Next Article in Journal
A Miniature Intermittent-Flow Respirometry System with a 3D-Printed, Palm-Sized Zebrafish Treadmill for Measuring Rest and Activity Metabolic Rates
Next Article in Special Issue
Human–Robot Interface for Embedding Sliding Adjustable Autonomy Methods
Previous Article in Journal
LRF-Net: Learning Local Reference Frames for 3D Local Shape Description and Matching
Previous Article in Special Issue
Sequential Localizing and Mapping: A Navigation Strategy via Enhanced Subsumption Architecture
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Trust in AI Agent: A Systematic Review of Facial Anthropomorphic Trustworthiness for Social Robot Design

The Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Kowloon 999077, Hong Kong
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sensors 2020, 20(18), 5087; https://doi.org/10.3390/s20185087
Submission received: 17 August 2020 / Revised: 3 September 2020 / Accepted: 4 September 2020 / Published: 7 September 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Human-Robot Interaction and Sensors for Social Robotics)

Abstract

:
As an emerging artificial intelligence system, social robot could socially communicate and interact with human beings. Although this area is attracting more and more attention, limited research has tried to systematically summarize potential features that could improve facial anthropomorphic trustworthiness for social robot. Based on the literature from human facial perception, product, and robot face evaluation, this paper systematically reviews, evaluates, and summarizes static facial features, dynamic features, their combinations, and related emotional expressions, shedding light on further exploration of facial anthropomorphic trustworthiness for social robot design.

1. Introduction

Since technology has evolved and been applied in different daily contexts [1,2,3,4], the social robot, as one of the representatives of latest innovation, is an artificial intelligence system that could socially communicate and interact with human beings [5,6,7]. Different from traditional humanoid robots (e.g., robotic product, Zora Robot) that are physically embodied with specific human features, some latest social robots (e.g., robotic products, Jibo, Welbo, Misa, QTrobot, Hub, Mykie, and Buddy Robot) are designed with a screen, interfaced with an animated human-like face, to communicate and interact with people [8,9]. For example, Figure 1 shows the Buddy Robot, designed with human-like eyes and mouth, could emotionally accompany and interact with humans, and respond to human needs. Indeed, it might be necessary to design a head-like interface for a social robot to facilitate communication in the human-robot relationship [10,11] since social cognition and perception processes in humans might encourage people to generalize their human-related knowledge and recognition to form an expectation on the behavioral interaction with a social robot [12].
Among various characteristics perceptions, such as dominance, friendliness, and attractiveness, trustworthiness towards a social robot plays a crucial role in human-robot interaction (HRI) for two reasons [13]. On the one hand, trustworthiness is crucial in social contexts since it has a significant impact on persuasion and could directly influence people’s intention to follow others’ suggestions [14,15]. On the other hand, social robots work as “communicators and reactors”, providing not only physical help but also emotional support to human, thus they should be initially perceived to be safe to be trusted in [16].
Furthermore, Gompei and Umemuro [17] have indicated that there are several issues playing crucial roles in determining trustworthiness in social robots: robot relevant issues (e.g., the characteristics and performance of the robot), human-relevant issues (the specific need, propensity to trust, personality, comfort, self-confidence, attitude, memory, attention, expertise, competency, workload, prior experience, and situation awareness), and scenario relevant issues (task application, task complexity, multi-tasking requirement, physical environment, in-group membership, culture, communication, team collaboration, etc.). Among those, robot-relevant issues are the most significant factors influencing people’s trustworthiness evaluation towards human-robot interaction [18,19]. To be more specific, they might be related to robot behavior, dependability, reliability, predictability, automation, failure rate, transparency, proximity, personality, adaptability, type, and anthropomorphism [18]. For example, prior studies have suggested screened anthropomorphic face tended to make people feel more arousal and more likable [10,20,21], eventually leading to a higher level of perceived trustworthiness for social robots (compared with a mechanical face of social robots) [12].
Indeed, people could evaluate faces on humans and on inanimate objects, such as robots and products, in incredibly limited time [22]. Previous research indicated 100 ms was sufficient for humans to be able to judge multiple personality traits, such as trustworthiness, competence, and aggressiveness [23]. The reason why people seem to be ready to perceive and process faces in objects lie in human evolutionary adaptation: the human face is a salient evolutionary and attention-catching stimuli that would be processed simultaneously [24]. When it comes to evaluating a robot, unlike simply intending to look for the resemblance to a human face, people could perceive concrete facial traits or expressions in robot by aligning particular robot features with human characteristics and making the analogy [10,11,25].
Facial features in a social robot might also have an impact on anthropomorphic trustworthiness for such artificial agents [17,19,26]. Previous relevant review papers on trustworthiness focus a lot on summarizing human facial trust features and discussing general trustworthiness in human-computer/human-machine relationship. For example, Hancock et al. [18] evaluate the effects of human, robot, and environmental factors on perceived trust in HRI in general. However, the term of trust is indeed a multi-constructs concept that contained several evaluation stages, such as initial evaluation in the first impression and post-evaluation in latter stages [23]. Besides, although previous literature has tried to assess the role of different facial features in the processing of trust [27], it could not be simply introduced in HRI due to the anthropomorphic nature of social robots [12]. Indeed, research on the facial design of the social robot is a multi-disciplinary field that is rarely systematically analyzed and sporadically studied by various fields. Specifically, there are at least three major disciplinary relating to the facial anthropomorphic trustworthiness of social robots [28]: (1) Since human and social robots might share similar facial features, such as eye and mouth, human facial trustworthiness from psychology, which has long discussed human-specific features on facial trustworthiness [29], might potentially contribute to the knowledge of facial anthropomorphic trustworthiness of social robot. (2) As a robotic product in the commercial market, the social robot might draw inspiration from previous literature on product appearance from marketing and engineering design, which have been discussed on how to build a trustworthy appearance for anthropomorphic products [30]. Anthropomorphic product appearance refers to the physical appearance of a product with human facial traits or features, such as the headlamp of a car, or the power pin of a plug [31]. For example, Maeng and Aggarwal [25] suggested the frontal face of an automobile with lower facial width-height-ratio (fWHR) might enjoy higher trustworthiness evaluation. Although anthropomorphic product design is not directly related to robot facial design, it might also provide, at least, some intuitions for designing a trustworthy robot since they might all share similar anthropomorphic features in communicating trustworthiness. (3) Although previous social robot literature has examined the facial trustworthiness of social robots, it mainly focused on the general effect of the anthropomorphic trustworthiness evaluation, such as the trustworthiness difference between anthropomorphic face and mechanical face in social robots [12]. Indeed, it is a multi-disciplinary research field while single research field could hardly provide specific guidance to help social robot designers and engineers improving trustworthiness on the robot’s face. Although both robot and behavior research has realized the significance of social robot design for its market success and related social benefit for its users [8,22,32], the specific facial features in eliciting the anthropomorphic trustworthiness of social robots still received limited attention.
Regarding this theoretical and practical research gap, the research question of this study is:
What are the potential facial features in influencing facial anthropomorphic trustworthiness towards social robots?
To address this research gap, this study tries to present a systematic review of trustworthiness features from the multi-disciplinary fields (human face research, product appearance research, and robot face research), summarize and compare the related theories, and suggest the potential facial features in eliciting anthropomorphic trustworthiness, which contributes to literature on trustworthiness in HRI and sheds light on potential trustworthy facial features in social robot design.

2. Methods

2.1. Search terms, Database, and Timespan

As mentioned above, facial trustworthiness features of robot is a multidisciplinary field that should consider its robotic nature from related perspectives: since human and robot share facial structure, human facial trust features could potentially contribute to its facial anthropomorphic trustworthiness; since social robot might be considered as a robotic product, product appearance could potentially contribute to its facial anthropomorphic trustworthiness; robot’s own distinct characteristics, such as anthropomorphism, could also contribute to it. Accordingly, a systematic review of original research paper in English was conducted based on the followed search strings: search strings on facial trustworthiness contained “(face OR facial) AND (trust OR trustworthiness* OR credibility OR trust traits* OR trust features* OR trust signs*)” and strings on product or robot trustworthiness contained “(product OR robot OR anthropomorphism) AND (face OR facial) Trust”. The asterisk * indicates to search both single and plural forms of the keyword. Electronic databases were searched on 1 March, 2019. The databases included Scopus (1960 to March 2019), PsycInfo (1967 to March 2019), and Web of Science (1955 to March 2019). Detailed information was summarized in Table 1.

2.2. Search Strategy

The search was first narrowed by selecting articles within the subject area of trustworthiness, facial traits, product personality, product evaluation, robot facial trustworthiness, or any other related area of robot trustworthiness design. Articles not in the English language and not original were excluded. Relevant articles were then selected based on four main criteria: (i) the study must have used one or more trustworthiness traits of human face; (ii) the study must have used one or more trustworthiness traits of product appearance; (iii) the study must have used one or more trustworthiness traits of robot “face”; (iv) the study must have used one or more trustworthiness traits of anthropomorphized item.
The screening of articles was carried out manually in three stages: (A) title screening; (B) abstract screening; and then (C) full-text screening of the related research. The screening criteria at this stage did not retrieve the studies that discussed the general effect of trustworthy face on social judgments, rather than the effect of specific facial features; studies that reported duplicate results, rather than original results; studies that discussed the relationship between facial features and other social judgments, rather than trustworthiness; studies that discussed the characteristics of participants, rather than the specific facial features; studies that explored the neuroscientific explanation of facial trustworthiness. Figure 2 shows the process for this systematic review.

3. Results

In total, 2720 published papers (1214 from PsycInfo; 849 from Scopus; and 657 from Web of Science) were potentially identified to be related to this systematic review before the screening process (Figure 1). After excluding the duplicate papers (N = 1469), 1251 papers were then retrieved for the review. Following the screening criteria, 1056 articles were excluded due to their irrelevancy to the current review in the title and abstract screening. Then, 195 research articles were retrieved for full-text screening, out of which 45 are within the inclusion criteria 1 to 4 of this study (Table 2).

4. Discussion

The research trend on specific human, product, or robot facial anthropomorphic trustworthy features has been divided into four streams: internal, external, combinations, and emotions (see Table 3). As Calvo et al. and Santos and Young indicated [38,64], the internal features refers to the region containing the eye size, eye color, eye shape, eye gaze, eyebrow, color cues, luminance contrast, cheek, nose, lips, and mouth; the external features refers to the region containing facial width-height ratio (fWHR, refers to a ratio used to determine the width of a person’s face compared to its height), brow-nose-chin ratio, forehead-sellion-nose ratio, hair, forehead, ears, beard, chin, glasses, tattoo, age, and ethnicity; the combinations of different facial features refers to a set of facial features, which make people appear in certain characteristics, such as cuteness, symmetry, and masculine. Dynamic features refer to the movement of specific facial features, while emotional expressions refer to a set of facial features, which activate people to perceive the emotions it evoked.

4.1. Implications for Internal Features

The eye region is considered as one of the most significant features that could influence people’s evaluation of trustworthiness, both for human and product [22,52,53,64,72,73,74,75]. This region has several specific attributes that could communicate trustworthiness, such as eye size, eye shape, eye gaze, eye color, and eyebrow [64,67]. Studies on eye shape and size suggest that people with round eyes (vs. narrow) [44,59] and larger eyes (vs. smaller) [53,56] are perceived to be more trustworthy since these characteristics all shared and enjoyed the baby-face appearance traits from an evolutionary perspective [36,58,76]. In addition, eye gaze and eyebrow would also contribute to the people’s credibility. Because eye gaze and related eyebrow are crucial attention-catching cues for social recognition and social interest, the majority research on human facial features suggested that a direct-gaze (vs. looking at others) face with thin (vs. thick) and up-shaped (vs. down) inner ridge eyebrows was anticipated to be not only more trustworthy but also more attractive [52,57,59,64,67,71,77]. In the field of a social robot, there might exist a nuanced relationship between gaze and trustworthiness: Stanton and Stevens [66] suggested constant gaze, compared with averted gaze, might indicate dominance, rather than trustworthiness, and this effect was especially significant when female participants tried to evaluate the robot. As the author mentioned that one of the limitations in their work is the relatively small sample size (N = 52 in three between-subject experiments) and unbalanced gender (N = 14 for male) [66], further research might be necessary to confirm this effect [78]. Unlike other internal features, eye color is not an isolated trait but an ethnic group-related feature, appearing with other facial features within the cultural origin [63]. Although Kleisner and his colleagues [53] mentioned that brown-eyed faces are perceived to be more trustworthy than the blue-eyed faces, they further explained the difference in trustworthiness perception might be related to the facial traits associated.
Nose and mouth region are also perceived to be significant features that have an impact on people’s evaluation of trustworthiness. Prior research has speculated that the central facial properties (nose and mouth region) [61] were significantly positively correlated with attention and trustworthiness. As for the shape of the mouth, there are three types of mouth in the past literature: an upturned mouth (smiling mouth), a downturned mouth (sad mouth), and a neutral mouth [22]. Regarding this, there is a significant difference in the perceived social attributes among these three scenarios: human face or product “facial” appearance with an upturned mouth or a smiling mouth (vs. neutral and downturned) were believed to be more trustworthy, friendlier, and attractive [22,25,33,53]. Cheek, lips, and even teeth could also influence trustworthiness evaluation: people with pronounced cheekbones, wide chins, and thin lips with no-missing front teeth might look more trustworthy than people with shallow cheekbones, thin chins, full lips with missing front teeth [67,69]. When it comes to the nose, previous empirical research shows a contrast result towards the effect of nose attributes on trustworthiness evaluation. While some researchers agreed that a man with a small nose would be perceived as less robust and trustworthy [53], the major literature believed that short nose and shallow nose sellion were significant features for trustworthiness judgments [56,67]. According to evolutionary psychology, people have a strong intention to trust infants whose faces are characterized to have a pug nose [59,71]. The reason for this inconsistency may lie in that Kleisner and his colleagues [53] analyzed the attributes of the nose and its related combinations as a whole (a small nose, chin, and mouth) rather than evaluating the attributes of the nose, separately [71].
Various studies have been carried out to explore the effect of facial color cue and luminance on people’s social perception [42,79]. Numerous researches have shown that evaluations of attractiveness could be influenced by the difference in skin color and condition [80]. Regarding the judgments of trustworthiness, researchers [42] have shown that cosmetics (vs. without cosmetics) could increase facial luminance and color cue, which, in return, improved the perception of likability, attractiveness, and trustworthiness. Similarly, researchers [79] suggest that color hue could have an impact on the evaluation of attractiveness in the face and healthiness in the skin.
To sum up, a social robot with followed internal features or its combinations might be considered as more trustworthy: round eyes, large eyes, direct gaze, brown eyes, short noses, upturned mouth, increased color cue, and luminance.

4.2. Implications for External Features

Face shape, including fWHR, brow-nose-chin ratio, and forehead-sellion-nose ratio, plays an important role in trustworthiness evaluation. Among those facial ratios, fWHR is the most prominent human secondary sexual characteristic and also the most commonly explored feature that could have an impact on social recognition in previous studies [25]. To be more specific, in human perception, large fWHR (vs. small fWHR) is perceived to be more dominant, aggressive, unattractive, and untrustworthy [29,44,56]. However, in the field of product evaluation, what might be counter-intuitive is a large fWHR of product design would be like more since it works as a signal of user’s dominant status (Detailed discussion in Section 4.5) [25]. Similarly, the brow-nose-chin ratio and forehead-sellion-nose ratio are negatively correlated with trustworthiness judgment [57]. However, the relationship between these ratios and trustworthiness might differ in various contexts. For example, the brow-nose-chin ratio is the only significant predictor for rating trustworthiness of 12-years old male’s face, however, it was not significantly correlated with other ages and another gender. Forehead-sellion-nose ratio was also the only significant factor for adult’s trustworthiness, but it was not significant for other scenarios.
There are several studies trying to explore other external features, influencing the judgment of trustworthiness [59,64,71]. Prior research has shown an ambiguous relationship between forehead size and trustworthiness. Based on the evidence from cross-cultural participants, prior research [71] suggested that taller and smaller (vs. shorter and bigger) forehead could contribute to trustworthiness evaluation. However, researchers [59] indicated that infants usually had a relatively prominent forehead, small chin, and short ears that implied trustworthiness based on evolutionary psychology [25,53]. The reason why this contradiction occurs might due to the different definitions of the same word. To be more specific, the word, “taller and smaller forehead”, previous research [71] mentioned refers to a relatively small area of a forehead (= width x height) with a relatedly long height. However, the forehead size mentioned in [29] is actually the distance from the eyes to the hair. Accordingly, the definition of forehead size and height needs to be explained more clearly in different contexts. Besides, Hellström and Tekle [49] suggested people with glasses (vs. no glasses) and a beard (vs no beard) were generally considered to be more helpful and trustworthy. In addition, hair (vs. bald) or absence of facial tattoos (vs. have facial tattoos) could contribute to the evaluation of good-looking, credibility, integrity, and leadership [46,81]. However, this effect relies on different occupations. For instance, salesman, who was typically considered with hair but no glasses, was strongly correlated with untrustworthiness, unintelligence, and suspect, in return, decreasing sales while highly educated people, such as professor, who were usually considered to wear glasses, a beard but no hair, were believed to be trustworthy, intelligent, and helpful [49,50].
Age and ethnicity also work as salient factors in facial trustworthiness [38,59,64,71,79]. There is a U-shape relationship between age and trustworthiness. Specifically, babyface (young age) and old face (old age) enjoyed the higher level of trustworthiness when compared with an adult face (middle age) due to the baby-face overgeneralization effect, a stereotype that children are unreliable witnesses [38,59]. Furthermore, although the evolution of signaling has shown human might consciously adapt visual cues or characteristics to emphasize or conceal heritable facial traits, influencing social perception and recognition, different ethical or cultural groups (e.g., Chinese vs. Canadian [42] or Caucasian vs. African vs. East Asian vs. South Asian [35]) tended to share and adopt similar facial cues to judge trustworthiness and attractiveness. However, some ethnic groups (e.g., Hungarians) [35,38] or their implicit ethical attitude [63] might be biased toward their own facial ethnicity.
In this way, a social robot with followed external features or its combinations might be considered as more trustworthy: large fWHR, small brow-nose-chin ratio and forehead-sellion-nose ratio, tall forehead, short ears, small chin, babyish looking, and consistent ethnicity.

4.3. Implications for Combinations of Features

According to the baby-face overgeneralization effect (also called “baby schema”), people whose facial features have childlike traits (vs. without such traits) tended to have a high rate of cuteness and honesty, which are components of trustworthiness [57,58,59]. Typically, facial babyishness tended to have large eyes, a high brow ridge, a small chin, a pug nose, short ears, thin lips, and no-missing visible front teeth [25,56,57,59,69]. Despite all these social benefits, a “babyface” could be anticipated as being the opposite of dominance, namely, being considered as socially dependent, intellectually naive, and physically weak [25]. In accordance with a baby’s face, a feminine face (vs. masculine) usually shared similar facial traits, such as a bigger eye and short eye spacing [82,83]. Thus, people would believe masculine faces to be generally more dominant, less cooperative, and less honest while people would assume feminine faces to be more dependable, more cooperative, and more trustworthy [40,53].
In addition, people would have a high rating of trustworthiness towards those who looked similar to the perceivers, those who looked typical in perceivers’ cultural group or affiliation, those who previously presented before, and those whose face looked symmetrical [43,54,61,65,68,72]. The reasons lie in that both similarity and typicality could increase the familiarity that could eventually enhance positive evaluation of trustworthiness [43,61,65]. Exposure to socially relevant information could influence facial prototypes, shaping the unknown facial information processing, which mainly relies on the expectation of real-life experience. For example, our perceptions of strangers might be relied on the generalization of behavioral traits associated with previously seen facial features [54]. Furthermore, an evolutionary connection has well documented the relationship between symmetry in the face and trustworthiness since facial symmetry is a strong indicator of attractiveness and fitness [72]. When there exist hemifacial asymmetries, the left hemi-face (vs. right hemi-face) is responsible to communicate trustworthiness more efficiently in happy expressions because left hemi-face is associated with the emotional side of the brain (the right hemisphere), having the advantage to conceal anti-social intentions than right hemi-face [61].

4.4. Implications for Dynamic Features and Emotions

Regarding the effect of facial movements on trustworthiness, previous studies have generally focused on the dynamic features of three regions: eye region movement, mouth region movement, and head movement. As discussed in Section 4.1, compared with eyes blink, eyes squint, and averted gaze, direct gaze might play a crucial role in attractiveness and trustworthiness evaluation since it could influence people’s attention and indicate social interest [33,52].
Similar to eye region, mouth movement also works as an effective predictor to communicate honesty and trustworthiness since it is strongly associated with positive or negative emotion expression, such as smiles [37,51,57,64]. Generally speaking, smiling is often associated with a U-shape mouth with raised lip corner and raised an eyebrow, indicating the related positive emotion expression, such as happiness. On the other hand, an inverted U-shaped mouth with the lower lip and lower eyebrow is often associated with sadness and anger [37,47,51,57]. Indeed, emotion and perceived trustworthiness interact with each other: while the happy face is considered more trustworthy, the trustworthy face is also believed to be happier [48,60,70]. Since the judgment of trustworthiness is often associated with happiness [33], mouth movement then seems to be a salient signal of social perception [37]. Although the smile is universally recognized as an indication of positive emotional experience, people could spontaneously notice different types of smiling, such as enjoyment/authentic smiles and non-enjoyment smiles, since different smiles might be associated with specific social meanings. Compared with non-enjoyment/fake smiles, people have a strong intention to trust and cooperate with people with enjoyment smiles [51,55].
Negative emotions, such as anger, disgust, or fear, might have a more nuanced effect on communicating credibility. They might be a context-related signal to communicate trustworthiness [48]. For example, the fear expression is characterized by raised inner and outer eyebrows, widened eyes, an outward pull of the lip corners, and dropped the jaw. When we evaluated them in the given context, for example, in the context of announcing alert message, Reed and DeScioli [84] have shown that people had a higher intention to believe this message with a fear expression, rather than a neutral expression, suggesting the negative expression could also add credibility in some cases, as positive emotion expression does. Another example could be found in evaluating criminal appearance ratings, Flowe [45] indicated angry expression would be perceived as less trustworthy and more dominant and even more criminal. However, when we evaluated negative emotions in a context-free scenario, fear expression might not significantly influence trustworthiness evaluation though angry or disgust expression still effectively contributed to untrustworthy perceptions [48].
Further, Engell and his colleagues [55] explored the scenario of how people evaluate the trustworthiness of neutral representations after initially adapting to a happy or angry face. Results showed initial adaptation to happy (or angry) expression would increase (or decrease) the perceived trustworthiness of neutral face in the later stage while fearful expression did not have such effect, suggesting a generalization effect that a common neutral system might be engaged when evaluating facial trustworthiness in angry or happy expressions.
In addition to eye and mouth movement, other movements or responses could also lead to trustworthiness evaluation. For instance, facial blushing along with a head slightly downward movement usually indicates people concern about other opinions, feel sorry about their misdeed, and apologize in this non-verbal way [39]. A similar observation might also be seen in head nodding, which substantiates the reward power of facial cues in social interactions [70]. Accordingly, people with such embarrassment responses would like to be evaluated more positively and considered as more trustworthy.
Consequently, a social robot might be perceived as more trustworthy if accompanied by the following dynamic and emotional features: to have a babyface, to have a symmetrical and feminine feature, direct gaze design, keep enjoyment smiles, or head nodding for the positive emotion.

4.5. Trustworthiness Evaluation in Human and Non-Human Perception

When making an evaluation of trustworthiness in humans and non-humans, people tended to rely on similar facial cues, such as eye shape, to make social perceptions [22]. However, when it comes to social robot design, selecting the appropriate set of facial characteristics from the previous human and non-human literature may not be simple. Indeed, there are several conflicts or inconsistencies in communicating trustworthiness worth noting and further examination.
Previous research on the effect of fWHR on human trustworthiness has suggested humans with large fWHR (vs. small fWHR) are considered as more untrustworthy and unattractive [29,44]. However, Maeng and Aggarwal [25] have suggested that products with large fWHR (vs. small fWHR) are actually liked more, rather than less. The reason why people generally dislike dominant-looking human faces but like dominant-looking product appearance lies in that people could feel more arousal when faced with large fWHR products, thus enhancing and signaling their own dominant social status. On one hand, concerning the high association relationship between attractiveness and trustworthiness [57], it is reasonable to predict that large fWHR (vs. small fWHR) social robot might be generally regarded as more attractive and trustworthy. On the other hand, an appropriate match between a robot’s social cues and its task will improve people’s acceptance of and cooperation with the robot [85], it suggests that fWHR of social robots might depend on the roles of assigned tasks. For example, social robot such as an expert or a doctor that user would consider in a consultant role might be regarded as more professional if designed with large fWHR faces, whereas social robot such as a housekeeper that users wish to control over (e.g., like a servant) might be perceived as more trustworthy if endowed with small fWHR faces.
As stated in Section 4.4, typical looking people would be rated higher in trustworthiness because the typical face is extracted and averaged from faces previously seen (as more familiar) and serves as a standard against which all faces are evaluated in a given group or cultural affiliation [65]. However, when it comes to a social robot, it does not have a “typical” robot face since it is just an artificial machine without any heritable families. Although a social robot does not have a “typical” face, it would be interesting to explore whether a social robot face, adapted based on the principle of a typical human face of a given group, would also be treated as trustworthy accordingly.
Another point worth mentioning is the Uncanny Valley effect [13,21,86]. It refers to the relationship between trustworthiness and likeness in a robot that does not follow a simple linear positive pattern: it might decrease when the artificial agent gets increasingly realistic but still have imperfect characteristics [86]. People would positively evaluate and interact with a robot when the robot looks like a human until a level beyond which people would suddenly show strong revulsion to the robot. As the appearance of a robot is increasingly made more human-like, people would gradually positively evaluate the robot again. Based on evaluating trustworthiness in 80 traditional humanoid robots, Mathur and Reichling [13] confirmed the existence of UV effect. However, different from traditional humanoid robots, social robots are designed with a screen to represent a ’face’ to dynamically communicate with users [87]. Regarding this, the user is actually interacting with an animated face, which is different from real facial features designed in humanoid robots [21]. The human facial resemblance degree (from animated face to artificial face to real face) in social robots might be the crucial point in addressing the nuanced effect on facial trustworthiness: the majority of people might find animated face trustworthy [12,21] while others tend to trust real human face (vs. artificial faces) [34]. Thus, it is theoretically and practically interesting to explore whether the UV effect still occurs within the domain of social robots.

5. Conclusions Remarks

Based on the systematic review on facial features from the human face, product appearance, and robot face, this paper evaluates and summarizes static facial features, dynamic features, their combinations, and related emotional expressions, shedding light on further exploration of facial trustworthiness for social robot design.
Concerning the results of the current systematic review, there are still some points that need to be acknowledged. To begin with, although we have discussed the potentially optimal features of facial trustworthiness in Section 4, we might still face an issue on how to integrate different features to create a harmonious face. Considering the abstract characteristics of an animated face in social robot [88], simply optimizing single features and combining all of them together does not necessarily make the whole face most trustworthy: creating a balanced trustworthy face is not an easy job and we might still take a risk of getting an uncanny “Frankenstein-like” face [86,89]. In order to address this problem, significant facial features, regions, and facial balance should be emphasized and given the priority for social robot design. Indeed, previous studies have suggested facial trustworthiness communication mainly depends on the interaction among static features, dynamic expressions, and general appearance characteristics [41]. As for static features, eye and face shape might be the most promising area because eye is a salient facial feature for catching people’s attention [52,57,59,64,67,71,77] while face shape (e.g., fWHR and forehead) is the most prominent human secondary sexual characteristics [25] and also the most obvious feature when evaluating a face [90]. Regarding dynamic facial features, the mouth region is the most pronounced feature for emotional expressions (happiness, anger, or sadness) due to its spontaneous muscle activity around mouth and lips [91]. With respect to the general appearance, babyface, which enjoyed the advantage of evolution and is characterized by the impression of extreme youth and innocence, might act as a significant factor communicating facial trustworthiness [31]. Another appearance concern that needs noticing is to avoid the negative influence of uncanny valley [13]. As Jentsch [86] indicated, “It is an old experience that the traditional, the usual, and the hereditary is dear and familiar to most people and that they incorporate the new and the unusual with mistrust, unease, and even hostility (misoneism)”. Under the existing level of technology, it might be not easy to create a highly realistic (three-dimensional or embodied) human-like robot [92]. Thus, it might be a smart and safe choice to create an animated (or photorealistic) face with certain human facial resemblance before the extent of resemblance could elicit unexpected negative reactions. Though it might be still difficult to determine the exact extent of facial resemblance, we have tried to take those factors into considerations to give a relative promising robot model. Figure 3 shows robot models (an animated face and a realistic face) with trustworthy-looking features.
Since research on the facial design of the social robot is a multi-disciplinary field that is rarely systematically analyzed and sporadically studied by various fields, the current study has tried to systematically summarize potential features that could improve facial anthropomorphic trustworthiness for social robot. Future studies on human-robot trustworthiness might have the following research directions: theoretical exploration and empirical validation. As for theoretical exploration, a promising future study could try to explicitly discuss the theoretical foundation and evolution of trust in HRI since the theoretical works could also ground our comprehension of facial trust in HRI, which might not be the focus of the current study. In addition, the current study did not provide a robot model with dynamic trustworthy features. Concerning empirical validation, another promising stream of future studies could empirically examine the effect of different facial features on perceived trustworthiness from four main fields and compare their difference with human facial trust studies: (1) different shapes of eye and mouth in robotic face are essential internal features. Hence, they should be further validated the conclusions in human facial features; (2) WHR works as a salient trustworthy signal in human face. Thus, it would be theoretically interesting to compare the results between trust perceptions towards human and robot; (3) baby schema enjoys evolutionary advantages in human facial perception. Therefore, it would also be theoretically intriguing to verify whether it works in HRI; (4) future studies might also try to explore the effect of emotional expressions on trustworthiness and their interaction with different daily contexts, such as valence and arousal (also known as “urgency”) [93]. It would be both theoretically and practically significant to explore the interaction of emotional expressions and different daily contexts in influencing trustworthiness. Last, in order to have a more comprehensive illustration, a future study was planned to build a multi-media website to systematically illustrate trustworthy-looking robot models with static and dynamic features.
To sum up, since limited research has systematically provided specific guidance to help social robot designers and engineers improving trustworthiness in the robot’s face, future studies could try to obtain a holistic picture of trust in a social robot through a series of experiments, contributing to literature on HRI.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, Y.S.; methodology, Y.S. and Y.L.; writing, Y.S. and Y.L.; supervision, Y.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the UGC Funding Scheme (RL6B) from The Hong Kong Polytechnic University.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Gómez-del-Río, N.; González-González, C.S.; Toledo-Delgado, P.A.; Muñoz-Cruz, V.; García-Peñalvo, F. Health promotion for childhood obesity: An approach based on self-tracking of data. Sensors 2020, 20, 3778. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. González-González, C.S.; Guzmán-Franco, M.D.; Infante-Moro, A. Tangible technologies for childhood education: A systematic review. Sustainability 2019, 11, 2910. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Torres-Carrión, P.V.; González-González, C.S.; Toledo-Delgado, P.A.; Muñoz-Cruz, V.; Gil-Iranzo, R.; Reyes-Alonso, N.; Hernández-Morales, S. Improving cognitive visual-motor abilities in individuals with down syndrome. Sensors 2019, 19, 3984. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. González-González, C.S.; Infante-Moro, A.; Infante-Moro, J.C. Implementation of e-proctoring in online teaching: A study about motivational factors. Sustainability 2020, 12, 3488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Fasola, J.; Mataric, M.J. Evaluation of a spatial language interpretation framework for natural human-robot interaction with older adults. In Proceedings of the 24th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication (RO-MAN 2015), Kobe, Japan, 31 August–4 September 2015; Volume 2015-November, pp. 301–308. [Google Scholar]
  6. González-González, C.S.; Herrera-González, E.; Moreno-Ruiz, L.; Reyes-Alonso, N.; Hernández-Morales, S.; Guzmán-Franco, M.D.; Infante-Moro, A. Computational thinking and down syndrome: An exploratory study using the KIBO robot. Informatics 2019, 6, 25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Fernández-Rodicio, E.; Castro-González, Á.; Alonso-Martín, F.; Maroto-Gómez, M.; Salichs, M.Á. Modelling Multimodal Dialogues for Social Robots Using Communicative Acts. Sensors 2020, 20, 3440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Westlund, J.K.; Lee, J.J.; Plummer, L.; Faridi, F.; Gray, J.; Berlin, M.; Quintus-Bosz, H.; Hartmann, R.; Hess, M.; Dyer, S.; et al. Tega: A social robot. In Proceedings of the 11th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI), Christchurch, New Zealand, 7–10 March 2016; IEEE: Piscataway, NJ, USA, 2016; p. 561. [Google Scholar]
  9. Oh, Y.H.; Ju, D.Y. Age-Related Differences in Fixation Pattern on a Companion Robot. Sensors 2020, 20, 3807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. McGinn, C. Why Do Robots Need a Head? The Role of Social Interfaces on Service Robots. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 2019, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Stroessner, S.J.; Benitez, J. The Social Perception of Humanoid and Non-Humanoid Robots: Effects of Gendered and Machinelike Features. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 2019, 11, 305–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Prakash, A.; Rogers, W.A. Why Some Humanoid Faces Are Perceived More Positively Than Others: Effects of Human-Likeness and Task. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 2015, 7, 309–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Mathur, M.B.; Reichling, D.B. Navigating a social world with robot partners: A quantitative cartography of the Uncanny Valley. Cognition 2016, 146, 22–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Salem, M.; Lakatos, G.; Amirabdollahian, F.; Dautenhahn, K. Would You Trust a (Faulty) Robot?: Effects of Error, Task Type and Personality on Human-Robot Cooperation and Trust. In Proceedings of the 10th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction, Portland, OR, USA, 2–5 March 2015; Volume 2015-March, pp. 141–148. [Google Scholar]
  15. Song, Y.; Luximon, Y.; Luo, J. A moderated mediation analysis of the effect of lettering case and color temperature on trustworthiness perceptions and investment decisions. Int. J. Bank Mark. 2020, 38, 987–1005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Yu, P.L.; Balaji, M.S.; Khong, K.W. Building trust in internet banking: A trustworthiness perspective. Ind. Manag. Data Syst. 2015, 115, 235–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Gompei, T.; Umemuro, H. Factors and Development of cognitive and affective trust on social robots. In Proceedings of the Lecture Notes in Computer Science (including Subseries Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence and Lecture Notes in Bioinformatics); Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2018; Volume 11357 LNAI, pp. 45–54. [Google Scholar]
  18. Hancock, P.A.; Billings, D.R.; Schaefer, K.E.; Chen, J.Y.C.; De Visser, E.J.; Parasuraman, R. A meta-analysis of factors affecting trust in human-robot interaction. Hum. Factors 2011, 53, 517–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Saunderson, S.; Nejat, G. How Robots Influence Humans: A Survey of Nonverbal Communication in Social Human–Robot Interaction. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 2019, 14, 575–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Sproull, L.; Subramani, M.; Kiesler, S.; Walker, J.; Waters, K. When the Interface Is a Face. Hum. Comput. Interact. 1996, 11, 97–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Luo, J.T.; McGoldrick, P.; Beatty, S.; Keeling, K.A. On-screen characters: Their design and influence on consumer trust. J. Serv. Mark. 2006, 20, 112–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Landwehr, J.R.; McGill, A.L.; Herrmann, A. It’s Got the Look: The Effect of Friendly and Aggressive “Facial” Expressions on Product Liking and Sales. J. Mark. 2011, 75, 132–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Willis, J.; Todorov, A. First Impressions. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 17, 592–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Simion, F.; Giorgio, E. Di Face perception and processing in early infancy: Inborn predispositions and developmental changes. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Maeng, A.; Aggarwal, P. Facing dominance: Anthropomorphism and the effect of product face ratio on consumer preference. J. Consum. Res. 2018, 44, 1104–1122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Ghazali, A.S.; Ham, J.; Barakova, E.I.; Markopoulos, P. Effects of Robot Facial Characteristics and Gender in Persuasive Human-Robot Interaction. Front. Robot. AI 2018, 5, 73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Santos, S.; Almeida, I.; Oliveiros, B.; Castelo-Branco, M. The role of the amygdala in facial trustworthiness processing: A systematic review and meta-analyses of fMRI studies. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0167276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  28. Song, Y. Building a ‘Deeper’ Trust: Mapping the Facial Anthropomorphic Trustworthiness in Social Robot Design through Multidisciplinary Approaches. Des. J. 2020, 23, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Stirrat, M.; Perrett, D.I. Valid Facial Cues to Cooperation and Trust. Psychol. Sci. 2010, 21, 349–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Creusen, M.E.H.; Schoormans, J.P.L. The Different Roles of Product Appearance in Consumer Choice. J. Prod. Innov. Manag. 2005, 22, 63–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Miesler, L.; Leder, H.; Herrmann, A. Isn’t it cute: An evolutionary perspective of baby-schema effects in visual product designs. Int. J. Des. 2011, 5, 17–30. [Google Scholar]
  32. Song, Y.; Luximon, Y. Design for sustainability: The effect of lettering case on environmental concern from a green advertising perspective. Sustainability 2019, 11, 1333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Arminjon, M.; Chamseddine, A.; Kopta, V.; Paunović, A.; Mohr, C. Are we modular lying cues detectors? The answer is “yes, sometimes.”. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0136418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Balas, B.; Pacella, J. Trustworthiness perception is disrupted in artificial faces. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2017, 77, 240–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Birkás, B.; Dzhelyova, M.; Lábadi, B.; Bereczkei, T.; Perrett, D.I. Cross-cultural perception of trustworthiness: The effect of ethnicity features on evaluation of faces’ observed trustworthiness across four samples. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2014, 69, 56–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Brownlow, S. Seeing is believing: Facial appearance, credibility, and attitude change. J. Nonverbal. Behav. 1992, 16, 101–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Calvo, M.G.; Álvarez-Plaza, P.; Fernández-Martín, A. The contribution of facial regions to judgements of happiness and trustworthiness from dynamic expressions. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2017, 29, 618–625. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Cowell, A.J.; Stanney, K.M. Manipulation of non-verbal interaction style and demographic embodiment to increase anthropomorphic computer character credibility. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 2005, 62, 281–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Dijk, C.; Koenig, B.; Ketelaar, T.; de Jong, P.J. Saved by the blush: Being trusted despite defecting. Emotion 2011, 11, 313–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Dzhelyova, M.; Perrett, D.I.; Jentzsch, I. Temporal dynamics of trustworthiness perception. Brain Res. 2012, 1435, 81–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Engell, A.D.; Todorov, A.; Haxby, J.V. Common neural mechanisms for the evaluation of facial trustworthiness and emotional expressions as revealed by behavioral adaptation. Perception 2010, 39, 931–941. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Etcoff, N.L.; Stock, S.; Haley, L.E.; Vickery, S.A.; House, D.M. Cosmetics as a feature of the extended human phenotype: Modulation of the perception of biologically important facial signals. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e25656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Farmer, H.; McKay, R.; Tsakiris, M. Trust in me: Trustworthy others are seen as more physically similar to the self. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 25, 290–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Ferstl, Y.; Kokkinara, E.; Mcdonnell, R. Facial Features of Non-player Creatures Can Influence Moral Decisions in Video Games. ACM Trans. Appl. Percept. 2017, 15, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Flowe, H.D. Do Characteristics of Faces That Convey Trustworthiness and Dominance Underlie Perceptions of Criminality? PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e37253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Funk, F.; Todorov, A. Criminal stereotypes in the courtroom: Facial tattoos affect guilt and punishment differently. Psychol. Public Policy Law 2013, 19, 466–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Gill, D.; Garrod, O.G.B.; Jack, R.E.; Schyns, P.G. Facial Movements Strategically Camouflage Involuntary Social Signals of Face Morphology. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 25, 1079–1086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Gutiérrez-García, A.; Calvo, M.G. Social anxiety and trustworthiness judgments of dynamic facial expressions of emotion. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 2016, 52, 119–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Hellström, Å.; Tekle, J. Person perception through facial photographs: Effects of glasses, hair, and beard on judgments of occupation and personal qualities. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 1994, 24, 693–705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Bonnefon, J.F.; Hopfensitz, A.; Neys, W. De The modular nature of trustworthiness detection. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2013, 142, 143–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  51. Johnston, L.; Miles, L.; Macrae, C.N. Why are you smiling at me? Social functions of enjoyment and non-enjoyment smiles. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2010, 49, 107–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Kaisler, R.E.; Leder, H. Trusting the Looks of Others: Gaze Effects of Faces in Social Settings. Perception 2016, 45, 875–892. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Kleisner, K.; Priplatova, L.; Frost, P.; Flegr, J. Trustworthy-Looking Face Meets Brown Eyes. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e53285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Kocsor, F.; Bereczkei, T. First Impressions of Strangers Rely on Generalization of Behavioral Traits Associated with previously Seen Facial Features. Curr. Psychol. 2017, 36, 385–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Krumhuber, E.; Manstead, A.S.R.; Cosker, D.; Marshall, D.; Rosin, P.L.; Kappas, A. Facial Dynamics as Indicators of Trustworthiness and Cooperative Behavior. Emotion 2007, 7, 730–735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Linke, L.; Saribay, S.A.; Kleisner, K. Perceived trustworthiness is associated with position in a corporate hierarchy. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2016, 99, 22–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Ma, F.; Xu, F.; Luo, X. Children’s and Adults’ Judgments of Facial Trustworthiness: The Relationship to Facial Attractiveness. Percept. Mot. Skills 2015, 121, 179–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Maoz, I. The Face of the Enemy: The Effect of Press-Reported Visual Information Regarding the Facial Features of Opponent Politicians on Support for Peace. Polit. Commun. 2012, 29, 243–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Masip, J.; Garrido, E.; Herrero, C. Facial appearance and impressions of ‘credibility’: The effects of facial babyishness and age on person perception. Int. J. Psychol. 2004, 39, 276–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Oosterhof, N.N.; Todorov, A. Shared Perceptual Basis of Emotional Expressions and Trustworthiness Impressions From Faces. Emotion 2009, 9, 128–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Okubo, M.; Ishikawa, K.; Kobayashi, A. No trust on the left side: Hemifacial asymmetries for trustworthiness and emotional expressions. Brain Cogn. 2013, 82, 181–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Reed, L.I.; DeScioli, P. Watch out! How a fearful face adds credibility to warnings of danger. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2017, 38, 490–495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Stanley, D.A.; Sokol-Hessner, P.; Banaji, M.R.; Phelps, E.A. Implicit race attitudes predict trustworthiness judgments and economic trust decisions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2011, 108, 7710–7715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Santos, I.M.; Young, A.W. Inferring social attributes from different face regions: Evidence for holistic processing. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2011, 64, 751–766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Sofer, C.; Dotsch, R.; Wigboldus, D.H.J.; Todorov, A. What Is Typical Is Good. Psychol. Sci. 2015, 26, 39–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Stanton, C.J.; Stevens, C.J. Don’t Stare at Me: The Impact of a Humanoid Robot’s Gaze upon Trust During a Cooperative Human–Robot Visual Task. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 2017, 9, 745–753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Todorov, A.; Baron, S.G.; Oosterhof, N.N. Evaluating face trustworthiness: A model based approach. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2008, 3, 119–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Verberne, F.M.F.; Ham, J.; Midden, C.J.H. Trusting a Virtual Driver That Looks, Acts, and Thinks Like You. Hum. Factors 2015, 57, 895–909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Willis, M.S.; Esqueda, C.W.; Schacht, R.N. Social Perceptions of Individuals Missing Upper Front Teeth. Percept. Mot. Skills 2008, 106, 423–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Woodall, W.G.; Burgoon, J.K.; Markel, N.N. The effects of facial-head cue combinations on interpersonal evaluations. Commun. Q. 1980, 28, 47–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Xu, F.; Wu, D.; Toriyama, R.; Ma, F.; Itakura, S.; Lee, K. Similarities and differences in chinese and caucasian adults’ use of facial cues for trustworthiness judgments. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e34859. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  72. Zebrowitz, L.A.; Voinescu, L.; Collins, M.A. “Wide-eyed” and “crooked-faced”: Determinants of perceived and real honesty across the life span. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1996, 22, 1258–1269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Windhager, S.; Hutzler, F.; Carbon, C.-C.; Oberzaucher, E.; Schaefer, K.; Thorstensen, T.; Leder, H.; Grammer, K. Laying eyes on headlights: Eye movements suggest facial features in cars. Coll. Antropol. 2010, 34, 1075–1080. [Google Scholar]
  74. Cyr, D.; Head, M.; Larios, H. Colour appeal in website design within and across cultures: A multi-method evaluation. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 2010, 68, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Ichikawa, H.; Kanazawa, S.; Yamaguchi, M.K. Finding a face in a face-like object. Perception 2011, 40, 500–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Haselhuhn, M.P.; Wong, E.M.; Ormiston, M.E. Self-Fulfilling Prophecies as a Link between Men’s Facial Width-to-Height Ratio and Behavior. PLoS ONE 2013, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Kompatsiari, K.; Ciardo, F.; Tikhanoff, V.; Metta, G.; Wykowska, A. It’s in the Eyes: The Engaging Role of Eye Contact in HRI. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 2019, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Ehrenberg, A.S.C.; Lindquist, E.F. Design and Analysis of Experiments in Psychology and Education. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. A 2006, 117, 105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Roberts, S.C.; Little, A.C.; De Bruine, L.M.; Petrie, M. Discrimination of Attractiveness and Health in Men’s Faces: The Impact of Color Cues and Variation in Relation to Sex and Age of Rater. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 2017, 3, 401–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  80. Fink, B.; Grammer, K.; Thornhill, R. Human (Homo sapiens) facial attractiveness in relation to skin texture and color. J. Comp. Psychol. 2001, 115, 92–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  81. Bakmazian, A. The Man Behind the Beard: Perception of Men’s Trustworthiness as a Function of Facial Hair. Psychology 2014, 5, 185–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  82. Todorov, A.; Olivola, C.Y.; Dotsch, R.; Mende-Siedlecki, P. Social Attributions from Faces: Determinants, Consequences, Accuracy, and Functional Significance. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2015, 66, 519–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  83. Buckingham, G.; DeBruine, L.M.; Little, A.C.; Welling, L.L.M.; Conway, C.A.; Tiddeman, B.P.; Jones, B.C. Visual adaptation to masculine and feminine faces influences generalized preferences and perceptions of trustworthiness. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2006, 27, 381–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  84. Desor, J.A.; Beauchamp, G.K. The human capacity to transmit olfactory information. Percept. Psychophys. 1974, 16, 551–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  85. Goetz, J.; Kiesler, S.; Powers, A. Matching robot appearance and behavior to tasks to improve human-robot cooperation. In Proceedings of the 12th IEEE International Workshop on Robot and Human Interactive Communication, RO-MAN 2003, Millbrae, CA, USA, 2 November 2003; IEEE: Piscataway, NJ, USA, 2003; Volume 2003, pp. 55–60. [Google Scholar]
  86. Jentsch, E. On the psychology of the uncanny (1906). Angelaki J. Theor. Humanit. 1997, 2, 7–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Breazeal, C.; Dautenhahn, K.; Kanda, T. Social Robotics. In Springer Handbook of Robotics; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2016; pp. 1935–1972. [Google Scholar]
  88. Dehn, D.M.; Van Mulken, S. Impact of animated interface agents: A review of empirical research. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 2000, 52, 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Spotts, M.F.; Shoup, T.E.; Hornberger, L.E.; Kazmer, D.O. Design of Machine Elements, Eighth Edition. J. Mech. Des. 2004, 126, 201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Kramer, R.S.S. Facial width-to-height ratio in a large sample of commonwealth games athletes. Evol. Psychol. 2015, 13, 197–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  91. Menne, I.M.; Schwab, F. Faces of Emotion: Investigating Emotional Facial Expressions Towards a Robot. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 2018, 10, 199–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Mathur, M.B.; Reichling, D.B.; Lunardini, F.; Geminiani, A.; Antonietti, A.; Ruijten, P.A.M.; Levitan, C.A.; Nave, G.; Manfredi, D.; Bessette-Symons, B.; et al. Uncanny but not confusing: Multisite study of perceptual category confusion in the Uncanny Valley. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2020, 103, 21–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Zhu, M.; Yang, Y.; Hsee, C.K. The Mere Urgency Effect. J. Consum. Res. 2018, 45, 673–690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Social robotic product—Buddy Robot®.
Figure 1. Social robotic product—Buddy Robot®.
Sensors 20 05087 g001
Figure 2. Flow chart of the systematic review process.
Figure 2. Flow chart of the systematic review process.
Sensors 20 05087 g002
Figure 3. Trustworthy-looking robot models. (a) an animated face (Buddy Robot®) and (b) a realistic face (Sophia Robot®).
Figure 3. Trustworthy-looking robot models. (a) an animated face (Buddy Robot®) and (b) a realistic face (Sophia Robot®).
Sensors 20 05087 g003
Table 1. Summarized facial features on trustworthiness.
Table 1. Summarized facial features on trustworthiness.
DatabaseSearch TermsHits
Scopus (1960–2019)Facial trustworthiness contained “(face OR facial) AND (trust OR trustworthiness* OR credibility OR trust traits* OR trust features* OR trust signs*)”
Product or robot trustworthiness contained “(product OR robot OR anthropomorphism) AND (face OR facial) Trust”.
849
PsycInfo (1967–2019)1214
Web of Science (1955–2019)657
Table 2. Summarized facial features on trustworthiness.
Table 2. Summarized facial features on trustworthiness.
AuthorsSampleCountryApplication/ Purpose of studyMeasureProcessing TechniqueResults
Arminjon et al. (2015) [33]57 To test the effect of lying cues (LC) in guessing behavior.Yes or no proportionRepeated measures ANOVACompared with NLC, LC was significant to lying decisions and is related to the automatic processing of lying detection.
Balas and Pacellaa (2017) [34]51USTo test the difference of trustworthiness perception between the artificial face and real face1-7 Likert scaleT-testComputer-generated faces were considered to be less trustworthy than real human faces
Birkás et al., (2014) [35]266US, Hungarian, East, and South AsiaTo examine the effect of facial ethnicity on trustworthiness evaluation.1-7 Likert scaleTwo-way ANOVADifferent ethical groups showed similar trustworthiness evaluation. However, Hungarians tended to be biased toward their own ethnicity for medium/low trustworthy faces.
Brownlow (1992) [36]128USTo evaluate the difference in trustworthiness perception in baby-faced (vs. mature-faced) people1-9 Likert scaleThree-way ANOVABaby-faced (vs. mature-faced) speakers enjoyed more positive trustworthiness evaluation and might induce more agreement when their trustworthiness was questioned.
Calvo et al. (2017)
[37]
64SpanishTo explore the effect of the combination of different mouth and eye on trustworthiness evaluation.1-9 Likert scale;
iNVT
Repeated measured ANOVAFaces with an unfolding smile and eye looked more trustworthy. The contribution of the mouth was greater for happiness than for trustworthiness.
Cowell and Stanney (2005) [38]45USTo investigate the significance of face region in influencing the trustworthiness of anthropomorphic computer characters1-7 Likert scaleA Kruskal–Wallis testFace region plays a significant role in communicating trustworthiness, compared with the body region. Users tended to trust young-looking and ethnicity-consistent characters.
Dijk et al. (2011)
[39]
196DutchTo explore the effect of blushing on trustworthiness.Trust game choice;
1-7 Likert scale
Two-way ANOVAThe blushing people were perceived to be more trustworthy.
Dzhelyova et al., (2012) [40]32 To test the relationship between trustworthiness and sex of faceAccuracy RateMixed ANOVAA female face would be perceived to be more trustworthy than a male face.
Engell et al., (2010) [41]49USTo investigate whether the previously perceptual similarities between trust and emotions (fear/happy) could extend to neutral representations.1-9 Likert scaleThree-way ANOVAAdapting to happy/angry faces could increase/decrease in the subsequent evaluation of trustworthiness in a neutral face.
Etcoff et al. (2011)
[42]
149 To evaluate the effect of color cosmetics on trustworthiness.1-7 Likert scaleA linear mixed-effects modelCosmetics can exaggerate cues to sexual dimorphism, improving trustworthiness.
Farmer et al., (2013) [43]59 To examine whether facial similarity could influence the judgments of trustworthiness and cooperative behavior.Percentage of others’ face in the point of subjective equalityRepeated-measures ANOVAFacial similarity has shown to have an effect on improving trustworthiness evaluation.
Ferstl et al. (2017)
[44]
48 To explore the effect of facial features on the perceived personality and moral decisions.1-7 Likert scaleA generalized linear mixed modelHuman faces trustworthy traits might not be consistent with abstract faces.
Flowe (2012) [45]512UKTo investigate the relationship among perceived criminality, trustworthiness, facial mature, and emotional expression.1-7 Likert scaleTwo-way ANOVAAngry faces are deemed as less trustworthy and more dominant.
Funk and Todorov (2013) [46]286USTo examine the effect of facial tattoos on perceived trustworthiness1-7 Likert scaleThree-way ANOVAFacial tattoos might lead to a lower level of trustworthiness evaluation.
Gill et al. (2014)
[47]
12 To test the effect of phenotypic morphology on the default social traits.1-5 Likert scaleCorrelation AnalysisThe facial movement could predictably modulate the perception of basic social traits in face morphology.
Gutiérrez-García and Calvo (2016) [48]48SpainTo investigate the relationship between trustworthiness and emotional facial expression1-9 Likert scaleThree-way ANOVATrustworthiness is positively associated with the intensity of happy expression while negatively correlated with the intensity of angry and disgust face.
Hellström and Tekle (1994) [49]75SwedishTo evaluate the effects of different facial attributes (glasses, beard, and hair) on characteristic profiles.1-6 Likert scaleThree-way ANOVAThe judges associated wearing glasses with intellectualism and goodness, being bald with idealism, and wearing a beard with unconventionality and goodness.
Jean François et al., (2013) [50]180BeguilerTo test whether hairstyle could influence trust detection.Trust game;
Money transfer rate
Three-way ANOVAThe hairstyle could influence people’s detection of trust.
Johnston et al. (2010) [51]30New ZealanderTo investigate the effect of different types of smiling on attention.1-7 Likert scaleRepeated-measures ANOVAEnjoyment smiles are positively evaluated and are considered to have higher rates of cooperation.
Landwehr et al. (2011) [22]263 To investigate the effect of product facial design on people’s liking.1000 points scaleRepeated-measures ANOVAPerception of friendliness is associated with the product with an upturned mouth, while aggressiveness is associated with the product with both an upturned mouth and slanted eyes.
Kaisler and Leder (2016) [52]70AustrianTo explore how eye contacting affects social and aesthetic evaluations.1-7 Likert scaleRepeated-measures ANOVADirect-looking faces are considered to be more trustworthy.
Kleisner et al., (2013)
[53]
238Czech Republic To test whether eye color influences the perception of trustworthiness.1-10 Likert scaleA generalized linear mixed modelBrown-eyed faces were perceived as more trustworthy and the reason lies in the facial features associated with it.
Kocsor and Bereczkei (2016) [54]116 To explore whether facial traits could have an impact on a composite face with such traits. 1-9 Likert scaleChi-square testComposite faces with high social desirability tended to be considered more trustworthy.
Krumhuber et al., (2007) [55]90UKTo examine whether facial dynamics could influence perceived trustworthiness and cooperative behavior0-6 Likert scaleMANOVAAuthentic smiles enjoyed the highest level of perceived trustworthiness, followed by a fake smile and a neutral face.
Linke et al., (2016) [56]187 To explore the relationship between facial geometric morphometrics and facial trustworthiness1-7 Likert scaleMultivariate regressionsA trustworthy face might have lower fWHR, narrow lips, longer nose, larger eyes, and shorter eyebrows.
Luo et al. (2006)
[21]
183 To investigate whether or not the on-screen characters representation influence trustworthiness perception.1-7 Likert scaleOne-way ANOVA and Paired t-testsOn-screen characters (OSCs) are considered to be more trustworthy in general. There is a mismatch between the expectations and capabilities of OSCs.
Ma et al. (2015)
[57]
139ChineseTo explore how children judge trustworthiness from faces1-3 Likert scaleStepwise linear regressions8-years children could use a similar inference to evaluate trustworthiness. Different age groups could use different facial features to make an evaluation.
Maeng and Aggarwal (2018) [25]248 To explore the face width-to-height ratio (fWHR) can signal dominance and affect its overall evaluation1-7 Likert scaleA linear mixed-effects analysis using lme4 and lmerTestHigh fWHR product is considered to be more dominant and liked more.
Maoz (2012) [58]88IsraeliTo test the effect of babyface (vs. mature face) on politician trustworthiness evaluation1-7 Likert scaleTwo-way ANOVAA baby-faced politician is believed to be more trustworthy (vs. mature face).
Masip et al. (2004)
[59]
324SpanishTo examine the impact of facial maturity on impressions of truthfulness.1-7 Likert scaleMANCOVABaby-face and age are perceived to be a significantly static cue to make trustworthiness evaluation.
Mathur and Reichling (2016) [13]334 To investigate whether human-robot interactions may be complicated by Uncanny Valley (UV)Mean dollars wageredPolynomial regressionThe Uncanny Valley, in which imperfect human-likeness cues elicits dislike, could influence human perceptions of robots.
Oosterhof and Todorov (2009) [60]60USTo test the relationship between facial expression (anger and happiness) and perceptions of trustworthiness1-8 Likert scaleRepeated-measures ANOVAA trustworthy face with happy emotion was perceived happier than an untrustworthy face; an Untrustworthy face with angry emotion was perceived angrier than the trustworthy face.
Okubo et al. (2013) [61]100JapaneseTo investigate the effect of a posed smile on people’s attitudes.Response biasThree-way ANOVAThe left–left composites were perceived to be more trustworthy when posed with a happy face.
Reed and DeScioli (2017) [62]218 To test whether fear expressions add credibility to a speaker’s warnings of danger1-7 Likert scaleChi-squareWarning of danger with a fear expression is considered to be more trustworthy.
Stanley et al., (2011) [63]50USTo examine the effect of implicit ethical attitude on trustworthiness evaluation.1-9 Likert scaleStepwise regression analysesPerceived trustworthiness towards people with various ethical racial backgrounds is associated with the extent of that individual’s implicit race bias.
Santos and Young (2011) [64]Study 1: 24;
Study 2: 48
UKTo investigate the importance of holistic processing in the inference of social attributes from faces.1-7 Likert scaleRepeated-measures ANOVAExperiment 1: internal features plays a more significant role in trustworthiness inferences. Experiment 2: different facial cues are used in different evaluations.
Sofer et al. (2015)
[65]
53IsraelTo test whether face typicality is an important factor for social perception.1-9 Likert scaleRepeated-measures ANOVAFor a continuum of faces that vary on a typicality-attractiveness dimension, trustworthiness evaluations peak around the typical face.
Stanton and Stevens (2017) [66]52AustraliaTo explore the relationship between gaze and trustworthiness evaluationMean answer changeTwo-way ANOVAPeople might trust the robot more on hard trials, compared with on medium trials. In addition, females are least likely to trust a robot that stared at them.
Stirrat and Perrett (2010) [29]62UKTo explore the effect of fWHR on trustworthiness evaluationThe proportion of trust in the image.A least-squares regressionWide face in men was perceived to be less trustworthy.
Todorov et al., (2008) [67]21UKTo examine the relationship between judgments of facial trustworthiness and approach/avoidance responses and approximate the valence evaluation1-8 Likert scaleA least-squares regressionHigh inner eyebrows, pronounced cheekbones, wide chins, and shallow nose sellion looked more trustworthy
Verberne et al. (2015) [68]111DutchTo examine the effect of facial similarity on trust evaluation.1-7 Likert scaleA one-way MANOVAAs the rules in human similarity, the similarity in the virtual agent would also be considered as more trustworthy.
Willis and Esqueda (2008) [69]200USTo investigate the social consequences, such as trustworthiness evaluation, for individuals missing visible front teeth.1-7 Likert scaleA one-way MANOVAThe absence of visible front teeth could decrease trustworthiness evaluation.
Wooddall et al., (1980) [70]148USTo test the role of visual cues in interpersonal trustworthiness1-7 Likert scaleMixed ANOVASmile and head nodes are strong indicators for trustworthiness evaluation.
Xu et al. (2012)
[71]
144Chinese and CaucasianTo explore the difference in the ethnical group in trustworthiness evaluation.1-9 Likert scaleA least-squares regressionChinese and Caucasian shared similar cues to make trustworthiness evaluation.
Zebrowitz et al. (1996) [72]103USTo investigate the effect of age on trustworthiness evaluation. 1-7 Likert scaleCorrelation analysisBabyfaceness, attractiveness, facial symmetry, and large eyes had a significant impact on trustworthiness evaluation.
Note: “Authors” refers to the author(s) of the specific article; “Sample” refers to the sample size used in the article; “Country” refers to the nationality of the sample in the article; “Application/ Purpose of study” refers to the research objective of the article; “Measure” refers to the measurement strategy conducted in the specific article; “Processing Technique” refers to the analytical method used in the article; “Results” refers to the relevant conclusion in the article.
Table 3. Summarized facial features on trustworthiness.
Table 3. Summarized facial features on trustworthiness.
Static FeaturesCombinationsDynamic FeaturesEmotions
Internal FeaturesExternal Features
Eye sizefWHR Baby-face (Cuteness)Eye movementAnger
Eye colorBrow-nose-chin (ratio)Masculine/feminineMouth movementSadness
Eye shape Forehead-sellion-nose (ratio)SymmetrySmile (Authentic/Fake)Fear
Eye gazeHairLook similarOther movementsHappiness
EyebrowForeheadLook typical Disgust
NoseEars
Mouth Beard
LipsChin
TeethGlasses
CheekTattoo
Color Cue Age
Luminance ContrastEthnicity

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Song, Y.; Luximon, Y. Trust in AI Agent: A Systematic Review of Facial Anthropomorphic Trustworthiness for Social Robot Design. Sensors 2020, 20, 5087. https://doi.org/10.3390/s20185087

AMA Style

Song Y, Luximon Y. Trust in AI Agent: A Systematic Review of Facial Anthropomorphic Trustworthiness for Social Robot Design. Sensors. 2020; 20(18):5087. https://doi.org/10.3390/s20185087

Chicago/Turabian Style

Song, Yao, and Yan Luximon. 2020. "Trust in AI Agent: A Systematic Review of Facial Anthropomorphic Trustworthiness for Social Robot Design" Sensors 20, no. 18: 5087. https://doi.org/10.3390/s20185087

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop