Next Article in Journal
Climate Activism in Our Part of The World and Methodological Insights on How to Study It
Previous Article in Journal
An Analysis of Chronic Stress, Substance Use, and Mental Health Among a Sample of Young Sexual Minority Men in New York City: The P18 Cohort Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Nomophobia Levels in Turkish High School Students: Variations by Gender, Physical Activity, Grade Level and Smartphone Use

by
Piyami Çakto
1,
İlyas Görgüt
2,†,
Amayra Tannoubi
3,4,†,
Michael Agyei
5,
Medina Srem-Sai
6,
John Elvis Hagan
5,7,*,
Oğuzhan Yüksel
2 and
Orhan Demir
1
1
Physical Education and Sports (DR), Graduate Education Institute, Kutahya Dumlupınar University, Kutahya 72000, Turkey
2
Faculty of Sport Sciences, Kutahya Dumlupınar University, Kutahya 72000, Turkey
3
High Institute of Sport and Physical Education of Gafsa, University of Gafsa, Gafsa 2100, Tunisia
4
Sports Performance Optimization Research Laboratory (LR09SEP01), National Center for Sports Medicine and Science (CNMSS), Tunis 1003, Tunisia
5
Department of Health, Physical Education and Recreation, University of Cape Coast, Cape Coast PMB TF0494, Ghana
6
Department of Health, Physical Education, Recreation and Sports, University of Education, Winneba P.O. Box 25, Ghana
7
Neurocognition and Action-Biomechanics-Research Group, Faculty of Psychology and Sports Science, Bielefeld University, 33501 Bielefeld, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Youth 2025, 5(3), 78; https://doi.org/10.3390/youth5030078
Submission received: 15 May 2025 / Revised: 18 July 2025 / Accepted: 21 July 2025 / Published: 1 August 2025

Abstract

The rapidly changing dynamics of the digital age reshape the addiction relationship that high school students establish with technology. While smartphones remove boundaries in terms of communication and access to information, their usage triggers a source of anxiety and nomophobia. The increase in students’ anxiety levels because of their over-reliance on mobile phone use leads to significant behavioral changes in their mental health, academic performance, social interactions and financial dependency. This study examined the nomophobia levels of high school students according to selected socio-demographic indicators. Using the relational screening model, the multistage sampling technique was used to select a sample of 884 participants: 388 from Science High School and 496 from Anatolian High School (459 female, 425 male, Mage = 16.45 ± 1.14 year). Independent sample test and One-way ANOVA were applied. Depending on the homogeneity assumption of the data, Welch values were considered, and Tukey tests were applied as a second-level test from post hoc analyses. Comprehensive analyses of nomophobia levels revealed that young individuals’ attitudes towards digital technology differ significantly according to their demographic and behavioral characteristics. Variables such as gender, physical activity participation, grade level and duration of smartphone use are among the main factors affecting nomophobia levels. Female individuals and students who do not participate in physical activity exhibit higher nomophobia scores.

1. Introduction

The influx of technological tools and models to enhance human development and progression has skyrocketed. These tools range from mobile smartphones, laptops, computers, tablets, projectors and cameras to pen drives. The use of these tools has become essential due to the ease in accessing them and the increased use of social media and for fun and entertainment from other digital platforms. Smartphones are one of the most widely used tools offered by technology (Le Steunf et al., 2024). Smartphones can perform various functions like computers. People have started to access information more easily with their smartphones, and this has caused smartphones to become a part of individuals’ lives (Liébana-Cabanillas et al., 2014). In addition to communicating, they are also used for a wide variety of purposes such as connecting to the internet, accessing social networks, taking photos, shopping, listening to music, playing games and navigation (Lee et al., 2014; Prasad et al., 2017). The digitalization of society has profoundly changed the way individuals interact socially. While digitalization has enabled us to communicate, acquire knowledge, develop ideas and maximize opportunities, it has also brought challenges and risks that lead to new psychosocial pathologies, especially for young people (Salmela-Aro & Motti-Stefanidi, 2022). While anonymity allows individuals to interact by hiding their online identities, ease of access can lead to uncontrolled internet use. Accelerated intimacy allows online relationships to develop faster than those in the real world, while time distortion causes individuals to stay online for a long time without realizing it and changes in their perception of reality. When these factors come together, they contribute to the reinforcement of internet addiction (Greenfield, 1999). With the advancement of technology, social networks and access to the internet have enabled individuals to communicate and express themselves in the virtual world, but this has also led to addictions such as nomophobia (Lin & Huang, 2025). It occurs when an individual loses control over the use of technology, and this increases their dependence on technological tools. According to Bragazzi and Del Puente (2014), nomophobia is considered as a disorder of contemporary virtual and digital society and refers to anxiety, discomfort, irritability or distress caused by lack of contact with a computer or cell phone (Bragazzi & Del Puente, 2014). Nomophobia can cause pathological consequences such as depression, anxiety, distraction and aggressive behavior in adolescents (Black et al., 2022; Hällgren & Björk, 2022). Frequent use of mobile phones causes psychological, physical and behavioral problems in adolescents, exposing them to misinformation and inappropriate or risky contacts, and jeopardizing privacy and personal data security (Cerisola, 2017). Although there is no agreement on how we label or conceptualize problematic use of smartphones, we need to better understand this phenomenon and improve the way we intervene psychosocially to prevent and treat it (Ruiz-Ruano et al., 2020).
Nomophobia, defined as the fear or anxiety of being without or unable to use a mobile phone, has emerged as a growing public health problem worldwide. Extensive research shows that mobile device addiction negatively affects individuals’ sleep quality, academic performance and social interactions (Durak, 2019). Long-term smartphone use is associated with increased social isolation and decreased physical activity (Buke et al., 2021; Dwyer et al., 2023; Kardos et al., 2018; Lepp et al., 2014; Srivastava, 2005). These patterns are particularly prominent among adolescents and young adults, whose developmental stages increase susceptibility to digital overuse (Dursun et al., 2024). Recent studies show that the ubiquity of smartphones contributes to a sedentary lifestyle in youth and adult populations (Brailovskaia et al., 2023; Gonçalves et al., 2020; Precht et al., 2024). The captivating nature of digital content, including social media platforms, mobile games and short form videos, often replaces outdoor and physical activities, increasing health risks such as obesity and cardiovascular disorders (Geçgel, 2020; Brailovskaia et al., 2023). Moreover, excessive screen time has been associated with disrupted sleep routines and reduced motivation for physical activity (Al Battashi et al., 2021; Daraj et al., 2023). There is a strong correlation between mobile device addiction and sedentary behaviors, especially among university-aged individuals (AlMarzooqi et al., 2022; Daraj et al., 2023).
While global trends highlight the rise of nomophobia, its effects on the adolescent population in Turkey warrant closer scrutiny. Turkey has experienced rapid growth in smartphone penetration rates, and adolescents exhibit some of the highest levels of daily screen engagement. Cultural norms, educational pressures and urban lifestyle factors uniquely shape young people’s interaction with digital technologies in Turkey. Moreover, limited local data on the behavioral and health-related consequences of smartphone addiction creates a critical gap in understanding how nomophobia manifests in this context. Therefore, investigating nomophobia among high school students in Turkey is essential to inform region-specific preventive strategies and educational interventions.
This study examined nomophobia levels among high school students in Turkey, aiming to identify potential differences according to gender, physical activity participation, grade level and duration of smartphone use. The following research questions guided the study:
  • Does the gender variable affect the level of nomophobia?
  • Does the participation in physical activity affect the level of nomophobia?
  • Does the high school type affect the level of nomophobia?
  • Does the grade level affect the level of nomophobia?
  • Does the smartphone usage affect the level of nomophobia?

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Research Design and Participants’ Selection

Relational survey design was used to conduct this study. The study was conducted within the scope of the comparison type relationship model, which is preferred to examine the differences of two or more groups on a specific variable (Creswell, 1994).
The participants include high schools affiliated with the Eskişehir Provincial Directorate of National Education (Turkey), and the sample group consists of individuals studying in high schools affiliated with Eskişehir Central districts (Odunpazarı, Tepebaşı, Turkey). The data used in the study were collected during the 2024–2025 academic year. A total of 884 participants (male = 425, female = 459) from Science High School and Anatolian High School were sampled for the study using multistage sampling procedures (cluster, simple random, quota and purposive). More details on participants’ characteristics are presented in Table 1.

2.2. Measures

In this study, five sociodemographic characteristics of the participants were evaluated: gender, participation in physical activity, school type, grade level and duration of smartphone use. The Nomophobia Scale developed by Yildirim and Correia (2015) was used to measure the nomophobia levels of the participants. This scale was adapted to the Turkish context by Yildirim et al. (2016), and this Turkish version was used in this study. The scale consists of 20 items on a 7-point Likert-type scale with four sub-dimensions: not being able to access information, losing connectedness, not being able to communicate and giving up convenience.
In the validity study conducted by Yildirim et al. (2016), the combined reliability coefficient (Cronbach α) of the whole scale was reported as 0.92. The reliability coefficients of the four sub-dimensions are 0.90, 0.74, 0.94 and 0.91, respectively. In this study, the overall reliability of the scale was found to be 0.82, and the Cronbach α values of the sub-dimensions ranged between 0.78 and 0.86. These findings show that the scale has a satisfactory level of internal consistency across all dimensions. High scores obtained in the measurement tool indicate that individuals have high levels of nomophobia.

2.3. Ethical Statements

The necessary ethics committee permissions were obtained from the Kütahya Dumlupınar University Social and Human Sciences Research and Publication Ethics Committee (Code N°11/2023; dated on 28 November 2023). In addition, the research was carried out in accordance with the latest Helsinki guidelines for ethical principles in research involving human participants (Association, 2025).

2.4. Data Collection Procedures

To collect the research data, official high schools affiliated with the Provincial Directorate of National Education in the Eskişehir city center were visited, and students were asked to fill out the form. Participants were briefed on the purpose of the study, the confidentiality and anonymity of responses, how to fill out the scales and the liberty to withdraw from the study at any time. A total of 995 participants filled out the form. The incomplete or incorrectly completed forms of 86 participants were excluded from the study, and the responses of the remaining 884 participants were prepared for statistical analysis.
High school students between the ages of 14 and 18 who extensively use smartphones—defined as those who have used a smartphone every day for at least the last six months—were included in this survey. Furthermore, participants who understand the content of the survey must provide informed consent; for minors, parental permission must be requested. Study participants excluded were those without smartphones, those not enrolled in school and those with cognitive problems preventing accurate self-reporting. Individuals participating in ongoing digital detox initiatives and inconsistent or partial survey responses were also excluded.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

The data obtained were transferred to the SPSS 24.0 package program, and the Kolmogorov–Smirnov normality test was applied to the data, and although the results obtained did not show a normal distribution, it was accepted that the data had a normal distribution because the skewness and kurtosis values were between −2 and +2 (Kim, 2013). Accordingly, an independent sample test (t-test) and one-way analysis of variance (One-Way ANOVA) tests were applied to the data. Depending on the homogeneity assumption of the data, Welch values were considered, and Tukey tests were applied as second-level post hoc tests (Agbangba et al., 2024; Takane et al., 2025). Additionally, we used RStudio, Version 2025.05.0 for Windows (R Core Team, 2024) including the “ggplot2” (Wickham, 2016) and “dplyr” (Wickham et al., 2023) packages to generate the figure for the summary of the study results. The findings were evaluated according to the p < 0.05 significance value.

3. Results

3.1. Normality Assumption

Skewness and kurtosis values of the sub-dimensions of the measurement tool were between −2 and +2 (Table 2).

3.2. Variation in Nomophobia Levels by Gender

Nomophobia levels of the study participants were analyzed across gender. Statistically significant differences were found across all the sub-dimensions of nomophobia such as the not being able to access information sub-dimension (t(882) = −4.494; p < 0.05), not being able to communicate sub-dimension (t(882) = −8.571; p < 0.05), losing connectedness sub-dimension (t(882) = −8.232; p < 0.05) and giving up convenience sub-dimension (t(882) = −3.643; p < 0.05).
Statistically significant differences were found in nomophobia total scores (t(882) = −8.286; p < 0.05) (see Table 3 and Appendix A for details). Analysis of the mean scores indicated that female participants had higher nomophobia levels than the male participants.

3.3. Nomophobia Level Variations by Physical Activity Participation

This section analyzed nomophobia differences because of participation in physical activities. Statistically significant differences were found in all the sub-dimensions of nomophobia such as inability to access information (t(882) = −0.212; p < 0.05), giving up convenience (t(882) = −1.038; p < 0.05), inability to communicate (t(882) = −2.246; p < 0.05) and losing connectedness (t(882) = −2.121; p < 0.05). Statistically significant differences were found in nomophobia total scores (t(882) = −1.925; p < 0.05) (see Table 4 and Appendix A for details).
According to Table 4, nomophobia levels of individuals who did not participate in physical activity were higher than those of individuals who participated in physical activity in all sub-dimensions and the total score.

3.4. Nomophobia Levels According to High School Type

Nomophobia levels of the participants were analyzed according to the high school type variable. Statistically significant differences were not found in all sub-dimensions of nomophobia (p > 0.05) (see Table 5 and Appendix A for more details).

3.5. Nomophobia Levels According to High School Students’ Grade Level

This section analyzed nomophobia levels of study participants’ grade level. Statistically significant differences were found across all the sub-dimensions of nomophobia such as inability to access information (F(3;883) = 3.077; p < 0.05), inability to communicate (F(3;883) = 3.016; p < 0. 05), losing connectedness (F(3;883) = 3.201; p < 0.05) and giving up convenience (F(3;883) = 3.428; p < 0.05). Statistically significant differences were found in nomophobia total scores (F(3;883) = 3.748; p < 0.05). When the multiple comparison (Tukey) post hoc test was examined, the following grade level differences were identified: (a) level 9 and level 10 on inability to access information; (b) level 11 and level 12 on inability to communicate and losing connectedness; and (c) level 12 and levels 9 and 11 on giving up convenience. When the total scores were analyzed, significant differences were observed in nomophobia levels according to the grade levels of the students. Significant differences were found between 9th grade students and 10th and 12th grade students; between 10th grade students and 11th grade students; and between 11th grade students and 12th grade students. This shows that students’ nomophobia levels change as the grade level increases (see Table 6 and Appendix A).

3.6. Nomophobia Levels by Duration of Smartphone Use

This section looked at the nomophobia levels of the participants based on the duration of smartphone use. While no statistically significant difference was found across the sub-dimensions of inability to access information, inability to communicate and giving up convenience, a statistically significant difference was found in the sub-dimension of losing connectedness (F(3;883) = 2.794; p < 0.05). There were no statistically significant differences in nomophobia total scores (F(3;883) = 1.861; p < 0.05) (see Table 7 and Appendix A).
A multiple comparison test (Tukey post hoc) revealed that a difference between the participants with less than 1 year of smartphone use and those with 5 years or more in the sub-dimension of losing connectedness existed. Hence, the nomophobia levels of study participants who have used mobile phones for a long time are higher than those who have used phones for a short duration.

4. Discussion

The study examined the nomophobia levels of high school students across selected socio-demographic indicators such as gender, participation in physical activity, class level and duration of smartphone use. In this study, statistically significant differences were found in the sub-dimensions of the nomophobia levels across the gender variable. In line with previous studies (e.g., Göktaş & Demirer, 2023), current findings showed that the nomophobia levels of female participants are higher than male participants. In the study conducted by Yılmaz et al. (2018) on nomophobia, the phenomenon became widespread among students in the sub-dimensions of “Losing Communication” and “Inability to Access Information” (Yılmaz et al., 2018). It was also found that the nomophobia levels of women differed significantly from men. It was observed that the nomophobia levels of women in the dimensions of “Losing Communication and Device Deprivation” were significantly higher than men. Society’s emphasis on women’s socialization and communication roles may have triggered women’s dependence on mobile devices.
Participation in physical activity revealed statistically significant differences in all the sub-dimensions of the nomophobia scale. Further comparisons revealed that individuals who participated in physical activity were lower in the sub-dimensions where significant differences were observed than those of individuals who did not participate in physical activity. Atıcı and Erbaş (2021) examined the nomophobia levels of students studying at the faculty of sports sciences during the COVID-19 pandemic process and found that the inability of students to do sports due to restrictions on going out during the pandemic period, social isolation and the closure of sports facilities pushed them to a sedentary life at home (Atıcı & Erbaş, 2021). This situation inevitably directed them to digital tools at home which affected their nomophobia levels. An experimental study by (AlMarzooqi et al., 2022) to determine the effects of sports activities on nomophobia revealed statistically significant differences between pre- and post-test nomophobia intervention in the experimental group. Liu et al. (2022) also reported significant relationships between nomophobia and physical activity (Liu et al., 2022). Physical activity can reduce digital anxiety by supporting individuals’ mental health. Elements such as time management and face-to-face social interactions can balance the dependence on the online world. Given Turkey’s rapidly digitalizing society, these results suggest the need for a strategic approach to reduce digital addiction, especially among the young population, by promoting physical activity. Furthermore, by decreasing in-person social interactions and encouraging digital engagement as the main socialization method, physical inactivity could make nomophobia more severe. Thus, the higher levels of nomophobia among subgroups may stem from adolescent’s developmental need for autonomy and peer approval, which smartphones conveniently fulfill.
There was no statistically significant difference in the sub-dimensions of the nomophobia according to the type of high school study participants attended. This finding corroborates the finding of a similar study by Terzi et al. (2024) on nomophobia perceptions of high school students which did not differ according to the type of school they attended (Terzi et al., 2024). This suggests that the type of high school may not be a determining variable on nomophobia levels and that nomophobia may develop more due to individual or environmental factors.
Regarding the grade level, a statistically significant difference was found among all the nomophobia sub-dimensions. Further comparisons revealed differences among certain grade levels on specific nomophobia sub-dimensions, a finding that is in line with previous studies (Çelebi et al., 2020; Çilek et al., 2024). Both studies found that the nomophobia levels of students differ according to the grade level. Class level differences also characterized nomophobia sub-dimensions of inability to access information and sacrifice of comfort (Aygün et al., 2024). The finding of statistically significant differences in the sub-dimensions of the nomophobia scale according to the grade level of the participants suggests that individuals’ relationships with digital technologies may be related to developmental processes and age. For example, the difference between 9th and 10th grade students in the sub-dimension of inability to access information may be due to the diverse needs or expectations of students in this age group to access information. Similarly, the differences between 11th and 12th graders in the sub-dimensions of not being able to communicate and losing online connection may be influenced by the increased social responsibilities and communication needs of higher-grade students. The fact that 12th graders did not differ in the sacrifice of convenience dimension suggests that this group’s attitudes towards technology may be more established and stable. These findings suggest that grade level is a key factor that can affect students’ behaviors and perceptions towards technology use. Moreover, different academic expectations and changing social dynamics may lead to differences in nomophobia across grades. For example, older students may use their phones for stress management or to interact with friends in increasingly complicated social networks.
Depending on the duration of the participants’ smartphone usage, no significant difference was found in the sub-dimensions of not being able to access information, not being able to communicate and giving up convenience. However, a statistically significant difference was found in the sub-dimension of losing connectedness. The multiple comparison (Tukey) test showed a significant difference in the sub-dimension of losing connectedness between participants with less than 1 year of smartphone usage and participants with 5 years or more. This finding supports an earlier study by (Arslan et al., 2019). Arslan et al. found significant differences among teachers’ mobile phone deprivation. The nomophobia levels of the participants who used the devices for 1–3 h, 4–7 h and 7 h or more were higher than those of the participants who used the devices for less than 1 h. In the study conducted by Sırakaya (2018), it was concluded that the increase in years of smartphone use, years of mobile internet use and daily mobile internet usage time significantly differentiated nomophobia levels (Sırakaya, 2018). Contrary to the above findings, Adnan and Gezgin (2016) did not find any significant difference between the nomophobia levels of students on mobile phone usage time (Adnan & Gezgin, 2016). Though the research included Turkish students, variations in findings could stem from variations in sample size, geographical or institutional settings, or the method of measuring cell phone use and nomophobia.

4.1. Strengths and Limitations

Given the considerable sample size, this study offers insights into nomophobia levels among Turkish students, hence supporting the validity of the statistical analysis and allowing significant sub-group comparisons. In addition, including several sub-dimensions of nomophobia helps to clarify this condition as well. Conversely, the research has certain limitations. First, it was based on a single sample from a specific community which restricts the generalizability of the findings to broader or diverse populations. Second, the cross-sectional design of the study could restrict any causal conclusion, either directly or indirectly.

4.2. Practical Implications

Sports or club activities organized in schools can be an effective tool in reducing the effect of physical activity on nomophobia levels. School authorities of Science High and Anatolian High schools should from time to time organize physical education, cultural and other co-curricular activities to engage the students during and after school hours. Considering that female students exhibit higher levels of nomophobia, training programs on digital awareness and healthy technology use can be organized for this cohort. During these training programs, the benefits and harms of technology can be addressed in a balanced manner. School authorities can also educate students on the causes and effects of nomophobia and institute remedial measures to curb that anxiety. Observing differences by grade level requires the development of strategies specific to age groups. For example, workshops on stress management and technology use can be organized for upper-grade students. This approach will equip them with the requisite knowledge on how to manage anxious moments of phone unavailability or usage. Considering the effect of duration of smartphone use on the sub-dimension of losing online connection, individual or group counseling sessions can be organized by telecommunication agencies and school authorities to assess long-term users’ relationship with technology. The lack of significant differences according to high school type suggests further studies to examine the effect of cultural and environmental factors on nomophobia in more detail.

5. Conclusions

The analyses of nomophobia levels revealed that digital addiction status of individuals showed significant differences according to various demographic and behavioral characteristics. The gender findings show that female individuals exhibit significantly higher addiction tendency in all sub-dimensions and total nomophobia scores compared to males. This situation emphasizes that addiction to digital technologies differs according to gender and the importance of developing policies and programs to ensure digital balance, especially for female users.
For physical activity participation, movement-based lifestyles may have a reducing effect on nomophobia levels. It was observed that individuals who did not participate in physical activity were more dependent on digital connectivity, which revealed that physical activities should be supported in improving technology use habits. In contrast, there was no significant effect of school type on nomophobia levels, suggesting that digital addiction is more closely related to individual and environmental factors.
Evaluations by grade level revealed that nomophobia levels were higher especially among 11th grade students, revealing the effect of age and developmental periods on digital addiction. Accordingly, awareness and intervention programs should be differentiated according to age groups and educational levels. Regarding the duration of smartphone use, a significant difference was found only in the “losing connectedness” sub-dimension; the fact that long-term users had higher nomophobia levels in this dimension showed that the duration of use should be considered when combating technology addiction. All these findings show that nomophobia is a multifaceted phenomenon and that combating strategies should be shaped according to the individual’s lifestyle, developmental period and behavioral characteristics.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, P.Ç. and İ.G.; methodology, P.Ç. and İ.G.; formal analysis, resources, P.Ç., İ.G. and A.T.; data curation, P.Ç. and İ.G.; writing—original draft preparation, P.Ç., İ.G., A.T. and M.A.; writing—review and editing, P.Ç., İ.G., A.T., M.A., M.S.-S., J.E.H. and O.Y.; supervision, O.D. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study received no external funding. However, the authors sincerely thank Bielefeld University, Germany, for providing financial support through the Institutional Open Access Publication Fund for the article-processing charge (APC).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The necessary ethics committee permissions were obtained from the Kütahya Dumlupınar University Social and Human Sciences Research and Publication Ethics Committee (Code N°11/2023; dated on 28 November 2023). In addition, the research was carried out in accordance with the latest Helsinki guidelines (2025) for ethical principles in research involving human participants.

Informed Consent Statement

An informed consent form was received and completed by each participant.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article. Further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

All the students who participated in the study are appreciated by the authors.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Appendix A

Figure A1. Nomophobia sub-dimension scores by demographic categories.
Figure A1. Nomophobia sub-dimension scores by demographic categories.
Youth 05 00078 g0a1

References

  1. Adnan, M., & Gezgin, D. M. (2016). A modern phobia: Prevalence of nomophobia among college students. Ankara University Journal of Faculty of Educational Sciences (JFES), 49(1), 141–158. [Google Scholar]
  2. Agbangba, C. E., Aide, E. S., Honfo, H., & Kakai, R. G. (2024). On the use of post-hoc tests in environmental and biological sciences: A critical review. Heliyon, 10(3), e25131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Al Battashi, N., Al Omari, O., Sawalha, M., Al Maktoumi, S., Alsuleitini, A., & Al Qadire, M. (2021). The relationship between smartphone use, insomnia, stress, and anxiety among university students: A cross-sectional study. Clinical Nursing Research, 30(6), 734–740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. AlMarzooqi, M. A., Alhaj, O. A., Alrasheed, M. M., Helmy, M., Trabelsi, K., Ebrahim, A., Hattab, S., Jahrami, H. A., & Ben Saad, H. (2022). Symptoms of nomophobia, psychological aspects, ınsomnia and physical activity: A cross-sectional study of ESports Players in Saudi Arabia. Healthcare, 10(2), 257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Arslan, H., Tozkoparan, S. B., & Kurt, A. A. (2019). Öğretmenlerde mobil telefon yoksunluğu korkusunun ve gelişmeleri kaçırma korkusunun incelenmesi. Erzincan Üniversitesi Eğitim Fakültesi Dergisi, 21(3), 237–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Association, W. M. (2025). World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: Ethical principles for medical research involving human participants. JAMA, 333(1), 71–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Atıcı, A. R., & Erbaş, M. K. (2021). COVİD-19 sürecinde spor bilimleri fakültesi öğrencilerinin nomofobi düzeylerinin incelenmesi. Available online: https://acikerisim.aksaray.edu.tr/items/bfc93b80-3e8c-4269-b497-91532a160d70 (accessed on 10 April 2025).
  8. Aygün, A., Ulu, E., & Akça, R. P. (2024). Nomofobi Üzerine Bir Araştırma: Lise Öğrencileri Örneği. Socıal Scıences Studıes Journal (SSSJournal), 9(116), 8649–8656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Black, R., Walsh, L., Waite, C., Collin, P., Third, A., & Idriss, S. (2022). In their own words: 41 stories of young people’s digital citizenship. Learning, Media and Technology, 47(4), 524–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Bragazzi, N. L., & Del Puente, G. (2014). A proposal for including nomophobia in the new DSM-V. Psychology Research and Behavior Management, 7, 155–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Brailovskaia, J., Delveaux, J., John, J., Wicker, V., Noveski, A., Kim, S., Schillack, H., & Margraf, J. (2023). Finding the “sweet spot” of smartphone use: Reduction or abstinence to increase well-being and healthy lifestyle?! An experimental intervention study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 29(1), 149–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Buke, M., Egesoy, H., & Unver, F. (2021). The effect of smartphone addiction on physical activity level in sports science undergraduates. Journal of Bodywork and Movement Therapies, 28, 530–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Cerisola, A. (2017). Impacto negativo de los medios tecnológicos en el neurodesarrollo infantil. Pediátr Panamá, 46, 126–131. [Google Scholar]
  14. Creswell, J. W. (1994). Research design: Qualitative & quantitative approaches. Sage Publications, Inc. [Google Scholar]
  15. Çelebi, M., Metin, A., İncedere, F., Aygün, N., Bedir, M., & Özbulut, Ö. (2020). Investigation of relationship between nomophobia and loneliness level: Erciyes university sample. Sciences, 10(2), 315–334. [Google Scholar]
  16. Çilek, A., Özgüray, Ö., Demircan, S., Özgüray, A., & Demircan, N. (2024). Farklı kademelerde öğrenim gören öğrencilerin nomofobi düzeyleri. Academıc Socıal Resources Journal, 8(47), 2314–2333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Daraj, L. R., AlGhareeb, M., Almutawa, Y. M., Trabelsi, K., & Jahrami, H. (2023). Systematic review and meta-analysis of the correlation coefficients between nomophobia and anxiety, smartphone addiction, and insomnia symptoms. Healthcare, 11, 2066. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Durak, H. Y. (2019). Investigation of nomophobia and smartphone addiction predictors among adolescents in Turkey: Demographic variables and academic performance. The Social Science Journal, 56(4), 492–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Dursun, B., Gerçek, H., & Torlak, M. S. (2024). Üniversite Öğrencilerinde Fiziksel Aktivite Düzeyi ile Nomofobi ve İnternet Bağımlılığı Arasındaki İlişki. Başkent Üniversitesi Sağlık Bilimleri Fakültesi Dergisi, 9(2), 223–235. [Google Scholar]
  20. Dwyer, R. J., Zhuo, A. X., & Dunn, E. W. (2023). Why do people turn to smartphones during social interactions? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 109, 104506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Geçgel, H. (2020). Investigation of smartphone addiction levels of Turkish pre-service teachers with regards to various variables. Journal of Language and Linguistic Studies, 16(3), 1415–1442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Gonçalves, S., Dias, P., & Correia, A.-P. (2020). Nomophobia and lifestyle: Smartphone use and its relationship to psychopathologies. Computers in Human Behavior Reports, 2, 100025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Göktaş, H., & Demirer, V. (2023). LİSE ÖĞRENCİLERİNİN NOMOFOBİ, AKILLI TELEFON BAĞIMLILIĞI VE AKADEMİK BAŞARI DÜZEYLERİ ARASINDAKİ İLİŞKİ. Eğitim Teknolojisi Kuram ve Uygulama, 13(1), 209–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Greenfield, D. N. (1999). Psychological characteristics of compulsive Internet use: A preliminary analysis. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 2(5), 403–412. [Google Scholar]
  25. Hällgren, C., & Björk, Å. (2022). Young people’s identities in digital worlds. The International Journal of Information and Learning Technology, 40(1), 49–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Kardos, P., Unoka, Z., Pléh, C., & Soltész, P. (2018). Your mobile phone indeed means your social network: Priming mobile phone activates relationship related concepts. Computers in Human Behavior, 88, 84–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Kim, H. Y. (2013). Statistical notes for clinical researchers: Assessing normal distribution (2) using skewness and kurtosis. Restorative Dentistry & Endodontics, 38(1), 52–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Lee, Y.-K., Chang, C.-T., Lin, Y., & Cheng, Z.-H. (2014). The dark side of smartphone usage: Psychological traits, compulsive behavior and technostress. Computers in Human Behavior, 31, 373–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Lepp, A., Barkley, J. E., & Karpinski, A. C. (2014). The relationship between cell phone use, academic performance, anxiety, and Satisfaction with Life in college students. Computers in Human Behavior, 31, 343–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Le Steunf, A., Page, E., Guillodo, Y., & Saraux, A. (2024). Does reducing smartphone use impact physical activity? PLoS ONE, 19(10), e0311248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Liébana-Cabanillas, F., Sánchez-Fernández, J., & Muñoz-Leiva, F. (2014). Antecedents of the adoption of the new mobile payment systems: The moderating effect of age. Computers in Human Behavior, 35, 464–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Lin, Y.-C., & Huang, P.-C. (2025). Digital traps: How technology fuels nomophobia and insomnia in Taiwanese college students. Acta Psychologica, 252, 104674. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Liu, W., Chen, J.-S., Gan, W. Y., Poon, W. C., Tung, S. E. H., Lee, L. J., Xu, P., Chen, I.-H., Griffiths, M. D., & Lin, C.-Y. (2022). Associations of problematic ınternet use, weight-related self-stigma, and nomophobia with physical activity: Findings from Mainland China, Taiwan, and Malaysia. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(19), 12135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Prasad, M., Patthi, B., Singla, A., Gupta, R., Saha, S., Kumar, J. K., Malhi, R., & Pandita, V. (2017). Nomophobia: A cross-sectional study to assess mobile phone usage among dental students. Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research: JCDR, 11(2), ZC34–ZC39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Precht, L.-M., Mertens, F., Brickau, D. S., Kramm, R. J., Margraf, J., Stirnberg, J., & Brailovskaia, J. (2024). Engaging in physical activity instead of (over)using the smartphone: An experimental investigation of lifestyle interventions to prevent problematic smartphone use and to promote mental health. Journal of Public Health, 32(4), 589–607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. R Core Team. (2024). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 5 May 2025).
  37. Ruiz-Ruano, A. M., López-Salmerón, M. D., & Puga, J. L. (2020). Evitación experiencial y uso abusivo del smartphone: Un enfoque bayesiano. [Experiential avoidance and excessive smartphone use: A Bayesian approach.]. Adicciones, 32(2), 116–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Salmela-Aro, K., & Motti-Stefanidi, F. (2022). Digital revolution and youth: Consequences for their development and education (Vol. 27). Hogrefe Publishing. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Sırakaya, M. (2018). Ön Lisans Öğrencilerinin Nomofobi Düzeylerinin Akıllı Telefon Kullanım Durumlarına Göre İncelenmesi. Mersin Üniversitesi Eğitim Fakültesi Dergisi, 14(2), 714–727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Srivastava, L. (2005). Mobile phones and the evolution of social behaviour. Behaviour & Information Technology, 24(2), 111–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Takane, Y., Beh, E. J., & Lombardo, R. (2025). A theory of contrasts for modified Freeman–Tukey statistics and its applications to Tukey’s post-hoc tests for contingency tables. Computational Statistics, 40(3), 1423–1446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Terzi, H., Ayaz-Alkaya, S., & Köse-Kabakcıoğlu, N. (2024). Nomophobia and eHealth literacy among adolescents: A cross-sectional study. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 75, 158–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Wickham, H. (2016). ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis. Springer. Available online: https://ggplot2.tidyverse.org/ (accessed on 5 May 2025).
  44. Wickham, H., François, R., Henry, L., Müller, K., & Vaughan, D. (2023). dplyr: A grammar of data manipulation, (R package version 1.1.3); Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=dplyr (accessed on 5 May 2025).
  45. Yildirim, C., & Correia, A.-P. (2015). Exploring the dimensions of nomophobia: Development and validation of a self-reported questionnaire. Computers in Human Behavior, 49, 130–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Yildirim, C., Sumuer, E., Adnan, M., & Yildirim, S. (2016). A growing fear:Prevalence of nomophobia among Turkish college students. Information Development, 32(5), 1322–1331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Yılmaz, M., Köse, A., & Doğru, Y. B. (2018). Akıllı Telefondan Yoksun Kalmak: Nomofobi Üzerine Bir Araştırma [Staying away from the smart phone: A research on nomophobia]. AJIT-e: Academic Journal of Information Technology, 9(35), 31–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Distribution of demographic information of the participants.
Table 1. Distribution of demographic information of the participants.
Variable N%
GenderMale42548.1
Female45951.9
Participation in Physical ActivityYes37142.0
No51358.0
High School TypeScience High School38843.9
Anatolian High School49656.1
Grade Level9. class25028.3
10. class20523.2
11. class20923.6
12. class22024.9
Smartphone Usage TimeLess than 1 year222.5
1–2 years879.8
3–4 years29633.5
5 years and above47954.2
Table 2. Descriptive measures and univariate normality.
Table 2. Descriptive measures and univariate normality.
Sub-DimensionMeanSdSkewnessKurtosis
Participation in physical activity1.580.494−0.326−1.136
High school type1.560.496−0.247−0.944
Grade level2.451.140.052−1.420
Smartphone usage time3.390.7650.4310.164
Not being able to access information15.75.84−0.074−0.760
Not being able to communicate25.66.40−0.336−0.733
Losing connectedness18.66.920.146−0.639
Giving up convenience13.06.880.8210.095
Nomophobia total72.922.90.077−0.333
Table 3. Nomophobia levels according to gender.
Table 3. Nomophobia levels according to gender.
Sub-DimensionGenderNX ± SStp
Not being able to access informationMale42514.8 ± 5.97−4.4940.00 *
Female45916.6 ± 5.59
Not being able to communicateMale42522.9 ± 9.19−8.5710.00 *
Female45928.1 ± 8.90
Losing connectednessMale42516.7 ± 6.52−8.2320.00 *
Female45920.4 ± 6.80
Giving up convenienceMale42512.1 ± 6.82−3.6430.00 *
Female45913.8 ± 6.85
Nomophobia totalMale42566.5 ± 22.2−8.2860.00 *
Female45979.0 ± 21.9
* p < 0.05.
Table 4. Nomophobia levels by physical activity participation.
Table 4. Nomophobia levels by physical activity participation.
Sub-DimensionParticipation in
Physical Activity
NX ± SStp
Not being able to access informationYes37115.7 ± 5.83−0.2120.032 *
No51316.8 ± 5.84
Not being able to communicateYes37124.7 ± 9.44−2.2460.025 *
No51327.2 ± 9.34
Losing connectednessYes37118.0 ± 6.75−2.1210.034 *
No51320.0 ± 7.01
Giving up convenienceYes37112.7 ± 6.88−1.0380.017 *
No51314.2 ± 6.89
Nomophobia totalYes37171.2 ± 23.3−1.9250.045 *
No51378.2 ± 22.6
* p < 0.05.
Table 5. t-test results according to the high school variable.
Table 5. t-test results according to the high school variable.
Sub-DimensionHigh School TypeNX ± SStp
Not being able to access informationScience High School38815.7 ± 5.72−0.1660.408
Anatolian High School49615.8 ± 5.93
Not being able to communicateScience High School38825.3 ± 9.64−0.6470.312
Anatolian High School49625.7 ± 9.21
Losing connectednessScience High School38818.67 ± 7.110.0990.518
Anatolian High School49618.62 ± 6.77
Giving up convenienceScience High School38813.01 ± 6.97−0.1050.226
Anatolian High School49613.06 ± 6.83
Nomophobia totalScience High School38872.8 ± 24.0−0.3090.760
Anatolian High School49673.7 ± 22.1
Table 6. Nomophobia levels according to the students’ grade level.
Table 6. Nomophobia levels according to the students’ grade level.
Sub-DimensionGrade LevelNX ± SSFpTukey
Not being able to access information9. class25015.6 ± 5.873.0770.027 *b–c
10. class20514.8 ± 5.49
11. class20916.5 ± 6.08
12. class22016.0 ± 5.80
Not being able to communicate9. class25025.8 ± 9.603.0160.029 *c–d
10. class20525.0 ± 9.66
11. class20927.1 ± 8.43
12. class22024.5 ± 9.67
Losing connectedness9. class25018.6 ± 6.713.2010.023 *c–d
10. class20518.3 ± 7.09
11. class20919.8 ± 6.73
12. class22017.8 ± 7.05
Giving up convenience9. class25013.8 ± 7.073.4280.017 *a–d
c–d
10. class20512.8 ± 6.71
11. class20913.4 ± 6.83
12. class22011.9 ± 6.77
Nomophobia total9. class25074.0 ± 22.83.7480.011 *a–b
a–d
b–c
c–d
10. class20571.0 ± 23.1
11. class20976.9 ± 22.1
12. class22070.3 ± 23.2
* p < 0.05. (a) 9. class. (b) 10. class. (c) 11. class. (d) 12. class.
Table 7. Nomophobia levels by duration of smartphone use.
Table 7. Nomophobia levels by duration of smartphone use.
Sub-DimensionSmartphone Usage TimeNX ± SSFpTukey
Not being able to access informationLess than 1 year2214.5 ± 6.781.5320.205
1–2 years8715.12 ± 5.78
3–4 years29615.4 ± 5.72
5 years and above47916.13 ± 5.86
Not being able to communicateLess than 1 year2221.3 ± 12.32.1120.097
1–2 years8724.5 ± 9.61
3–4 years29625.9 ± 9.29
5 years and above47925.7 ± 9.25
Losing connectednessLess than 1 year2215.0 ± 8.792.7940.039 *a–d
1–2 years8717.7 ± 6.63
3–4 years29618.82 ± 6.71
5 years and above47918.87 ± 6.96
Giving up ConvenienceLess than 1 year2212.1 ± 7.580.2090.890
1–2 years8713.5 ± 7.14
3–4 years29613.2 ± 6.94
5 years and above47913.0 ± 6.79
Nomophobia TotalLess than 1 year2262.9 ± 31.71.8610.135
1–2 years8770.8 ± 23.3
3–4 years29673.3 ± 22.5
5 years and above47973.7 ± 22.9
* p < 0.05. Less than 1 year (a), 1–2 years (b), 3–4 years (c), 5 years and above (d).
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Çakto, P.; Görgüt, İ.; Tannoubi, A.; Agyei, M.; Srem-Sai, M.; Hagan, J.E.; Yüksel, O.; Demir, O. Nomophobia Levels in Turkish High School Students: Variations by Gender, Physical Activity, Grade Level and Smartphone Use. Youth 2025, 5, 78. https://doi.org/10.3390/youth5030078

AMA Style

Çakto P, Görgüt İ, Tannoubi A, Agyei M, Srem-Sai M, Hagan JE, Yüksel O, Demir O. Nomophobia Levels in Turkish High School Students: Variations by Gender, Physical Activity, Grade Level and Smartphone Use. Youth. 2025; 5(3):78. https://doi.org/10.3390/youth5030078

Chicago/Turabian Style

Çakto, Piyami, İlyas Görgüt, Amayra Tannoubi, Michael Agyei, Medina Srem-Sai, John Elvis Hagan, Oğuzhan Yüksel, and Orhan Demir. 2025. "Nomophobia Levels in Turkish High School Students: Variations by Gender, Physical Activity, Grade Level and Smartphone Use" Youth 5, no. 3: 78. https://doi.org/10.3390/youth5030078

APA Style

Çakto, P., Görgüt, İ., Tannoubi, A., Agyei, M., Srem-Sai, M., Hagan, J. E., Yüksel, O., & Demir, O. (2025). Nomophobia Levels in Turkish High School Students: Variations by Gender, Physical Activity, Grade Level and Smartphone Use. Youth, 5(3), 78. https://doi.org/10.3390/youth5030078

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop