whitfield-Gabrieli, S. Anticorrelations in resting state networks without global signal regression. Neuroimage 2012, 59, 1420–1428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Whitfield-Gabrieli, S.; Nieto-Castanon, A. Conn: A functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connect. 2012, 2, 125–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Walter, B.; Blecker, C.; Kirsch, P.; Sammer, G.; Schienle, A.; Stark, R. MARINA: An Easy to use Tool for the Creationof MAsks for Region of Interest Analyses. 2003. Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/286632632_MARINA_An_easy_to_use_tool_for_the_creation_of_MAsks_for_Region_of_INterest_Analyses (accessed on 3 March 2023).
  30. Brett, M.; Anton, J.L.; Valabrgue, R.; Poline, J.B. Region of Interest Analysis Using an SPM Toolbox. In Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain., Sendai, Japan, 2–6 June 2002; Volume 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Grady, C.L.; McIntosh, A.R.; Craik, F.I.M. Age-related differences in the functional connectivity of the hippocampus during memory encoding. Hippocampus 2003, 13, 572–586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Tang, L.; Pruitt, P.J.; Yu, Q.; Homayouni, R.; Daugherty, A.M.; Damoiseaux, J.S.; Ofen, N. Differential functional connectivity in anterior and posterior hippocampus supporting the development of memory formation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2020, 14, 204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Chai, X.J.; Ofen, N.; Jacobs, L.F.; Gabrieli, J.D.E. Scene complexity: Influence on perception, memory, and development in the medial temporal lobe. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2010, 4, 21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Bar, M. The proactive brain: Using analogies and associations to generate predictions. Trends Cogn. Sci. Regul. Ed. 2007, 11, 280–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Leshikar, E.D.; Gutchess, A.H.; Hebrank, A.C.; Sutton, B.P.; Park, D.C. The impact of increased relational encoding demands on frontal and hippocampal function in older adults. Cortex 2010, 46, 507–521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Simons, J.S.; Koutstaal, W.; Prince, S.; Wagner, A.D.; Schacter, D.L. Neural mechanisms of visual object priming: Evidence for perceptual and semantic distinctions in fusiform cortex. Neuroimage 2003, 19, 613–626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Kirchhoff, B.A.; Wagner, A.D.; Maril, A.; Stern, C.E. Prefrontal-temporal circuitry for episodic encoding and subsequent memory. J. Neurosci. 2000, 20, 6173–6180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Schacter, D.L.; Wagner, A.D. Medial temporal lobe activations in fMRI and PET studies of episodic encoding and retrieval. Hippocampus 1999, 9, 7–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Buckner, R.L.; Kelley, W.M.; Petersen, S.E. Frontal cortex contributes to human memory formation. Nat. Neurosci. 1999, 2, 311–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Garrett, H.E. A developmental theory of intelligence. Am. Psychol. 1946, 1, 372–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Addis, D.R.; Moloney, E.E.J.; Tippett, L.J.; P Roberts, R.; Hach, S. Characterizing cerebellar activity during autobiographical memory retrieval: ALE and functional connectivity investigations. Neuropsychologia 2016, 90, 80–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Andreasen, N.C.; O’Leary, D.S.; Paradiso, S.; Cizadlo, T.; Arndt, S.; Watkins, G.L.; Ponto, L.L.B.; Hichwa, R.D. The cerebellum plays a role in conscious episodic memory retrieval. Hum. Brain Mapp. 1999, 8, 226–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Fliessbach, K.; Trautner, P.; Quesada, C.M.; Elger, C.E.; Weber, B. Cerebellar contributions to episodic memory encoding as revealed by fMRI. Neuroimage 2007, 35, 1330–1337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Gatti, D.; Vecchi, T.; Mazzoni, G. Cerebellum and semantic memory: A TMS study using the DRM paradigm. Cortex 2021, 135, 78–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Hughes, M.L.; Agrigoroaei, S.; Jeon, M.; Bruzzese, M.; Lachman, M.E. Change in Cognitive Performance from Midlife Into Old Age: Findings from the Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) Study. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2018, 24, 805–820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Stimulus presentation to each participant was blocked by the condition in the order of (a) Novel Pictures condition, (b) Repeating Pictures condition, and (c) Scrambled Pictures condition for 5 cycles. (d) Each block lasted 24 s and one run took 360 s in total.
Figure 1. Stimulus presentation to each participant was blocked by the condition in the order of (a) Novel Pictures condition, (b) Repeating Pictures condition, and (c) Scrambled Pictures condition for 5 cycles. (d) Each block lasted 24 s and one run took 360 s in total.
Sensors 23 04107 g001
Figure 2. (a) Age × condition mixed design ANOVA on ACC revealed a significant interaction effect and a main effect of condition, at p < 0.05. (b) Age × condition mixed design ANOVA on RT revealed significant interaction effect, main effects of age and condition, at p < 0.05 *.
Figure 2. (a) Age × condition mixed design ANOVA on ACC revealed a significant interaction effect and a main effect of condition, at p < 0.05. (b) Age × condition mixed design ANOVA on RT revealed significant interaction effect, main effects of age and condition, at p < 0.05 *.
Sensors 23 04107 g002
Figure 3. (a) Performance × condition mixed design ANOVA on ACC revealed significant interaction effect, main effects of performance and condition, at p < 0.05. (b) Performance × condition mixed design ANOVA on RT revealed significant interaction effect, main effects of performance and condition, at p < 0.05 *.
Figure 3. (a) Performance × condition mixed design ANOVA on ACC revealed significant interaction effect, main effects of performance and condition, at p < 0.05. (b) Performance × condition mixed design ANOVA on RT revealed significant interaction effect, main effects of performance and condition, at p < 0.05 *.
Sensors 23 04107 g003
Figure 4. Age × condition mixed design ANOVA on post-scan ACC revealed significant interaction effect, main effects of age and condition, at p < 0.05 *.
Figure 4. Age × condition mixed design ANOVA on post-scan ACC revealed significant interaction effect, main effects of age and condition, at p < 0.05 *.
Sensors 23 04107 g004
Figure 5. Significant cluster-level activations for novelty processing of scenes (N > R contrast), at p < 0.05 (FDR-corrected) with k ≥ 20, after adjusting for gray matter probability, signal-to-fluctuation-noise (SFNR), and accuracy (ACC) performance. In the figure, from the left panel: (a) Significant activation among older adults (n = 33) for novelty processing of scenes (N > R contrast); (b) Significant activation among older high performers (n = 24) for novelty processing of scenes (N > R contrast); (c) Significant activation among young adults (n = 48) for novelty processing of scenes (N > R contrast).
Figure 5. Significant cluster-level activations for novelty processing of scenes (N > R contrast), at p < 0.05 (FDR-corrected) with k ≥ 20, after adjusting for gray matter probability, signal-to-fluctuation-noise (SFNR), and accuracy (ACC) performance. In the figure, from the left panel: (a) Significant activation among older adults (n = 33) for novelty processing of scenes (N > R contrast); (b) Significant activation among older high performers (n = 24) for novelty processing of scenes (N > R contrast); (c) Significant activation among young adults (n = 48) for novelty processing of scenes (N > R contrast).
Sensors 23 04107 g005
Figure 6. Significant cluster-level activations for relational processing of scenes (N > S contrast), at p < 0.05 (FDR-corrected) with k ≥ 20, after adjusting for gray matter probability, signal-to-fluctuation-noise (SFNR), and accuracy (ACC) performance. In the figure, from the top left panel: (a) Significant activation among older adults (n = 33) for relational processing of scenes (N > S contrast); (b) Significant activation among older high performers (n = 24) for relational processing of scenes (N > S contrast); (c) Significant activation among young adults (n = 48) for relational processing of scenes (N > S contrast); (d) Significant activation for young versus older adults (n = 81) for relational processing of scenes (N > S contrast); (e) Significant activation for young versus older low performers (n = 57) for relational processing of scenes (N > S contrast).
Figure 6. Significant cluster-level activations for relational processing of scenes (N > S contrast), at p < 0.05 (FDR-corrected) with k ≥ 20, after adjusting for gray matter probability, signal-to-fluctuation-noise (SFNR), and accuracy (ACC) performance. In the figure, from the top left panel: (a) Significant activation among older adults (n = 33) for relational processing of scenes (N > S contrast); (b) Significant activation among older high performers (n = 24) for relational processing of scenes (N > S contrast); (c) Significant activation among young adults (n = 48) for relational processing of scenes (N > S contrast); (d) Significant activation for young versus older adults (n = 81) for relational processing of scenes (N > S contrast); (e) Significant activation for young versus older low performers (n = 57) for relational processing of scenes (N > S contrast).
Sensors 23 04107 g006
Figure 7. Significant ROI-to-ROI functional connectivity for relational processing of scenes (N > S contrast), at p < 0.05 (FDR-corrected), after adjusting for intracranial brain volume and ACC performance. N: Novel; S: Scrambled. The in-plot descriptions, top left, (a) Significant positive left parahippocampus-right parahippocampus functional connectivity among older low performers (n = 6) for relational processing of scenes (N > S contrast); Top right, (b) Significant positive left hippocampus-right hippocampus functional connectivity among young performers (n = 25) for relational processing of scenes (N > S contrast); Bottom, (c) Significant negative left IFG-right hippocampus/parahippocampus functional connectivity for young versus older low performers (n = 31) for relational processing of scenes (N > S contrast).
Figure 7. Significant ROI-to-ROI functional connectivity for relational processing of scenes (N > S contrast), at p < 0.05 (FDR-corrected), after adjusting for intracranial brain volume and ACC performance. N: Novel; S: Scrambled. The in-plot descriptions, top left, (a) Significant positive left parahippocampus-right parahippocampus functional connectivity among older low performers (n = 6) for relational processing of scenes (N > S contrast); Top right, (b) Significant positive left hippocampus-right hippocampus functional connectivity among young performers (n = 25) for relational processing of scenes (N > S contrast); Bottom, (c) Significant negative left IFG-right hippocampus/parahippocampus functional connectivity for young versus older low performers (n = 31) for relational processing of scenes (N > S contrast).
Sensors 23 04107 g007
Table 1. Significant cluster-level activations for novelty encoding of scenes (N > R contrast), at p < 0.05 (FDR-corrected) with k ≥ 20.
Table 1. Significant cluster-level activations for novelty encoding of scenes (N > R contrast), at p < 0.05 (FDR-corrected) with k ≥ 20.
Brain RegionHemisphereBACluster SizePeak ActivationMNI Coordinates
p-Value (FDR-Corrected)t-Valuexyz
Old adults
Superior frontal gyrusR637<0.013.4921−675
Precentral gyrusR622<0.013.7063326.25
<0.052.3863615
434<0.053.1645−960
<0.052.6839−2167.5
Postcentral gyrusR1 <0.052.4154−1856.25
Superior temporal gyrusL2145<0.013.25−54−9−15
22 <0.013.24−633−11.25
Middle temporal gyrusL21 <0.052.86−606−22.5
Fusiform gyrusL1913,263<0.00112.51−27−60−7.5
Parahippocampal gyrusL36 <0.00111.74−24−42−7.5
R <0.00110.5433−33−15
Cerebellum (Cbl) Lobule 9R 106<0.0014.8321−42−48.75
Old-High adults
Middle frontal gyrusR623<0.053.28301863.75
<0.052.80332756.25
Precentral gyrusR6104<0.013.7860326.25
4 <0.013.6069−930
6 <0.013.5766915
440<0.053.0545−1260
Postcentral gyrusR3 <0.053.3036−2471.25
<0.052.5648−2463.75
Superior temporal gyrusR2246<0.0054.02630−11.25
<0.0053.97639−11.25
Middle temporal gyrusL2177<0.013.72−630−15
<0.013.64−54−15−18.75
<0.053.42−606−22.5
Fusiform gyrusL1912,670<0.00111.61−27−63−7.5
HippocampusR <0.00110.1233−30−15
Parahippocampal gyrusL36 <0.00110.03−24−42−7.5
Cbl Lobule 9R 48<0.0053.9421−42−48.75
Cbl Lobule 8 <0.052.7833−48−52.5
Cbl Louble 9 <0.052.6115−54−45
Young adults
Middle frontal gyrusR4638<0.0053.32514215
Fusiform gyrusR3723,638<0.00116.3833−45−15
HippocampusR <0.00115.7936−36−11.25
Parahippocampal gyrusL36 <0.00118.01−30−42−7.5
Table 2. Significant cluster-level activations for relational encoding of scenes (N > S contrast), at p < 0.05 (FDR-corrected) with k ≥ 20.
Table 2. Significant cluster-level activations for relational encoding of scenes (N > S contrast), at p < 0.05 (FDR-corrected) with k ≥ 20.
Brain RegionHemisphereBACluster SizePeak ActivationMNI Coordinates
p-Value (FDR-Corrected)t-Valuexyz
Old adults
Superior frontal gyrusL9563<0.0015.66−126011.25
Superior temporal gyrusR39199<0.0017.1057−5722.5
L38226<0.0017.10−54−3−11.25
<0.0054.71−459−30
R2286<0.0054.26603−11.25
Middle temporal gyrusL39302<0.0019.06−42−7233.75
21226<0.013.97−54−18−18.75
Angular gyrusL39302<0.0019.53−42−7541.25
Middle temporal gyrusR2186<0.0054.4051−9−18.75
InsulaR1395<0.0015.2439−1818.75
L 27<0.0054.22−36−2422.5
Cingulate gyrusL31716<0.0015.573−6030
Anterior cingulateL24563<0.0017.64024−15
32 <0.0016.05051−11.25
Posterior cingulateL30716<0.0019.85−9−5415
R23 <0.0018.0012−5418.75
HippocampusL 372<0.0018.86−30−27−15
Parahippocampal gyrusL36372<0.00110.45−27−42−7.5
R 325<0.0017.7430−45−7.5
35 <0.0017.1430−30−15
28 <0.0016.2521−12−18.75
Old-High adults
Superior frontal gyrusL9590<0.0016.91−126011.25
Superior temporal gyrusR39183<0.0018.4357−5722.5
L38210<0.0016.68−54−3−11.25
<0.0054.67−459−30
R3893<0.0054.72600−15
Middle temporal gyrusL39296<0.0018.76−45−7233.75
21210<0.0055.19−54−12−18.75
Inferior temporal gyrusR2093<0.014.2251−9−22.5
Angular gyrusL39296<0.0018.98−42−7541.25
Superior occipital gyrusL19296<0.0019.73−42−8133.75
InsulaR1376<0.0055.4839−1818.75
L1327<0.014.34−36−1818.75
<0.054.06−42−1215
Cingulate gyrusL31642<0.0055.623−6030
Anterior cingulateL32590<0.0017.12−948−3.75
<0.0016.90−351−11.25
R2425<0.014.3632711.25
Posterior cingulateL23642<0.0019.37−9−5718.75
R23642<0.0017.7712−5418.75
Parahippocampal gyrusL36337<0.0018.75−27−42−7.5
<0.0017.66−30−30−18.75
<0.0016.44−24−12−22.5
R36294<0.0016.9227−45−7.5
35 <0.0016.1130−30−15
28 <0.0055.3521−12−18.75
Young adults
Inferior frontal gyrusR4730<0.0015.053333−11.25
Middle temporal gyrusL3913,094<0.00116.56−45−7230
Posterior cingulateL3013,094<0.00116.82−12−5415
Parahippocampal gyrusL3613,094<0.00117.72−30−42−11.25
Cbl Crus IR 237<0.0017.7045−75−37.5
Cbl Crus IIL 54<0.0014.50−12−90−37.5
R 237<0.0017.5818−90−33.75
Cbl Crus I <0.0016.7633−90−33.75
Cbl Crus IIL 54<0.0014.74−27−90−33.75
Cbl Lobule 9R 37<0.0053.776−51−45
<0.0053.633−57−52.5
Young > Old adults
Superior frontal gyrusL6125<0.053.90−242156.25
R638<0.053.47121856.25
<0.053.30212148.75
1029<0.053.4733573.75
Middle frontal gyrusL6125<0.053.56−271245
R838<0.053.54273041.25
Inferior frontal gyrusL4533<0.054.64−60210
Medial frontal gyrusL8125<0.054.97−92745
R6360<0.054.736−1863.75
<0.054.066−1252.5
Precentral gyrusL6360<0.054.30−15−1263.75
Inferior parietal lobuleR4050<0.054.2157−4545
PrecuneusL7229<0.054.42−9−6045
<0.054.05−6−7248.75
R3958<0.053.7445−6941.25
Superior temporal gyrusR1322<0.053.7051−4818.75
Middle temporal gyrusL3930<0.054.26−48−7815
R3958<0.053.9648−7533.75
2160<0.053.9269−420
22 <0.053.8357−450
21 <0.053.1269−4811.25
CuneusR19229<0.054.3621−8141.25
L1822<0.053.54−12−8433.75
Lingual gyrusL1858<0.054.75−27−990
R1825<0.053.2318−90−11.25
Cingulate gyrusL3122<0.054.20−24−4226.25
Anterior cingulateR32143<0.055.051251−11.25
<0.054.9318390
<0.054.831542−7.5
Posterior cingulateL3035<0.053.96−24−5715
<0.053.54−6−6618.75
Parahippocampal gyrusR3631<0.054.1330−33−15
3033<0.054.0624−4511.25
<0.053.2218−420
L3048<0.053.66−18−42−7.5
<0.053.55−15−420
R 21<0.053.5233−15−22.5
Fusiform gyrusL 48<0.053.78−21−42−15
Cbl Crus IR 25<0.053.3421−87−18.75
Cbl Lobule 7bR 180<0.053.9530−75−48.75
Cbl Crus IIL 86<0.053.67−30−75−45
<0.053.61−39−72−37.5
<0.053.57−27−87−37.5
Cbl Lobule 8R 180<0.054.5336−54−52.5
Cbl Crus I <0.054.3339−63−41.25
Cbl Lobule 9 33<0.054.006−60−52.5
Young > Old-Low adults
Parahippocampal gyrusL3625<0.055.32−30−24−26.25
R3549<0.054.6230−30−18.75
36 <0.054.4236−27−26.25
<0.054.3436−24−18.75
Table 3. Significant ROI-to-ROI functional connectivity for relational encoding of scenes (N > S contrast), at p < 0.05 (FDR-corrected).
Table 3. Significant ROI-to-ROI functional connectivity for relational encoding of scenes (N > S contrast), at p < 0.05 (FDR-corrected).
ROI-ROIβp-Value (FDR-Corrected)t-Value
Old-Low adults
Left parahippocampus–Right parahippocampus0.12<0.055.83
Young adults
Left hippocampus–Right hippocampus0.12<0.052.92
Young > Old-Low adults
Left IFG–Right hippocampus−0.150.06−2.05
Left IFG–Right parahippocampus−0.230.06−2.20
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Miyakoshi, M.; Archer, J.A.; Wu, C.-Y.; Nakai, T.; Chen, S.-H.A. Age-Related Changes in Episodic Processing of Scenes: A Functional Activation and Connectivity Study. Sensors 2023, 23, 4107. https://doi.org/10.3390/s23084107

AMA Style

Miyakoshi M, Archer JA, Wu C-Y, Nakai T, Chen S-HA. Age-Related Changes in Episodic Processing of Scenes: A Functional Activation and Connectivity Study. Sensors. 2023; 23(8):4107. https://doi.org/10.3390/s23084107

Chicago/Turabian Style

Miyakoshi, Makoto, Josephine Astrid Archer, Chiao-Yi Wu, Toshiharu Nakai, and Shen-Hsing Annabel Chen. 2023. "Age-Related Changes in Episodic Processing of Scenes: A Functional Activation and Connectivity Study" Sensors 23, no. 8: 4107. https://doi.org/10.3390/s23084107

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop