Next Article in Journal
Detecting Overlapping Communities in Modularity Optimization by Reweighting Vertices
Next Article in Special Issue
Contextuality Analysis of Impossible Figures
Previous Article in Journal
Automatic Recognition of Human Interaction via Hybrid Descriptors and Maximum Entropy Markov Model Using Depth Sensors
Previous Article in Special Issue
A Unified Theory of Human Judgements and Decision-Making under Uncertainty
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Toward a Unified View of Cognitive and Biochemical Activity: Meditation and Linguistic Self-Reconstructing May Lead to Inflammation and Oxidative Stress Improvement

1
Department of Cardiac, Thoracic and Vascular Sciences, Padua University Medical School, Via Giustiniani 2, 35100 Padua, Italy
2
Department of Laboratory Medicine, Padua University Medical School, Via Giustiniani 2, 35100 Padua, Italy
3
Department of Physics “E.R. Caianiello”, Salerno University, Via Giovanni Paolo II, 132, 84084 Fisciano (Salerno), Italy
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Entropy 2020, 22(8), 818; https://doi.org/10.3390/e22080818
Submission received: 11 July 2020 / Revised: 23 July 2020 / Accepted: 25 July 2020 / Published: 27 July 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Quantum Models of Cognition and Decision-Making)

Abstract

:
Stress appears to be the basis of many diseases, especially myocardial infarction. Events are not objectively “stressful” but what is central is how the individual structures the experience he is facing: the thoughts he produces about an event put him under stress. This cognitive process could be revealed by language (words and structure). We followed 90 patients with ischemic heart disease and 30 healthy volunteers, after having taught them the Relaxation Response (RR) as part of a 4-day Rational–Emotional–Education intervention. We analyzed with the Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count software the words that the subjects used across the study following the progression of blood galectin-3 (inflammation marker) and malondialdehyde (oxidative stress marker). During the follow-up, we confirmed an acute and chronic decrease in the markers of inflammation and oxidative stress already highlighted in our previous studies together with a significant change in the use of language by the subjects of the RR groups. Our results and the precise design of our study would seem to suggest the existence of an intimate relationship and regulatory action by cognitive processes (recognizable by the type of language used) on some molecular processes in the human body.

1. Introduction

Stress appears to be the basis of many diseases [1]. Although a stress condition is undoubtedly linked to triggering events, the link between “stress” and “events” does not seem to be causal, nor necessary, nor sufficient. Different people, facing the same situation, respond differently on a psycho-emotional and behavioral level. But even one single person, in the course of a lifetime, can face the same situation or experience the same problem differently.
In the same way, the stress reaction is independent of the presence of pathologic psychological comorbidities [2].
Central is how the individual, consciously or not, structures the experience he is facing [3]: the thoughts he produces about that event put him under stress [4,5]. The subject reacts to the perceptual inputs through the brain’s action-perception cycle and his emotions and behaviors are generated [6]. Although this dynamical integration process is still not fully understood and under study, it appears that language plays a relevant role in it [7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19].
The individual’s stereotyped methods of response to the environment that surrounds him fall within the concept of “personality” [20,21], namely the subject is in a highly nonlinear interaction with his environment. It has been amply demonstrated that there are personalities more likely to develop psychological distress [4] and, consequently, stress-related pathologies [20,21,22].
As personality seems to be in strict relation with language [23], it is possible to identify several personality traits through the language used by a person [24]. Words are indicators of how the subject is structuring his reality, the flow of his attention, his attitudes, his limiting beliefs, etc. [25] (see below in the Discussion section). A ready-to-use tool to start to analyze these aspects is provided by appropriate software such as the Language Inquiry Word Count (LIWC) [24].
Recently, we followed for one year some patients with ischemic heart disease and some healthy volunteers, after having taught them the Relaxation Response (RR) as part of a 4-day Rational–Emotional–Education intervention and we documented a significant decrease in the perceived degree of stress combined with a clinically favorable variation of different neuro-endocrine-immune markers of inflammation, oxidative stress [26] and circulating microRNAs [27].
Given the introductory premises, we asked ourselves if the progressive well-being perceived by the subjects enrolled could be linked to a progressive restoration of their personality and their language.
To answer this question, we analyzed with the Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC) software (http://www.liwc.net) the words that the subjects used across the study, during the first educational days, in the two follow-up meetings and their emails. To confirm our observations from a biochemical point of view, we followed the progression of galectin-3 (GAL-3) as a marker of inflammation and malondialdehyde (MDA) concerning oxidative stress.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Biological Samples

We analyzed the serum samples of 120 subjects following an approved protocol (protocol number 3487/AO/15—13/7/2015) [26]. Briefly, we enrolled 90 consecutive patients after myocardial infarction and 30 healthy controls. Thirty patients were taught to meditate, 30 to appreciate music, and 30 did not carry out any intervention and served as controls. To rule out that the disease state could interfere with the relaxation effect, we enrolled 30 healthy volunteers (15 were trained to meditate and 15 had music appreciation). The practices of meditation and music appreciation can produce the so-called Relaxation Response (RR) in the same way [28]. The details of the RR techniques that we used and the description of their pathophysiological mechanism is described in our previous works [26,27].
After the initial four-days-training, after 6 and 12 months of RR practice, we collected a blood sample immediately before and after the relaxation session (according to the scheme reported in Figure 1) to describe any variation of GAL-3 and MDA, the two markers that moved with greater sensitivity in our previous work [26].
Clear variation of the physical characteristics of the serum samples (Figure 2), was observed.
According to Benson’s researches [18,29,30] and our previous study [26,27], there are no significant differences between relaxation techniques. Therefore, we merged into a single “intervention” group (called “RELAXATION RESPONSE”) all patients treated with meditation and music and into a single “intervention healthy controls” group (called “RELAXATION RESPONSE HEALTHY CONTROLS”) all healthy subjects. Finally, the patients that did not carry out any intervention constituted the “CONTROLS” group.
We emphasize that in our work we observed the RR using two conditioning techniques, meditation, and music, which have to be considered as two ways leading to the same relaxation effect [18]. Therefore, even from a strictly methodological point of view, we used a unique technique—precisely the RR—from which also the need to unite in a single “intervention group” the treated subjects.
Indeed, all subjects enrolled in the study have continued the practice at home, twice a day, as they taught. During the follow-up period, each subject reported having pleasantly performed more than 80% of the meditation or music listening sessions.

2.2. Language Evaluation

During the study, we recorded all the words spoken by the participants in the training meetings of the first 4 days and during the follow-up meetings at 6 and 12 months. We also analyzed the emails that periodically sent us during the study. In particular, each participant sent an email about 2 months and 4 months from the start of the study telling about their experience. Then, after the blood sample control at 6 months, another email at 8 and 10 months was sent before the final appointment at 12 months. All the texts have been analyzed through the LIWC software (http://www.liwc.net).
We divided the texts collected in the first 6 months of the study—initial training meetings and the two e-mails at 2 and 4 months—(the analyzed data referring to the first 6 months of the study were called “START”), from those collected in the final 6 months (6 and 12 months follow-up meetings and the two e-mails at 8 and 10 months—the analyzed data referring to the last 6 months of the study were called “END”). Then, we compared the means and standard deviations of the items provided by the software in the same subjects and among the RELAXATION RESPONSE group versus the CONTROLS and between the RELAXATION RESPONSE group and RELAXATION RESPONSE HEALTHY CONTROLS.
The items provided by the software are quite intuitive, for an explanation of them please see: https://repositories.lib.utexas.edu/bitstream/handle/2152/31333/LIWC2015_LanguageManual.pdf.

2.3. Statistical Analysis

GAL-3 and MDA levels are expressed as median and interquartile range. LIWC items are expressed as mean ± standard deviation. For biochemical markers, the comparison between the pre-post intervention changes was performed using Wilcoxon test. The comparison between groups was performed through the Mann–Whitney test. T-tests for dependent and independent samples was used to analyze language items. The distribution of the individual variables was assessed by the Shapiro–Wilk test. An initial comparison between groups was performed by means of Kruskal–Wallis test for independent samples or by Friedman test for paired data. Statistical significance was assumed if the null hypothesis could be rejected at p < 0.05. The statistical analysis was performed using software SPSS version 22.0 (Chicago, SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). All conditions that could have affected the improvement of the serum molecules have not changed and are comparable in the different groups at baseline, after 6 months and after 12 months (same physical rehabilitation and nutritional support, same therapy, same time of follow-up and same environmental conditions at the time of sampling).

Clarification on the Concept of Statistical Significance in Biology/Medicine

The P-value indicates the probability that two apparently different groups of data actually come (or not) from the same population and does not indicate whether the hypotheses under study is true or false [31].
Therefore, when the reader will observe in our graphs and tables that two distributions of data “differ in a statistically significant way, p < 0.05” it means that the analyzed data could theoretically belong to two different populations.
Statistical methods were applied in biomedical research to allow us to quantify uncertainty; they were not developed as a decision tool or as an instrument of dogmatic truth. The use of a p-value of 0.05 as a threshold for declaring statistical significance is just a simple clinical/biological convention, still under debate [32].

3. Results

3.1. Biochemical Markers

The results along with the statistical analysis of GAL-3 and MDA are reported in Figure 3 and Figure 4. The RR results in a significative decreasing of GAL-3 and MDA at every time point (p < 0.01 Wilcoxon test at every time point) while in the Controls do not vary (p > 0.05 at every timepoint). On the one hand, it is possible to notice that healthy controls have lower starting levels of both inflammation and oxidative stress markers than RR group (p < 0.001 Mann–Whitney test—basal), on the other hand, the behavior of the same markers is similar in individuals subjected to RR (p > 0.05 Mann–Whitney test at every time point).

3.2. Language Results

The results along with the statistical analysis of linguistic items are reported in Table A1 and Table A2 in the Appendix A. To make the results easier to follow, we summarized in Table 1 the trend of each item during the study in the three groups. The up arrow indicates a statistically significant increase during follow-up, the down arrow the opposite, the dash indicates no significant changes.

4. Discussion

The biological world operates on a multitude of scales from atoms to molecules, from cells to macro-organisms and ecosystems. All these levels are connected by a continuous flow of information made of electro-chemicals mediators, by physical forces and in-phase correlations and constitute a global system characterized by specific macroscopic behaviors.
Stress is the basis of many diseases, particularly cardiovascular ones [33]. It is accompanied by the perception of negative emotions such as depression, anxiety, anger, fear, or panic [34]. Each of these emotions can have dramatic consequences on the cardiovascular level [35].
But emotional expression can be effectively regulated by voluntary cognitive control processes [36].
During the 4 days of subject RR training, we taught how to try to rework their negative emotions to mitigate their biological effect. During the reworking process, we know that the activity of the prefrontal cortex increases with the reduction of the amygdala function [37,38], whose hyperactivation seems to promote inflammation and damage the heart [39].
Therefore, during the two daily sessions of 20 min of RR, brain attention network activates while the subject interrupts the flow of ordinary thoughts concentrating on the object of meditation (in this case a word sound or music) [40]; the parasympathetic tone prevails, and a series of neuroendocrine changes occur [26,27] leading to an acute attenuation of oxidative stress and inflammation (macroscopically, the physical appearance of the blood plasma also varies—Figure 2. We comment more on this point in the following).
We have to consider that Gal-3, besides playing an important regulatory role in inflammation, modulates basic cellular functions such as cell–cell and cell–matrix interactions, growth, proliferation and differentiation and it is involved in the pathogenesis of many relevant human diseases, including cardiovascular disorders and cancer [41]. The MDA molecule is able to interact with nucleic acid bases to form several different adducts able to induce sequence-dependent frameshift mutations and base-pair substitutions that may lead to cancer [42]. Nonetheless, MDA toxicity is directed also towards cardiovascular stability, modifying lipoproteins and impairing their interaction with macrophages, a key step of atherogenicity [43].
As briefly mentioned in the Introduction, language has an important role in the construction and development of some of the personality traits. Personality is much affected by the surroundings, since the subject can be described as a system permanently open to its environment. In its functional activity, the brain finds in the world the source of its energy needs and the sink where its energy waste goes. The persistent search of the harmonious balance of the outgoing and ingoing fluxes of energy and information between the subject and its world is at the core of the action-perception cycle characterizing brain activity [44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51].
Since the balance arises from the outgoing (forward in time) and ingoing (time-reversed) fluxes, with reference, e.g., to the subject, the world appears then to be the Double of the subject, his image in the mirror of time [44,45,46]. The openness of the subject to the environment finds then its realization in the permanent dialogue between the self and its Double. Such a “talking” between the two (sometimes felt as a “talking inside” since the Double is indeed the subject’s image) is therefore crucial in the continuous trade of the self with the world in the search of the best to-be-in-the-world. Any event or situation judged as “stressful” then introduces more or less severe perturbations unbalancing the energy flow between the self and the Double and it is not surprising that these unbalancing perturbations affect the words of the language through which the dialogue between the self and its Double is realized. We see that all of this is a strict consequence of the open (i.e., dissipative) character of the brain dynamics, which supports the thesis of the immanence of some of the linguistic structures to the brain physical dynamical processes (by resorting to results in Chomsky’s Minimalist Program (MP) [12,13], it has been shown [9,10,11] that specific linguistic features admit a representation in terms of the algebraic formalism used in the formulation of the dissipative quantum model of the brain [44,45,46]).
One important point which clarifies the relevance of the RR techniques in recovering from stress is that there is always a content of attention in the perception, so that among the many perceptual stimuli arriving from the world, the subject selects, focus only on the ones which he associates to a “value”, worth to expend energy on it. There is then always an intention content in the action aimed at the best to-be-in-the-world [44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51]. Out of this, the formation of meanings arises. Meanings emerge from the dynamical correlations with the world constructed during the perceptual history of the subject. Intentionality and meaningfulness are the basic ingredients of the subject identity, which manifests in its personality.
The harmonious to-be-in-the-world mentioned above, which according to Desideri constitutes the subject’s aesthetic experience [52,53], is damaged by events judged as “stressful”. The subject is then guided through the RR therapy to repair and recover a peaceful talking with its Double. It has to be observed that in the dissipative quantum model the act of consciousness is postulated to arise exactly in the dialogue with the Double [44,45,46], and thus it manifests itself in the words between the two. We also remark that there is entanglement between the self and its Double [46], so that they cannot be separated, they are always “an undivided two”, in phase resonating, so that words among them are not really messenger carrying information, they express the meaningfulness of their correlation. In this sense, we were saying in the Introduction that words are indicators of how the subject is structuring his reality, his vision of the world. From such a standpoint the use of the LIWC software in our analysis has to be regarded as the first, readily available tool to look at changes in the statistics of the elementary language components. Nevertheless, it is interesting to see that the use of the pronouns, the specificity of the emotions, the tenses of the verbs, are actually fitting with the remarks and considerations made above in our discussion. In fact, the rational-emotional education and the RR processes have allowed, over time, to restructure the personality of the subjects [54].
The patients we followed seem to be more aware of the experiences they lived, as witnessed by the change in their language of some syntactic structures. According to Yarkoni [24] and Pennebaker’s works [55], the changing of personal pronouns use that we documented, may reveal increased attention to social dynamics along with greater awareness in self boundaries; words linked to positive emotions together with cognitive verbs have also increased; verbal tenses are more declined in the present, in an active and not passive form, with an increase of verbs indicating “choice” and “will”, rather than “duty”; there has been also an increase in the use of definite articles and, in general, a lesser description of life events and a greater attention to the personal experience. Words and phrases have become shorter as if to underline more immediate experiences and greater presence of the subject in his reality. All of it pointing to the restoring of the more harmonious dialogue with the Double.
Thus, in the long term, we speculate that the combination of daily relaxation and, especially, the progressive psycho-emotional re-elaboration may have contributed to attenuate the psychological triggers of inflammation and oxidative stress unlike controls (with equal diet, physical activity, and medical therapy).
Emotions, although triggered by events, express the meaningfulness of our relationship with the world (how the dialogue with our Double develops). Our data seem to suggest that if the structure and sequence of thoughts do not change, emotions and behaviors do not change, even if external circumstances can apparently improve.
Moreover, our data regarding control patients may indicate that when a man constructs and follows thoughts of judgment, disapproval, separation, or condemnation, he automatically prepares himself to feel emotions coherent with those thoughts (such as annoyance, irritation, anger, malaise, suffering, sadness, etc.). Remaining in this structure of thought means remaining in these emotions letting the relative molecular mediators that regulate them flow inside the body. Even if the mechanisms by which this happens are still unknown, these psychological operative modalities can be reworked and replaced with others more adaptive and able to promote resilience, well-being and serenity.
As discussed elsewhere [9,10,11], the conceptual linguistic content (meaning) then emerges in a dynamical process going from the syntactic level of lexical elements to the semantic level of the “manifold of concepts” (called in the MP, the set of Logical Forms (LF) [56]). The mathematical formalism describing such a process has been shown [9,10,11] to be isomorph to the one describing in condensed matter physics the formation of coherent states starting from elementary components. In the jargon of the MP, this is expressed by saying that the Narrow Syntax makes contact at the interfaces with the conceptual interpretative (CI) system. The sensory-motor system (SM) is then involved through the dynamics of the cortex by means of the action–perception cycle [44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51]. The result is that the externalization of language expressions becomes possible in a way to manifest the meaningfulness of the subject’s perceptual experiences.
The mentioned coherence of the (brain) states refers to the collective dynamics of the synchronous amplitude modulated (and phase-modulated) oscillations of neuronal assemblies (which are observable by means of Electroencephalogram (EEG), functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI), and other imaging techniques). The collective dynamics is due to long-range neuronal correlations whose dynamical generation is triggered by the perceptual inputs (and maybe heart inputs [57,58]). On the linguistic side, the picture which emerges is that coherent waves spanning lexical components are at the basis of the concept’s generation [9,10,11]. Concepts are thus not specifically associated with individual lexical components. They are instead formally described as collective modes corresponding to the ordering of lexical elements.
Coherence also ensures the stability of the semantic content and corresponds to the minimization of free energy [9,10,11]. The language changes detected by us by means of the LIWC software analysis thus go beyond mere changes in the statistics of the elementary language components of the patient under study. They actually signal changes in the subject’s semantic landscape, namely its rearrangement into a new scenario corresponding to a different minimum free energy state (one can show indeed that the “concept space” includes subspaces, each one endowed with its minimum free energy state [9,10,11]).
On more remark concerns the nonlinearity of the interaction of the subject with its environment. The mathematical description in such a circumstance requires to consider both, the system of interest (the subject) and also its environment, both of which, as said above, must be considered in their persistent interaction. Formally this amounts to the doubling of the degrees of freedom, say A → A × A, where A in the first position denotes the system degrees of freedom (more precisely the neuronal degrees of freedom in the dissipative quantum model of the brain) and the “doubled” A, which we denote as Ã, the ones of the environment (the Double). One can show that the entanglement mentioned above is the one between the A and à modes [46,47,48,49,50,51]. It implies that observables depending on A are actually determined by à (and vice-versa), so that à modes act as the “address”, or the “dynamical reference” of the A modes (and vice-versa). From the linguistic standpoint, it is known that lexical elements may have “copies” which are “seen” at the conceptual interpretative (CI) system level, but remain silent (are not pronounced) at the sensory-motor (SM) system (e.g., in the sentence Which paintings did you see [paintings]? or John saw a man at the corner of the street, and Marc did_ _ _too.) (sometimes a whole syntactic component is omitted and a “syntax of silence” seems to exist [59]). Here, we did not go further in the analysis of these linguistics aspects. We only remarked that including in the formalism the copies of lexical elements allows the possibility of dynamical matching between the two and a “truth evaluation function”, or logical self-consistency, appears to be built-in in languages [9,10,11]. One of the effects of the RR therapy is then the reinforcement of truthfulness and realism feelings in the treated patients.
In summary, the strict interplay between language and thoughts [60,61,62], seems to be a tool of primary importance both at a diagnostic and therapeutic level, since, on the one hand, it seems to reveal the psycho-emotional positions of the person and, on the other, it allows them to be modified if necessary.
Recent evidence suggests that language in terms of “sound” can have a direct effect on cell biological structures [63] and is able to create coherent orders within biological fluids such as plasma [64].
Let us close with a final comment on this specific point. As already discussed in previous works [26,27,64] we have observed that the serum pH of subjects who undergo a Relaxation Response practice (Figure 2) could significantly increase while electric conductivity seems to decrease. According to the many-body physics modeling [64,65,66,67], the efficient metabolic activity in healthy biological systems is favored by coherent dynamics at intra- and inter-cellular levels. The system energetic feeding by ATP hydrolysis or by other sources is used for the generation of organizational activity, namely the formation of long-range coherent correlation modes among the electric dipoles characterizing macromolecules and the molecules of the water bath in which they are embedded. These correlation modes are responsible of the non-vanishing polarization density P(x,t) (the electrets) [66,67]. Chemical, oxidative stress or else functional, mechanical or electromagnetic agents may affect negatively the coherent molecular ordering.
A common laboratory observation [68,69,70,71] is that water molecular dipoles organize themselves in coherently ordered strata adjacent to hydrophilic polymers (for example nafion), extending for a thickness of a hundred of microns. The coherent ordering of these strata expels present particles and is impenetrable by them or other impurities. The strata are called EZ (exclusion zones). Their polarization is such that charges opposite to the ones of the material surface (negative in the case of nafion) are pushed out of the strata. In the direction orthogonal to the surface, the pH shows then a gradient: in the nafion case, pH is lowering moving far away from the surface. Moreover, observations show that in the EZ region it is energetically advantageous the splitting of water molecules into OH− and H+ [68,69,70,71], which of course affects the electrical conductivity [71]. Measurements also show that for water in the presence of nafion [72] higher pH corresponds to lower electric conductivity (and vice-versa).
These experimental observations suggest that similar behavior of pH, electrical conductivity and charge distribution may be also induced in biological fluids in the presence of surfaces such as those of cell membranes, of veins and arteries; a hypothesis which is indeed consistent with our measurements in the Relaxation Response practice [63]. The higher transparency of the plasma in Figure 2 in the post-RR practice case is also consistent with the higher purity of the fluid and higher coherence in its constituent organization, namely with a much lower number of photon scattering processes with “non-correlated” scattering centres (molecules), i.e., lower energy losses by diffusion of photons (they “see” the plasma as a global system, not as a collection of quasi-independent molecular components to scatter with and losing energy (lower transparency)).
The coherence in the plasma microscopic dynamics is, of course, consistent with the general coherent dynamics referred to in our discussion above on brain, RR practice, and language functional features.
As a perspective conclusion, the correspondences between language, stress, inflammation, oxidative stress, and somatization that we have described in this work, although still speculative in many respects, appear to be pointing to a unifying view of mental activity and biochemical activity [49,63,64,65]. Much work along such research lines has still to be done.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.D.L.; methodology, C.D.L.; formal analysis, C.D.L., G.V. and F.T.; investigation: C.D.L., L.B. and M.M.; resources: F.T., S.I. and M.P.; data curation, C.D.L., L.B. and M.M.; writing—original draft preparation, C.D.L. and G.V.; writing—review and editing, C.D.L., G.V. and F.T.; supervision, F.T., S.I. and M.P.; funding acquisition, F.T., S.I. and M.P. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The entire study was funded by the Department of Cardiac, Thoracic and Vascular Sciences, and by the Department of Laboratory Medicine, Padua University School of Medicine.

Acknowledgments

We thank the Pneumomeditazione© teachers and the Entolé staff for the recording of audio files used for meditation and for their support; the meditation music used in this study was composed by Paolo Spoladore.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Comparison of the language analysis according to the LIWC items between RELAXATION RESPONSE group and CONTROLS. The p describing the Intra-group comparison is under, the p describing the inter-group comparison is on the right.
Table A1. Comparison of the language analysis according to the LIWC items between RELAXATION RESPONSE group and CONTROLS. The p describing the Intra-group comparison is under, the p describing the inter-group comparison is on the right.
RELAXATION RESPONSECONTROLSp
Word countSTART344.69 ± 51.44339.32 ± 47.94>0.05
END244.69 ± 34.11329.32 ± 51.71<0.001
p<0.01>0.05
Words per sentenceSTART22.81 ± 5.7123.01 ± 7.39>0.05
END13.54 ± 3.6224.87 ± 6.89<0.05
p<0.01>0.05
Sentences ending with ?START1.82 ± 1.071.95 ± 0.86>0.05
END0.69 ± 0.722.07 ± 1.1<0.001
p<0.01>0.05
% words longer than 6 lettersSTART18.6 ± 3.6217.67 ± 2.11>0.05
END10.6 ± 1.7417.9 ± 3.71<0.01
p<0.001>0.05
Total pronounsSTART70.15 ± 14.0464.72 ± 13.53>0.05
END64.82 ± 14.6968.49 ± 13.71<0.05
p>0.05>0.05
1st person singularSTART10.15 ± 2.8310.01 ± 2.82>0.05
END5.15 ± 1.979.87 ± 2.81<0.01
p<0.001>0.05
1st person pluralSTART6.88 ± 3.547.15 ± 2.95>0.05
END9.88 ± 2.446.75 ± 3.13<0.05
p<0.05>0.05
Total first personSTART17.02 ± 3.6217.16 ± 4.66>0.05
END14.03 ± 2.5116.62 ± 4.64>0.05
p<0.05>0.05
Total second personSTART10.6 ± 2.4910.47 ± 2.47>0.05
END13.6 ± 1.1810.54 ± 2.69<0.05
p<0.01>0.05
Total third personSTART8.07 ± 1.977.57 ± 1.68>0.05
END8.04 ± 1.848.02 ± 1.85>0.05
p>0.05>0.05
NegationSTART16.08 ± 3.816.58 ± 2.81>0.05
END11.08 ± 2.616.04 ± 2.58<0.001
p<0.001>0.05
AssentsSTART6.05 ± 1.956.71 ± 1.79>0.05
END5.97 ± 1.896.62 ± 2.18>0.05
p>0.05>0.05
ArticlesSTART11.74 ± 3.1611.36 ± 3.23>0.05
END8.74 ± 3.2511.27 ± 2.76<0.01
p<0.01>0.05
PrepositionsSTART18.93 ± 4.6218.16 ± 4.52>0.05
END20.93 ± 4.1219.07 ± 4.61>0.05
p>0.05>0.05
NumbersSTART11.87 ± 2.3112.06 ± 2.25>0.05
END12.26 ± 2.3411.76 ± 2.42>0.05
p>0.05>0.05
Affective/emotional processesSTART24.07 ± 6.7924.86 ± 7.28>0.05
END31.07 ± 7.2719.86 ± 7.35<0.001
p<0.01<0.01
Positive emotionsSTART17.61 ± 4.5717.86 ± 4.87>0.05
END22.61 ± 3.6716.94 ± 3.88<0.001
p<0.01>0.05
Optimism and energySTART27.55 ± 5.1927.56 ± 5.55>0.05
END32.55 ± 4.6824.56 ± 6.45<0.05
p<0.05>0.05
Negative emotionsSTART39.83 ± 9.4241.32 ± 6.58>0.05
END31.83 ± 7.3143.32 ± 7.77<0.01
p<0.01>0.05
Anxiety or fearSTART52.57 ± 11.3950.78 ± 11.53>0.05
END37.57 ± 9.2952.78 ± 10.87<0.001
p<0.001>0.05
AngerSTART25.66 ± 6.4426.04 ± 5.93>0.05
END14.66 ± 5.2523.04 ± 6.63<0.01
p<0.001>0.05
Sadness or depressionSTART44.69 ± 9.2645.06 ± 9.36>0.05
END29.69 ± 7.7641.06 ± 10.46<0.001
p<0.001>0.05
Cognitive processesSTART31.42 ± 9.4435 ± 9.89>0.05
END24.42 ± 7.4138 ± 7.19<0.001
p<0.01>0.05
CausationSTART16.47 ± 3.6716.52 ± 3.76>0.05
END12.47 ± 2.1722.52 ± 4.76<0.01
p<0.01>0.05
InsightSTART9.64 ± 3.638.54 ± 4.16>0.05
END9.22 ± 3.388.7 ± 3.5>0.05
p>0.05>0.05
DiscrepancySTART10.88 ± 3.1111.16 ± 3>0.05
END6.88 ± 4.1114.16 ± 3.2<0.001
p<0.01>0.05
InhibitionSTART12.07 ± 2.6911.84 ± 2.01>0.05
END7.07 ± 1.9710.84 ± 2.41<0.001
p<0.001>0.05
TentativeSTART11.51 ± 3.1311.46 ± 3.3>0.05
END7.51 ± 2.7316.46 ± 1.3<0.001
p<0.001<0.05
CertaintySTART4.76 ± 1.895.44 ± 2.25>0.05
END2.8 ± 1.98.74 ± 2.87<0.001
p<0.05<0.05
Sensory and perceptual processesSTART33.36 ± 9.4131.68 ± 10.56>0.05
END40.36 ± 7.5126.68 ± 8.56<0.001
p<0.01<0.01
Social processesSTART21.51 ± 8.220.3 ± 8.27>0.05
END24.12 ± 3.213.3 ± 4.14<0.001
p<0.05<0.01
Past tense verbSTART14.47 ± 3.0915.42 ± 2.92>0.05
END9.47 ± 1.0920.42 ± 3.32<0.001
p<0.01<0.01
Present tense verbsSTART15.22 ± 3.4714.64 ± 3.49>0.05
END17.22 ± 2.1713.64 ± 2.16<0.01
p<0.05<0.05
Future tense verbSTART11.23 ± 3.0610.58 ± 3>0.05
END8.23 ± 2.0613.58 ± 1.7<0.01
p<0.01<0.05
InclusiveSTART5.28 ± 1.675.08 ± 1.58>0.05
END9.28 ± 0.674.08 ± 0.86<0.001
p<0.001<0.01
ExclusiveSTART6.98 ± 1.647.2 ± 0.99>0.05
END4.98 ± 0.949.2 ± 1.97<0.001
p<0.01<0.05
MotionSTART14.81 ± 2.6513.88 ± 2.9>0.05
END17.81 ± 1.659.88 ± 2.9<0.01
p<0.05>0.05
Leisure activitySTART7.2 ± 1.767.06 ± 1.88>0.05
END14.2 ± 0.966.06 ± 1.85<0.001
p<0.001>0.05
Money and financial issuesSTART17.83 ± 3.7217.76 ± 3.23>0.05
END15.83 ± 4.0222.76 ± 1.73<0.01
p>0.05<0.05
Body symptomsSTART25.02 ± 3.8924.98 ± 3.67>0.05
END17.02 ± 2.8921.98 ± 4.29<0.05
p<0.01>0.05
Body functionsSTART5.37 ± 2.555.62 ± 2.46>0.05
END5.64 ± 2.925.42 ± 2.41>0.05
p>0.05>0.05
Table A2. Comparison of the language analysis according to the LIWC items between RELAXATION RESPONSE group and RELAXATION RESPONSE HEALTHY CONTROLS. The p describing the Intra-group comparison is under, the p describing the inter-group comparison is on the right.
Table A2. Comparison of the language analysis according to the LIWC items between RELAXATION RESPONSE group and RELAXATION RESPONSE HEALTHY CONTROLS. The p describing the Intra-group comparison is under, the p describing the inter-group comparison is on the right.
RELAXATION RESPONSERELAXATION RESPONSE HEALTHY CONTROLSp
Word countSTART344.69 ± 51.44337.63 ± 46.15>0.05
END244.69 ± 34.11187.63 ± 41.26<0.01
p<0.01<0.001
Words per sentenceSTART22.81 ± 5.7121.43 ± 7.52>0.05
END13.54 ± 3.6213.43 ± 2.64>0.05
p<0.01<0.001
Sentences ending with ?START1.82 ± 1.071.95 ± 0.92>0.05
END0.69 ± 0.720.68 ± 0.68>0.05
p<0.01<0.001
% words longer than 6 lettersSTART18.6 ± 3.6217.62 ± 3.5>0.05
END10.6 ± 1.748.62 ± 4.3>0.05
p<0.001<0.01
Total pronounsSTART70.15 ± 14.0469.64 ± 13.83>0.05
END64.82 ± 14.6969.27 ± 13.73>0.05
p>0.05>0.05
1st person singularSTART10.15 ± 2.8310.4 ± 2.69>0.05
END5.15 ± 1.976.4 ± 2.28>0.05
p<0.001<0.01
1st person pluralSTART6.88 ± 3.546.76 ± 2.47>0.05
END9.88 ± 2.449.76 ± 1.87>0.05
p<0.05<0.05
Total first personSTART17.02 ± 3.6217.16 ± 3.8>0.05
END14.03 ± 2.5116.16 ± 4.7<0.05
p<0.05>0.05
Total second personSTART10.6 ± 2.4910.46 ± 2.89>0.05
END13.6 ± 1.1813.86 ± 1.96>0.05
p<0.01<0.01
Total third personSTART8.07 ± 1.977.67 ± 1.5>0.05
END8.04 ± 1.848.38 ± 1.94>0.05
p>0.05>0.05
NegationSTART16.08 ± 3.816.24 ± 2.69>0.05
END11.08 ± 2.610.24 ± 3.08>0.05
p<0.001<0.01
AssentsSTART6.05 ± 1.955.97 ± 1.84>0.05
END5.97 ± 1.896.02 ± 1.76>0.05
p>0.05>0.05
ArticlesSTART11.74 ± 3.1612.34 ± 3.17>0.05
END8.74 ± 3.258.34 ± 2.34>0.05
p<0.01<0.01
PrepositionsSTART18.93 ± 4.6218.54 ± 4.08>0.05
END20.93 ± 4.1220.54 ± 5.11>0.05
p>0.05>0.05
NumbersSTART11.87 ± 2.3111.94 ± 2.35>0.05
END12.26 ± 2.3412 ± 2.5>0.05
p>0.05>0.05
Affective/emotional processesSTART24.07 ± 6.7924.92 ± 6.46>0.05
END31.07 ± 7.2732.92 ± 5.44>0.05
p<0.01<0.01
Positive emotionsSTART17.61 ± 4.5717.94 ± 4.56>0.05
END22.61 ± 3.6723.94 ± 2.16>0.05
p<0.01<0.01
Optimism and energySTART27.55 ± 5.1927.66 ± 5.34>0.05
END32.55 ± 4.6833.66 ± 4.12>0.05
p<0.05<0.05
Negative emotionsSTART39.83 ± 9.4227.62 ± 7.72<0.05
END31.83 ± 7.3122.62 ± 6.53<0.01
p<0..1<0.01
Anxiety or fearSTART52.57 ± 11.3931.84 ± 9.53<0.001
END37.57 ± 9.2921.84 ± 7.95<0.01
p<0.001<0.01
AngerSTART25.66 ± 6.4411.3 ± 3.2<0.001
END14.66 ± 5.256.3 ± 2.1<0.001
p<0.001<0.01
Sadness or depressionSTART44.69 ± 9.2619.43 ± 4.18<0.001
END29.69 ± 7.7610.43 ± 4.78<0.001
p<0.001<0.01
Cognitive processesSTART31.42 ± 9.4433.02 ± 7.87>0.05
END24.42 ± 7.4127.02 ± 4.11>0.05
p<0.01<0.05
CausationSTART16.47 ± 3.6716.64 ± 3.88>0.05
END12.47 ± 2.1711.64 ± 2.77>0.05
p<0.01<0.01
InsightSTART9.64 ± 3.638.96 ± 3.07>0.05
END9.22 ± 3.389.58 ± 3.51>0.05
p>0.05>0.05
DiscrepancySTART10.88 ± 3.1110.62 ± 2.9>0.05
END6.88 ± 4.115.62 ± 2.87>0.05
p<0.01<0.001
InhibitionSTART12.07 ± 2.695.31 ± 1.94<0.001
END7.07 ± 1.972.31 ± 0.94<0.001
p<0.001<0.001
TentativeSTART11.51 ± 3.136.41 ± 2.07<0.001
END7.51 ± 2.734.41 ± 1.07<0.01
p<0.001<0.01
CertaintySTART4.76 ± 1.894.9 ± 1.88>0.05
END2.8 ± 1.92.94 ± 1.8>0.05
p<0.05<0.01
Sensory and perceptual processesSTART33.36 ± 9.4135.52 ± 8.8>0.05
END40.36 ± 7.5141.52 ± 6.3>0.05
p<0.01<0.001
Social processesSTART21.51 ± 8.225.54 ± 8.51<0.05
END24.12 ± 3.229.54 ± 5.32<0.05
p<0.05<0.05
Past tense verbSTART14.47 ± 3.096 ± 2.23<0.001
END9.47 ± 1.093 ± 2.03<0.001
p<0.01<0.01
Present tense verbSTART15.22 ± 3.4714.98 ± 3.44>0.05
END17.22 ± 2.1716.98 ± 2.48>0.05
p<0.05<0.01
Future tense verbSTART11.23 ± 3.064.64 ± 1.66<0.001
END8.23 ± 2.062.64 ± 1.97<0.001
p<0..1>0.05
InclusiveSTART5.28 ± 1.6710.26 ± 2.54<0.001
END9.28 ± 0.6712.26 ± 1.66<0.001
p<0.001<0.01
ExclusiveSTART6.98 ± 1.643.65 ± 2.19<0.01
END4.98 ± 0.942.7 ± 2.11<0.001
p<0.01>0.05
MotionSTART14.81 ± 2.6514.84 ± 3.19>0.05
END17.81 ± 1.6516.84 ± 4.33>0.05
p<0.05>0.05
Leisure activitySTART7.2 ± 1.7611.08 ± 2.71<0.05
END14.2 ± 0.9615.08 ± 3.66>0.05
p<0.001<0.05
Money and financial issuesSTART17.83 ± 3.7212.92 ± 2.68<0.01
END15.83 ± 4.028.92 ± 1.03<0.001
p>0.05<0.01
Body symptomsSTART25.02 ± 3.8912.86 ± 2.25<0.001
END17.02 ± 2.896.86 ± 1.74<0.001
p<0.01<0.001
Body functionsSTART5.37 ± 2.556.06 ± 2.65>0.05
END5.64 ± 2.925.74 ± 2.68>0.05
p>0.05>0.05

References

  1. Chrousos, G.P. Stress and disorders of the stress system. Nat. Rev. Endocrinol. 2009, 5, 374–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Song, H.; Fang, F.; Arnberg, F.K.; Mataix-Cols, D.; de la Cruz, L.F.; Almqvist, C.; Fall, K.; Lichtenstein, P.; Thorgeirsson, G.; Valdimarsdóttir, U.A. Stress related disorders and risk of cardiovascular disease: Population based, sibling controlled cohort study. BMJ 2019, 365, l1255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. David, D.; Cotet, C.; Matu, S.; Mogoase, C.; Stefan, S. 50 years of rational-emotive and cognitive-behavioral therapy: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Clin. Psychol. 2018, 74, 304–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Vîslă, A.; Flückiger, C.; Grosse Holtforth, M.; David, D. Irrational Beliefs and Psychological Distress: A Meta-Analysis. Psychother. Psychosom. 2016, 85, 8–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Disner, S.G.; Beevers, C.G.; Haigh, E.A.P.; Beck, A.T. Neural mechanisms of the cognitive model of depression. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2011, 12, 467–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Freeman, W.J. How Brains Make up Their Minds; Columbia University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  7. Chomsky, N. The Logical Structure of Linguistic Theory; Harvard University and MIT: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1955. [Google Scholar]
  8. Chomsky, N. Lectures on Government and Binding: The Pisa Lectures; Walter de Gruyter: Berlin, Germany, 1981. [Google Scholar]
  9. Piattelli-Palmarini, M.; Vitiello, G. Linguistics and some aspects of its underlying dynamics. Biolinguistics 2015, 9, 96–115. [Google Scholar]
  10. Piattelli-Palmarini, M.; Vitiello, G. Third factors in language design: Some suggestions from Quantum Field Theory. In The Cambridge Companion to Chomsky; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  11. Piattelli-Palmarini, M.; Vitiello, G. Quantum field theory and the linguistic Minimalist Program: A remarkable isomorphism. J. Phys. Conf. Ser. 2017, 880, 012016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Chomsky, N. The Minimalist Program; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  13. Chomsky, N. Minimalist inquiries: The framework. In Step by Step: Essays on Minimalist Syntax in Honor of Howard Lasnik; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2000; pp. 89–155. [Google Scholar]
  14. Chomsky, N. Derivation by phase. In A Life in Language; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2001; pp. 1–52. [Google Scholar]
  15. Chomsky, N. Three factors in language design. Linguist. Inq. 2005, 36, 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Chomsky, N. Problems of projection. Lingua 2013, 130, 33–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Moro, A. Dynamic Antisymmetry; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  18. Perlovsky, L. Language and cognition—Joint acquisition, dual hierarchy, and emotional prosody. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2013, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Perlovsky, L.; Sakai, K.L. Language and Cognition. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2014, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  20. Capitanio, J.P. Personality and disease. Brain. Behav. Immun. 2008, 22, 647–650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  21. Friedman, H.S. The multiple linkages of personality and disease. Brain Behav. Immun. 2008, 22, 668–675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  22. Penzel, I.B.; Persich, M.R.; Boyd, R.L.; Robinson, M.D. Linguistic Evidence for the Failure Mindset as a Predictor of Life Span Longevity. Ann. Behav. Med. 2017, 51, 348–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Lanning, K.; Pauletti, R.E.; King, L.A.; McAdams, D.P. Personality development through natural language. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2018, 2, 327–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Yarkoni, T. Personality in 100,000 Words: A large-scale analysis of personality and word use among bloggers. J. Res. Personal. 2010, 44, 363–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Boyd, R.L.; Pennebaker, J.W. Language-based personality: A new approach to personality in a digital world. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2017, 18, 63–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Dal Lin, C.; Marinova, M.; Rubino, G.; Gola, E.; Brocca, A.; Pantano, G.; Brugnolo, L.; Sarais, C.; Cucchini, U.; Volpe, B.; et al. Thoughts modulate the expression of inflammatory genes and may improve the coronary blood flow in patients after a myocardial infarction. J. Tradit. Complement. Med. 2018, 8, 150–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Dal Lin, C.; Gola, E.; Brocca, A.; Rubino, G.; Marinova, M.; Brugnolo, L.; Plebani, M.; Iliceto, S.; Tona, F. miRNAs may change rapidly with thoughts: The Relaxation Response after myocardial infarction. Eur. J. Integr. Med. 2018, 20, 63–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Benson, H.; Proctor, W. Relaxation Revolution: The Science and Genetics of Mind Body Healing; Simon & Schuster: New York, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  29. Dal Lin, C.; Poretto, A.; Scodro, M.; Perazzolo Marra, M.; Iliceto, S.; Tona, F. Coronary microvascular and endothelial function regulation: Crossroads of psychoneuroendocrine immunitary signals and quantum physics [Part A-B and C]. J. Integr. Cardiol. 2015, 1, 132–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Benson, H.; Klipper, M.Z. The Relaxation Response; HarperCollins: New York, NY, USA, 1975. [Google Scholar]
  31. Wasserstein, R.L.; Lazar, N.A. The ASA’s Statement on p-Values: Context, Process, and Purpose. Am. Stat. 2016, 70, 129–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Packer, M. The Parable of Schrödinger’s Cat and the Illusion of Statistical Significance in Clinical Trials. Circulation 2019, 140, 799–800. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Steptoe, A.; Kivimäki, M. Stress and cardiovascular disease. Nat. Rev. Cardiol. 2012, 9, 360–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Kinner, V.L.; Het, S.; Wolf, O.T. Emotion regulation: Exploring the impact of stress and sex. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2014, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Dal Lin, C.; Tona, F.; Osto, E. The Heart as a Psychoneuroendocrine and Immunoregulatory Organ. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 2018, 1065, 225–239. [Google Scholar]
  36. Gazzaniga, M.S.; Ivry, R.B.; Mangun, G.R. Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 4th ed.; Norton and Company Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  37. Ochsner, K.N.; Bunge, S.A.; Gross, J.J.; Gabrieli, J.D.E. Rethinking Feelings: An fMRI Study of the Cognitive Regulation of Emotion. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2002, 14, 1215–1229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Ochsner, K.N.; Ray, R.D.; Cooper, J.C.; Robertson, E.R.; Chopra, S.; Gabrieli, J.D.E.; Gross, J.J. For better or for worse: Neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion. Neuroimage 2004, 23, 483–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Tawakol, A.; Ishai, A.; Takx, R.A.; Figueroa, A.L.; Ali, A.; Kaiser, Y.; Truong, Q.A.; Solomon, C.J.; Calcagno, C.; Mani, V.; et al. Relation between resting amygdalar activity and cardiovascular events: A longitudinal and cohort study. Lancet 2017, 389, 834–845. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Schmidt, S. Meditation—Neuroscientific Approaches and Philosophical Implications. Available online: http://www.springer.com/us/book/9783319016337 (accessed on 31 August 2015).
  41. Sciacchitano, S.; Lavra, L.; Morgante, A.; Ulivieri, A.; Magi, F.; De Francesco, G.P.; Bellotti, C.; Salehi, L.B.; Ricci, A. Galectin-3: One molecule for an alphabet of diseases, from A to Z. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2018, 19, 379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Del Rio, D.; Stewart, A.J.; Pellegrini, N. A review of recent studies on malondialdehyde as toxic molecule and biological marker of oxidative stress. Nutr. Metab. Cardiovasc. Dis. 2005, 15, 316–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Ho, E.; Karimi Galougahi, K.; Liu, C.C.; Bhindi, R.; Figtree, G.A. Biological markers of oxidative stress: Applications to cardiovascular research and practice. Redox Biol. 2013, 1, 483–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Vitiello, G. Dissipation and memory capacity in the quantum brain model. Int. J. Mod. Phys. B 1995, 9, 973–989. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Vitiello, G. My Double Unveiled; John Benjamins: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  46. Vitiello, G. The dissipative brain. In Brain and Being: At the Boundary between Science, Philosophy, Language and Arts; John Benjamins: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  47. Freeman, W.J.; Vitiello, G. Nonlinear brain dynamics as macroscopic manifestation of underlying many-body dynamics. Phys. Life Rev. 2006, 3, 93–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Freeman, W.J.; Vitiello, G. Dissipation and spontaneous symmetry breaking in brain dynamics. J. Phys. A Math. Theor. 2008, 41, 304042. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Freeman, W.J.; Vitiello, G. Matter and mind are entangled in two streams of images guiding behavior and informing the subject through awareness. Mind Matter 2016, 14, 7–24. [Google Scholar]
  50. Pessa, E.; Vitiello, G. Quantum noise, entanglement and chaos in the quantum field theory of mind/brain states. Mind Matter 2003, 1, 59–79. [Google Scholar]
  51. Desideri, F. La Percezione Riflessa. Estetica e Filosofia Della Mente; Cortina: Milano, Italy, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  52. Vitiello, G. The Aesthetic Experience as a Characteristic Feature of Brain Dynamics; Firenze University Press: Firenze, Italy, 2015; Volume VIII, pp. 71–89. [Google Scholar]
  53. Dahl, C.J.; Lutz, A.; Davidson, R.J. Reconstructing and deconstructing the self: Cognitive mechanisms in meditation practice. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2015, 19, 515–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Pennebaker, J.W. The Secret Life of Pronouns: What Our Words Say about Us; Bloomsbury Press: London, UK, 2011; ISBN 1608194809. [Google Scholar]
  55. May, R. Logical Form: Its Structure and Derivation; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar]
  56. McCraty, R.; Atkinson, M. Influence of afferent cardiovascular input on cognitive performance and alpha activity. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Pavlovian Society, Tarrytown, NY, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  57. McCraty, R.; Atkinson, M.; Tomasino, D.; Bradley, R.T. The coherent heart: Heart-brain interactions, psychophysiological coherence, and the emergence of system-wide order. Integr. Rev. 2009, 5, 10–115. [Google Scholar]
  58. Merchant, J. The Syntax of Silence: Sluicing Islands and the Theory of Ellipsis; OUP: Oxford, UK, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  59. Newcombe, N.S.; Uttal, D.H. Whorf versus Socrates, round 10. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2006, 10, 394–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Vygotsky, L. Thought and Language; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1992; Volume 5, ISBN 9780511563782. [Google Scholar]
  61. Boroditsky, L. How language shapes thought. Sci. Am. 2011, 304, 62–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Dal Lin, C.; Radu, C.M.; Vitiello, G.; Romano, P.; Polcari, A.; Iliceto, S.; Simioni, P.; Tona, F. In vitro effects on cellular shaping, contratility, cytoskeletal organization and mitochondrial activity in HL1 cells after different sounds stimulation. A qualitative pilot study and a theoretical physical model. bioRxiv 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Dal Lin, C.; Grasso, R.; Scordino, A.; Triglia, A.; Tona, F.; Iliceto, S.; Vitiello, G.; Elia, V.; Napoli, E.; Germano, R.; et al. Ph, Electric Conductivity and Delayed Luminescence Changes in Human Sera of Subjects Undergoing the Relaxation Response: A Pilot Study. Preprints 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Goldstone, J.; Salam, A.; Weinberg, S. Broken Symmetries. Phys. Rev. 1962, 127, 965–970. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Del Giudice, E.; Doglia, S.; Milani, M.; Vitiello, G. Structure, correlations and electromagnetic interactions in living matter: Theory and applications. In Biological Coherence and Response to External Stimuli; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 1988; pp. 49–64. [Google Scholar]
  66. Del Giudice, E.; Doglia, S.; Milani, M.; Vitiello, G. Electromagnetic field and spontaneous symmetry breakdown in biological matter. Nucl. Phys. B 1986, 275, 185–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Del Giudice, E.; Doglia, S.; Milani, M.; Vitiello, G. A quantum field theoretical approach to the collective behavior of biological systems. Nucl. Phys. B 1985, 251, 375–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Zheng, J.; Pollack, G. Long range forces extending from polymer surfaces. Phys. Rev. E 2003, 68, 031408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Zheng, J.; Chin, W.; Khijniak, E.; Khijniak, E.J.; Pollack, G. Surfaces and interfacial water: Evidence that hydrophilic surfaces have long-range impact. Adv. Colloid Interface Sci. 2006, 23, 19–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Zheng, J.; Wexler, A.; Pollack, G. Effect of buffers on aqueous solute-exclusion zones around ion-exchange resins. J. Colloid Interface Sci. 2009, 332, 511–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  71. Del Giudice, E.; Voeikov, V.; Tedeschi, A.; Vitiello, G. The origin and the special role of coherent water in living systems. In Fields of the Cell; Research Signpost: Trivandrum, India, 2015; pp. 95–111. [Google Scholar]
  72. Capolupo, A.; Del Giudice, E.; Elia, V.; Germano, R.; Napoli, E.; Niccoli, M.; Tedeschi, A.; Vitiello, G. Self-Similarity properties of Nafionized and Filtered Water and Deformed Coherent States. Int. J. Mod. Phys. B 2014, 28, 1450007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. The study design. Explanation in the text. RR: Relaxation Response. RR 20 min: after 4 days of training, each subject relaxes through meditation or music appreciation for 20 min. A blood sample is taken immediately before and immediately after. The acute variation of the studied parameters can be attributed to the practice of relaxation according to the used methods because the precise timing of blood sampling (before and immediately after the end of the session) prevents any other influences. All groups were subjected to the same environmental conditions: in particular, also the control patients were taken in our classroom for 20 min and were not subjected to any intervention. We simply asked them to relax and most of them sat down with eyes closed. For more details please see our previous works [26,27].
Figure 1. The study design. Explanation in the text. RR: Relaxation Response. RR 20 min: after 4 days of training, each subject relaxes through meditation or music appreciation for 20 min. A blood sample is taken immediately before and immediately after. The acute variation of the studied parameters can be attributed to the practice of relaxation according to the used methods because the precise timing of blood sampling (before and immediately after the end of the session) prevents any other influences. All groups were subjected to the same environmental conditions: in particular, also the control patients were taken in our classroom for 20 min and were not subjected to any intervention. We simply asked them to relax and most of them sat down with eyes closed. For more details please see our previous works [26,27].
Entropy 22 00818 g001
Figure 2. Variation of the physical characteristics of the plasma of the same patient during 20 min of meditation. On the left: the blood sample (after 4 min of centrifugation at 5000 rpm) before meditation is opalescent. On the right, the blood sample immediately after meditation is clearer. The patient was fasting for more than 5 h before meditating.
Figure 2. Variation of the physical characteristics of the plasma of the same patient during 20 min of meditation. On the left: the blood sample (after 4 min of centrifugation at 5000 rpm) before meditation is opalescent. On the right, the blood sample immediately after meditation is clearer. The patient was fasting for more than 5 h before meditating.
Entropy 22 00818 g002
Figure 3. Galectin-3 (GAL-3) at different time points. Median and interquartile range, statistical analysis.
Figure 3. Galectin-3 (GAL-3) at different time points. Median and interquartile range, statistical analysis.
Entropy 22 00818 g003
Figure 4. Malondialdehyde (MDA) at different time points. Median and interquartile range, statistical analysis.
Figure 4. Malondialdehyde (MDA) at different time points. Median and interquartile range, statistical analysis.
Entropy 22 00818 g004
Table 1. Trend of the language analysis variations according to the Language Inquiry Word Count (LIWC) items between RELAXATION RESPONSE group, CONTROLS, and RELAXATION RESPONSE HEALTHY CONTROLS.
Table 1. Trend of the language analysis variations according to the Language Inquiry Word Count (LIWC) items between RELAXATION RESPONSE group, CONTROLS, and RELAXATION RESPONSE HEALTHY CONTROLS.
ITEMRELAXATION RESPONSECONTROLSRELAXATION RESPONSE HEALTHY CONTROLS
Word count, Words per sentence, Sentences ending with ? % words longer than 6 letters Entropy 22 00818 i001- Entropy 22 00818 i001
Total pronouns---
1st person singular Entropy 22 00818 i001- Entropy 22 00818 i001
1st person plural Entropy 22 00818 i002- Entropy 22 00818 i002
Total first person Entropy 22 00818 i001--
Total second person Entropy 22 00818 i002- Entropy 22 00818 i002
Total third person---
Negation Entropy 22 00818 i001- Entropy 22 00818 i001
Assents---
Articles Entropy 22 00818 i001- Entropy 22 00818 i001
Prepositions---
Numbers---
Affective/emotional processes, Positive emotions, Optimism and energy Entropy 22 00818 i002 Entropy 22 00818 i003 Entropy 22 00818 i002
Negative emotions, Anxiety or fear, Anger, Sadness or depression Entropy 22 00818 i001- Entropy 22 00818 i001
Cognitive processes, Causation Entropy 22 00818 i001- Entropy 22 00818 i001
Insight---
Discrepancy, Inhibition Entropy 22 00818 i001- Entropy 22 00818 i001
Tentative, Certainty Entropy 22 00818 i001 Entropy 22 00818 i004 Entropy 22 00818 i001
Sensory and perceptual processes, Social processes Entropy 22 00818 i002 Entropy 22 00818 i003 Entropy 22 00818 i002
Past tense verb, Future tense verb Entropy 22 00818 i001 Entropy 22 00818 i004 Entropy 22 00818 i001
Present tense verb Entropy 22 00818 i002 Entropy 22 00818 i003 Entropy 22 00818 i002
Inclusive Entropy 22 00818 i002 Entropy 22 00818 i003 Entropy 22 00818 i002
Exclusive Entropy 22 00818 i001 Entropy 22 00818 i004 Entropy 22 00818 i001
Motion, Leisure activity Entropy 22 00818 i002- Entropy 22 00818 i002
Money and financial issues- Entropy 22 00818 i004 Entropy 22 00818 i001
Body symptoms Entropy 22 00818 i001- Entropy 22 00818 i001
Body functions---

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Dal Lin, C.; Brugnolo, L.; Marinova, M.; Plebani, M.; Iliceto, S.; Tona, F.; Vitiello, G. Toward a Unified View of Cognitive and Biochemical Activity: Meditation and Linguistic Self-Reconstructing May Lead to Inflammation and Oxidative Stress Improvement. Entropy 2020, 22, 818. https://doi.org/10.3390/e22080818

AMA Style

Dal Lin C, Brugnolo L, Marinova M, Plebani M, Iliceto S, Tona F, Vitiello G. Toward a Unified View of Cognitive and Biochemical Activity: Meditation and Linguistic Self-Reconstructing May Lead to Inflammation and Oxidative Stress Improvement. Entropy. 2020; 22(8):818. https://doi.org/10.3390/e22080818

Chicago/Turabian Style

Dal Lin, Carlo, Laura Brugnolo, Mariela Marinova, Mario Plebani, Sabino Iliceto, Francesco Tona, and Giuseppe Vitiello. 2020. "Toward a Unified View of Cognitive and Biochemical Activity: Meditation and Linguistic Self-Reconstructing May Lead to Inflammation and Oxidative Stress Improvement" Entropy 22, no. 8: 818. https://doi.org/10.3390/e22080818

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop