Next Article in Journal
“To Show That There Is Hope”: A Mixed Methods Exploration of Parent Perspectives About an Online, Co-Facilitated Parenting Program for Carers of Adolescents with Disability
Previous Article in Journal
The Association between ADHD Symptoms and Antisocial Behavior: Differential Effects of Maternal and Paternal Parenting Behaviors
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Interparental and Parent–Teen Relationships during Adolescence as Predictors of Intra- and Interpersonal Emotion Regulation in Young Adulthood

1
Department of Counseling, Higher Education, and Special Education, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, USA
2
Department of Graduate Psychology, James Madison University, Harrisonburg, VA 22807, USA
3
Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22904, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Youth 2024, 4(4), 1417-1436; https://doi.org/10.3390/youth4040090
Submission received: 19 July 2024 / Revised: 12 September 2024 / Accepted: 24 September 2024 / Published: 3 October 2024

Abstract

:
Parents’ contributions to their children’s emotion regulation during adolescence has been a relatively understudied interpersonal context of development, even though parents’ roles as sources of social and emotional learning persist from childhood into adolescence and the complexity of teens’ lives grows during this time. This study aims to investigate the differential predictive utility of qualities and behaviors in interparental and parent–teen relationships during adolescence for predicting youths’ development of intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation over a 13-year period. To assess these hypotheses, data were obtained from a longitudinal, multi-method, multi-informant study of 184 adolescents (107 Caucasian, 53 African American, and 24 mixed/other ethnicity; median family income of USD 40,000–60,000/year in 1999, equivalent to about USD 75,000–112,000/year when accounting for inflation) and their parents. The results provide support for a differential pattern of prediction; qualities of interparental relationships in early adolescence were significant predictors of young adult interpersonal emotion regulation, whereas behaviors in interparental and parent–teen relationships in late adolescence were significant predictors of both young adult positive intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation. Notably, some father-reported relationship predictors during late adolescence had unexpected relations with later intrapersonal emotion regulation. The results are discussed in terms of the helpfulness of these specific relationship factors during each part of adolescence for supporting positive intra- and interpersonal emotional regulation development.

1. Introduction

Emotion regulation plays an essential role amid the demands of everyday life. Emotion regulation allows individuals to manage cognitive and physiological responses that can be triggered by both extraordinary and routine stressors [1,2]. Without the ability to moderate intense emotional responses, acute emotional experiences can be damaging to individuals’ physical and mental health and the quality of their relationships [3,4,5]. Much of the emotion regulation literature has assessed only intrapersonal emotion regulation, that is, how individuals regulate their own emotions without the aid of others, such as cognitive reappraisal [6,7,8]. Though most emotion regulation occurs in social contexts [9], when social interactions are purposefully aimed at the management of affective states, they may be described by the term interpersonal emotion regulation (IER) [8].
Much of the research on IER has been conducted with adults and young children. It should be noted that the age estimates that relate to distinct developmental periods often differ in various sources. Childhood may include ages ranging 3 to 11 years old in some sources, wherein ages of 3 to 8 years old would be considered early childhood and ages of 9 to 11 years old are considered late childhood, and there are slightly different age ranges in other studies [10]. The World Health Organization identifies the period of adolescence as beginning at age 10 years and ending at age 19 years [11]. This range aligns with Britannica’s definition of adulthood, which they describe as beginning at age 20 or 21 years, within which the Journal of Adolescent Health suggests that young adulthood exists, including ages 18 to 25 years old [12,13]. Within the adult population, some of the most profound contributions have been made with regard to coregulation and IER in romantic relationships [14,15]. However, while plenty of research has also examined emotion regulation with children and some with adolescent populations, this research often does not consider the intricacies of emotion regulation in an interpersonal context during these periods. It is also clear that, beginning in infancy, individuals learn how to regulate their emotions interpersonally through coregulation with their caregiver, defined as a shared emotional system between individuals, which allows for dyadic oscillation between affective states [16]. This early coregulation, when developed appropriately, also facilitates the creation of a secure attachment relationship between caregivers and their children [17]. An abundance of literature suggests that caregivers’ extrinsic IER toward their children in moments of intense emotion guides children’s own intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation use [18,19,20]. This direct engagement with children in periods of high emotion, termed emotion coaching, is the primary and most critical way that younger children learn to regulate emotions [21]. As children mature, however, they develop more complex cognitive and language skills, and these increased abilities in communication and cognition have implications for their intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation skills as they are able to understand and communicate their feelings more effectively [22,23]. Thus, these studies, and several others, indicate that the nature of emotion regulation, both in its application within the caregiver–child relationship and in general abilities, likely changes throughout development [24].
Still, the need to regulate emotional experiences is pervasive and vital across the human lifespan, and IER represents an essential component of emotional development. However, research on the interpersonal nature of emotion regulation development—in terms of both interpersonal processes and outcomes—has been infrequently extended into adolescence. The lack of research conducted during this developmental period may be a critical oversight, as both intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation skills are especially important during adolescence when youth are tasked with more complicated developmental tasks [25,26]. The current paper aims to explore this notion, focusing on the contribution of IER in caregiver relationships with offspring during adolescence to the development of their intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation in adulthood. A longitudinal, multi-method, multi-informant study of 184 adolescents and their parents, taking place in the Southeastern United States, provides data from three waves of collection across a 13-year period, representing early and late adolescence and young adulthood. Data from the first two waves of data collection will be compared for their utility in predicting intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation in adulthood.

2. Interpersonal Emotion Regulation in Adolescence

For 10- to 13-year-olds, the transition into adolescence, often referred to as its own period of early adolescence, involves increasingly complicated developmental tasks, including burgeoning romantic relationships, added responsibilities at home and in school, increasingly important social relationships, and newly influential social media [25,27]. Interpersonal emotion regulation skills are undoubtedly key for successfully navigating these complicated tasks and the distress that may sometimes accompany them. Furthermore, a review by Barthel and colleagues [28] suggests that use of IER facilitates greater closeness in relationships, contributes to an overall more positive affect, and further promotes the development of the intrapersonal emotion regulation skills. However, of the studies reviewed by Barthel and colleagues, few examine the effects of IER use specifically during adolescence, and none of the adolescent literature reviewed by these authors utilizes a longitudinal design [28].
Significant neurological development also promotes the use of intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation skills during adolescence. The physical and cognitive growth that individuals undergo when entering adolescence is consequential to practically all aspects of life [29]. During the transition to and throughout adolescence, the development of the prefrontal cortex enhances individuals’ capacity for higher-order processes that facilitate the regulation of emotions like cognitive control, executive functioning, and selective attention [30,31,32]. Silvers suggests that the salient cognitive maturation that takes place during this period also renders adolescent emotion regulation development particularly sensitive to external influences [31].
Although peers take on increased importance from middle to late adolescence, defined as ages 14 to 17 years and ages 18 to 21 years, respectively, teens across adolescence still look to their parents for emotion regulation to soothe the strong emotions associated with navigating the challenges associated with these relationships [26]. Accordingly, IER with parents at this time often occurs before or after an emotional event as opposed to the more immediate responses received in childhood [24]. Parents often engage in extrinsic IER with their teens by offering advice, which prompts adolescents to seek or avoid situations in which positive or negative emotions may be elicited (i.e., situation selection), or by soothing their teens in the aftermath of strong emotional events (i.e., response modulation). Though less common in adolescence, IER may still occur in the form of situation modification when parents or teens express their emotions to one another in the midst of an emotionally fraught situation.
However, different strategies for teaching children about emotion regulation become relevant in adolescence as compared with early childhood. Whereas emotion coaching was vital during childhood and early adolescence for children to learn to self-regulate, middle and late adolescents may also benefit from indirect teaching of specific emotion regulation strategies through modeling [33]. As teens mature into late adolescents and become more autonomous within and attuned to their social world, they are more likely to observe, understand, and learn from their parents’ relationship with one another, as well as from their specific relationship with each parent. Thus, understanding the nature of these interpersonal contexts during adolescence may be an important consideration for longitudinal emotion regulation research [26]. Not only do parent relationships provide a safe place for individuals to practice IER, but interparental relationships and interactions have been shown to have impacts on individuals’ later emotion regulation [34].
In their review, Silvers identifies a gap in the literature related to the social context of emotion regulation in adolescence, a period the author describes as socially rich [31]. Although research has extensively examined caregiver-led emotion regulation interventions with children and although there is a burgeoning body of research that has examined peer relationships in young adulthood as being valuable for emotion regulation development, the author suggests that there has been little research examining the social regulation of emotion in adolescence [31]. Silvers indicates that researchers should consider caregivers and friends as potential conduits for emotion regulation interventions in adolescence [31].

3. Parental Relationships as Predictors of Later Interpersonal Emotion Regulation

Morris and colleagues introduced and subsequently tested the tripartite model to describe the ways in which parents and families contribute to child and adolescent development of emotion regulation [35,36]. The authors posit three mechanisms through which children and adolescents learn about emotions: observation, emotion-related behaviors, and the family emotional climate. Specifically, children learn about emotions by observing emotion-related behaviors modeled to them by their parents (e.g., parent expressivity, emotion regulation strategies), based on the emotion-specific parenting behaviors of which they are on the receiving end (e.g., emotion coaching, reactions to children’s emotions, teaching emotion regulation strategies), and in the context of the family emotional climate, which is itself affected by parenting style, parent–child attachment, and interparental relations [35].
Importantly, while the parent–teen relationship remains incredibly influential for emotion regulation development through the family emotional climate, the parents’ relationship with one another may also prove to be important. Both represent formative experiences for adolescents, shaping what children learn from their parents and the context in which they learn it. Davies and Cummings’ emotional security hypothesis provides some evidence in support of the effect of interparental relationships on adolescent emotional development, proposing that these relationships also influence children’s sense of emotional security [37]. Specifically, the authors theorize that some forms of family conflict threaten children’s sense of emotional security, depending on the meaning of the conflict for family relations. Davies and Cummings’s emotional security hypothesis later garnered support in the literature; in multiple studies, children’s emotional security about interparental conflict has been seen to be related to their psychological [37,38,39]. The apparent relationship between parents’ relations with one another and children’s and teens’ emotional security, as evidenced by the emotional security hypothesis, suggests a link between parents’ social-emotional behaviors and their offsprings’ intrapersonal emotional development. Given the relational nature of these predictors, it is likely that there would exist a more direct link to behaviors in the interparental relationship and their children’s use of interpersonal emotion regulation strategies, as IER represents the fusion of social and emotional processes.
Teens in particular benefit greatly from learning how to engage in emotion regulation behaviors by observing how others engage in such behaviors. Modeling proves to be incredibly influential in teens’ development of emotion regulation strategies [36]. The context in which parents model emotion regulation strategies includes intrapersonally in distressing situations and interpersonally in interactions between parents and between parents and teens. The examples that parents provide in these circumstances may be valuable predictors of teens’ later emotion regulation skills. Though researchers have found evidence to suggest that adults’ retrospective reports regarding their childhood experience of their parents’ socialization of emotion, including discussion of and responses to their emotions, predicted their current emotion regulation skills [40], few studies have been conducted examining the longitudinal effects of parent emotion socialization during adolescence.
The tripartite model provides a structure for understanding the effects of family environment and parental relationships on the development of intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation. It describes these relationships as both a source of learning about emotions and relationships and as the context in which individuals develop the skills to regulate their emotions. Regardless of whether a factor is directly teaching individuals about emotions and relationships, however, through both observation and through the family environment, children and adolescents’ abilities to regulate their emotions are shaped by family experiences. The tripartite model represents just one of many ways in which these specific factors in parents’ relationships have been categorized in the literature. However, the current paper intends on classifying these factors, as defined by current models, more broadly in terms of relationship qualities versus behaviors. Though the study of relationship qualities and behaviors is not new, research has yet to describe parent relational factors in this manner, nor have they compared them so directly.

Parent Relationship Qualities versus Behaviors

Attachment theorists have suggested that caregivers impact their children and teens’ emotional development through two systems: by functioning as a secure base and as a safe haven [41,42]. By serving as a secure base for children and teens, parents provide indirect guidance and encouragement to support their children’s exploration of unfamiliar environments and developmental tasks [24]. When functioning as a safe haven, on the other hand, parents provide more direct guidance for their children through extrinsic IER in times of distress [24]. These descriptions of the ways in which parenting impacts children’s emotional development provide an interesting framework for considering how relationship qualities and behaviors differentially impact emotion regulation development.
First, it is important to define exactly what is meant by qualities and behaviors. Simply put, relationship qualities are states of being and feeling in relationships, or the salient, distinctive characteristics of a parent’s relationship. Similar to the secure base described in attachment theory [41,42], relationship qualities provide a safe place for teens to seek intrinsic IER, as defined by Zaki and Williams, after engaging in activating, unfamiliar experiences [8]. Some examples of relationship qualities include warmth, parenting style, attachment style, general conflictual relations, and the emotional climate of a family. In the parent–child relationship, prominent developmental theories like attachment theory indicate that qualities of warmth contribute to positive emotional outcomes for children. In the interparental relationship, Davies and Cummings’ process model provides support for the association between marital conflict and satisfaction and emotion regulation, though this research has been conducted less frequently in adolescence [37]. Thus, positive or negative qualities in the family relationships may impact adolescents’ sense of emotional security in the family environment and, subsequently, their willingness to seek intrinsic IER from their parents [42].
Behaviors, on the other hand, are defined as what is performed in a relationship, or the specific actions one takes at times in their relationships. Likened to the safe haven described by [24], parental behaviors impact teens’ emotional development directly in the form of response modulation when teens are experiencing strong emotions and by modeling to teens how to engage in intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation, such as the strategies outlined in the extended process model of emotion regulation [43]. Examples of behaviors in parent relationships include extrinsic and intrinsic IER like emotional self-disclosure, emotional coaching, and spending quality time with loved ones. Specific behaviors modeled by parents to children are one of three ways in which Morris and colleagues discuss that children and adolescents learn about emotions [36]. In the parent–teen relationship, parents’ discussion of emotions, including emotion coaching and parents’ own negative emotional expression, has been associated with fewer internalizing symptoms and more internalizing and externalizing symptoms [44]. Regarding behaviors within families and between parents, cohesion in families during adolescence has been associated with various self-regulatory outcomes [45,46].
It is likely that relationship qualities and behaviors differentially predict emotion regulation depending on which end of adolescence which they fall (i.e., early or late), though this has yet to be explored. Parent–child relationships during early adolescence are characterized by increased conflict with and decreased reliance on parents [35]. Positive qualities in parent relationships, then, prove vital to adolescent emotional development as adolescents are less likely to rely on specific supportive behaviors, and positive relationship qualities may mitigate some of these adverse relationship behaviors during this period. Furthermore, the challenges that early adolescents encounter, including increased responsibility at school and at home, complicated social dynamics, and unfamiliar romantic relationships, can be made even more difficult without a secure base, characterized by positive qualities in parents’ relationships, to turn to in times of need [26].
In late adolescence, individuals return to expecting the same level of support they received from parents before the start of adolescence [47]. This support is likely to be more useful in the form of specific parent behaviors toward their adolescent rather than relationship qualities, as the nature of their peer relationships change during this period. Late adolescents often engage in romantic relationships characterized by increased intimacy, as opposed to the primarily affiliative romantic relationships of early adolescence [48]. Specific parent behaviors, like emotional expression, can be utilized by late adolescents in these intimate relationships to facilitate mutual satisfaction.

4. The Current Study

The value of understanding the ways in which parents impact the development of emotion regulation cannot be overstated. However, while intrapersonal emotion regulation has received a great deal of attention in the developmental literature, the same cannot be said for IER. Furthermore, there are generally significant gaps in the emotion regulation literature related to adolescent development of emotion regulation and the persisting impact of parents in this period. The current paper aims to extend the study of emotion regulation by considering both early and late adolescent predictors of young adult emotion regulation. The nature of IER in parental and parent–teen dyads in adolescence and the relative temporal relevance of different predictors of intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation across adolescence will be explored. More specifically, the current paper examines the interpersonal role that parents play in the development of youths’ intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation and the differential predictive utility of emotional relationship qualities and behaviors in interparental and parent–teen relationships across adolescence.
Given the developmental tasks of early adolescence, interparental and parent-teen relationships with positive emotional qualities are hypothesized to be stronger predictors of intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation as they provide evidence of a safe emotional space from which teens may begin to explore new emotions and relationship experiences. In late adolescence, on the other hand, positive interparental and parent–teen emotional behaviors in their relationships are hypothesized to be stronger predictors as teens become involved in more complex relationships and challenges and may benefit from parents’ modeling of emotion regulation behaviors in relationships. More specifically, the following is hypothesized (see Figure 1 for reference): (1) positive qualities of parental relationships will predict more positive intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation in young adulthood when they are reported in early adolescence; and (2) positive parent behaviors in relationships will predict more positive intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation in young adulthood when they are reported in late adolescence.

5. Method

5.1. Participants and Procedure

Participants in this study are drawn from a larger, ongoing longitudinal study of adolescent and young adult social and emotional development. Data collection for the larger study began in 1999, when participants were approximately 13 years old, and has continued on a yearly basis. The sample is composed of 184 adolescents, 85 males, and 99 females. The sample is diverse and representative of the area from which it was collected with respect to participant race/ethnicity (107 Caucasian, 53 African American, 2 Hispanic/Latino, 2 Asian American, 1 Native American, 15 mixed ethnicity, and 4 “other”) and socioeconomic status (median family income of USD 40,000–60,000/yr. in 1999, equivalent to about USD 75,000–112,000/year when accounting for inflation) [49].
Participants were recruited from a local middle school in the Southeastern United States that draws from suburban and urban populations. All parents of students in the seventh and eighth grades at the school were sent an initial mailing, giving them the opportunity to opt out of further contact (N = 298), and 2% of parents opted out at this time. Families who indicated interest were then contacted by phone and, of those eligible, 63% agreed to participate as either a target participant or as a peer providing additional information about the target participant. The sample was comparable with the overall population of the school regarding racial/ethnic makeup and socio-economic status. When participants were under age 18, participants provided informed assent before each interview session, and parents provided active, informed consent. At age 18 and beyond, participants provided informed consent. Parents, target adolescents, and peers were all paid for their participation. Transportation and childcare were provided if necessary. This study was reviewed and approved by the university’s Institutional Review Board.
For the current study, data from three waves of study were utilized. Variables assessing parental relationship qualities and behaviors were collected in early adolescence, when participants were approximately age 13 in 1999, and in late adolescence, when participants were approximately age 18 in 2004. These measures, to be described further below, asked parents to complete self-report measures regarding their marital relationship (i.e., consensus and cohesion) and to participate in an interaction task with their adolescents, assessing parent–teen self-disclosure and valuing. When participants were age 26, they completed self-report measures regarding their use of intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation strategies during times of stress (i.e., acceptance, denial, instrumental support seeking, and emotional support seeking).

5.2. Measures

5.2.1. Interparental Relationship Qualities and Behaviors

The Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS) is a 32-item measure completed by teens’ parents when teens were age 13 and 18 regarding their martial quality with their current partner [50]. The consensus and cohesion subscales of the measure were utilized. The consensus subscale asks parents to rate how often they and their partner disagree about 15 different topics (finances, household tasks, goals, in-laws, etc.) on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from 1, always agree, to 6, always disagree. The consensus scale was used as a marker of relationship quality, acting as a context of development that may allow or disallow for teens to seek IER in relationships. The cohesion subscale was also utilized, identified as parent behavior. The cohesion subscale first asks parents if they engage in outside interests together on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 0, none of them, to 4, all of them. Additionally, the subscale utilizes 4 additional items that ask participants how often different events occur between them and their partner (laughing together, exchanging ideas, working together, calmly discussing) on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from 1, “Never”, to 6, “More Often”. The cohesion subscale measures the extent to which parents engage in various behaviors and activities with one another, and these behaviors are theorized to act as a model for IER at the situation selection and modification and response modulation levels of Gross’ process model [43]. The DAS has shown great overall reliability, with Spanier reporting a total internal consistency of α = 0.96 [50]. In the present study, at both ages 13 and 18, respectively, maternal consensus (α = 0.91, α = 0.92) and cohesion (α = 0.83; α = 0.84) demonstrated good reliability, as did paternal consensus (α = 0.88; α = 0.94) and cohesion (α = 0.73; α = 0.83).

5.2.2. Parent–Teen Relationship Qualities and Behaviors

The Supportive Behavior Task (SBT) is an 8 min interaction task with teens and parents in which teens present a problem to parents that they want advice or support about [51]. Common topics that teens brought to parents included dating, peer or sibling relationships, money, and sports teams. Codes of parents’ valuing and self-disclosure when teens were aged 13 and 18 were utilized. The valuing subscale, identified as a parent relationship quality, assessed the extent to which parents demonstrated that they care about, value, or genuinely like their adolescent. Coding of parents’ valuing ranged from 0, unclear whether the person likes the other, to 4, the parent’s behavior is overall quite warm and fuzzy, the affection and liking is strong and clear, and the adolescent knows their parent really cares about them. The self-disclosure subscale, identified as a specific parent behavior, assessed parents’ sharing of information about themselves that allowed the adolescent to know them better. Coding of parents’ self-disclosure ranged from 0, sharing briefly about likes and dislikes or one’s day, to 4, sharing about areas not commonly shared between somewhat close friends, expressing strong feelings, or sharing unusual or embarrassing information about the self. Parents’ responses to teens’ problems in the interaction task may or may not constitute IER; however, the specific subscales utilized in this study are intended to assess parent’s engagement in IER with their teens in the form of qualities (situation level IER) and behaviors (response level IER) [43]. Importantly, codes for fathers’ self-disclosure and valuing were not available when these variables were first assessed, when teens were aged 13. Therefore, father self-disclosure and valuing were not included in the analyses during this wave of data collection. Reliability was computed using intraclass correlation coefficients: (mother valuing age 13 ICC = 0.78, age 18 ICC = 0.72; mother self-disclosure age 13 ICC = 0.77; age 18 ICC = 0.84; father valuing age 18 ICC = 0.69; father self-disclosure age 18 ICC = 0.60).

5.2.3. Intra- and Interpersonal Emotion Regulation

The COPE inventory is a 60-item measure developed by Carver and colleagues to assess adaptive and dysfunctional responses to stress [52]. For the purposes of this study, coping is used as a proxy of emotion regulation. While studies suggest that these constructs are distinct, they share many similarities. A key distinction between the constructs lies in the types of emotional events that trigger emotional responses; while emotion regulation is an ongoing process in which emotions are regulated under stressful and non-stressful conditions, coping represents emotion regulation that occurs under stress [53]. Still, both constructs represent regulatory processes, include purposeful efforts, and unfold and change over time. The use of coping for a proxy of emotion regulation in this particular study is supported by the developmental period that is being assessed by the measure. While a great deal of research on emotion regulation has focused on infancy and early childhood, the construct of coping is typically studied in later childhood, adolescence, and adulthood.
At ages 21 and 26, participants completed the self-report measure by rating what they usually do in response to experiencing stressful events on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 1, “I usually don’t do this at all”, to 4, “I usually do this a lot”. In the current study, the acceptance and denial subscales of the measure were utilized to represent intrapersonal emotion regulation strategies and use of emotional social support, and the use of instrumental social support subscales were utilized to represent IER strategies. Carver and colleagues report good reliability for the COPE inventory subscales utilized in the current study (acceptance: α = 0.65; denial: α = 0.71; emotional support: α = 0.85; instrumental support: α = 0.75) [52]. In the present study, all scales of interest demonstrated adequate reliability at both ages 21 and 26, respectively: acceptance (α = 0.68, α = 0.74), denial (α = 0.77; α = 0.73), emotional support (α = 0.86; α = 0.88), and instrumental support (α = 0.81; α = 0.88).

5.3. Data Analysis and Interpretation

All variables in the current study were first examined to ensure adherence to the assumptions of maximum likelihood estimation and to assess bivariate relationships among variables of interest (see Table 1, Table 2 and Table 3). Full information maximum likelihood (FIML) methods were utilized for all study analyses. FIML accounts for biases in estimation that may result from missing data and allows for the least biased estimates by using all available data for longitudinal analyses [54]. Attrition analyses indicated no differences in any study variables for participants who did vs. did not have data at age 26. As a result, data were utilized for all 184 study participants; there were no data manipulations or exclusions.
A series of hierarchical multiple regression analyses predicting acceptance, denial, instrumental support seeking, and emotional support seeking were conducted in three steps. At step one, control variables (sex, income, and outcomes measured at age 21) were entered into the equations to limit the influence of potentially conflating influences on study outcomes. At step two, predictors contrary to study hypotheses (i.e., behaviors first in early adolescent models, qualities first in late adolescent models) were entered into the models. At step three, predictors in accordance with study analyses were entered into the models. Table 4, Table 5, Table 6, Table 7, Table 8 and Table 9 include the regression coefficients of variables at both entry to the model and in the final model that consider all variables together. Beta entry values represent the regression coefficients of variables most recently entered in a model and those entered before. This allows us to examine the effects of parent relationship qualities and behaviors on intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation separately in each model. Furthermore, estimation of values in full models show the effects of hypothesized-stronger predictors beyond what is accounted for by hypothesized weaker predictors. Early and late adolescent predictors were entered into separate models predicting each emotion regulation strategy separately

5.4. Transparency and Openness

We report within this manuscript how we determined our sample size, all data exclusions, all manipulations, and all measures in the study, and we follow JARS [55]. The raw data, analysis code, and research materials in this study are available by request to the corresponding author. Data analysis was conducted via computer software (SAS 9.4). This study’s design and its analysis were not pre-registered.

6. Results

6.1. Preliminary Analyses

Univariate and Correlational Analyses

Table 1, Table 2 and Table 3 display the descriptive statistics (i.e., means, standard deviations, minimums, and maximums) and correlations for all study predictors. Preliminary analyses found significant, positive relationships between income and predictor variables of parent reports of their consensus in their relationship when teens were age 13, mother valuing in interactions with teens aged 13, and father relationship variables with teens and relationship partners at age 18. Examining outcome variables, significant positive relationships were found between sex and the use of IER strategies such that being female was associated with a greater use of IER strategies. Significant positive relationships were also found between income and the use of intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation strategies. These results are presented in Table 3. Because of the significant relationships found in preliminary analyses, sex and income were controlled for in all study analyses. Regression analyses also included variables assessing each corresponding outcome assessed at its earliest time available in the study (age 21) to act as a baseline measurement of intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation. This allowed us to examine whether relationship qualities and behaviors could predict later intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation independently of participants’ pre-existing emotion regulation at age 21.

6.2. Primary Analyses

Multivariate Regression Analyses

Hypothesis 1. 
Positive qualities of parental relationships will predict more intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation in young adulthood when they are reported in early adolescence.
In early adolescent models, positive interparental relationship qualities predicted a relative increase in positive intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation strategies during young adulthood. Greater father-reported interparental consensus predicted youths’ increased use of emotional social support (β = 0.21, p = 0.044; see Table 4), and greater mother-reported interparental consensus predicted increased use of acceptance of emotions (β = 0.27, p = 0.034; see Table 5). There were no significant parent relationship predictors at age 13 for the use of denial or instrumental support as emotion regulation strategies at age 26.
Hypothesis 2. 
Positive parent behaviors in relationships will predict more intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation in young adulthood when they are reported in late adolescence.
In late adolescent models, positive interparental and parent–teen relationship behaviors were primarily strong predictors of adaptive intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation. Greater father-reported cohesion between parents predicted increased the use of both emotional and instrumental support during young adulthood (emotional: β = 0.30, p =.031; instrumental: β = 0.33, p = 0.035; see Table 6 and Table 7, respectively). Greater mother-to-teen self-disclosure predicted youths’ increased the use of acceptance and decreased the use of denial of emotions (acceptance: β = 0.30, p = 0.019; denial: β = −0.30, p = 0.016; see Table 8 and Table 9, respectively). However, some qualities and behaviors from fathers in late adolescence predicted the increased use of maladaptive intrapersonal emotion regulation strategies; as can be seen in Table 9, greater engagement in denial was predicted by greater father self-disclosure to teens (β = 0.29, p = 0.03) and greater father-reported consensus between parents (β = 0.36, p = 0.003).

7. Discussion

The present study sought to compare the predictive utility of positive interparental and parent–teen relationship qualities and behaviors for predicting intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation tendencies in young adulthood. Study hypotheses were grounded in previous research and theory suggesting that adolescents’ social and emotional development are largely impacted by familial relationships and that there are differing social and emotional tasks on opposing ends of adolescence [35,37,41,42,48]. Results generally provide support for the study hypotheses. Parental relationship qualities were stronger predictors of intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation in early adolescence, and relationship behaviors appeared to come online as predictors in late adolescence.
Attachment theory suggests that individuals’ emotional security and regulation is built through early parent–child interactions that form internal working models from which individuals continue to operate and apply to new situations throughout their lives [41,42]. An individual’s attachment relationship to their caretaker depends on their ability to rely on their caretaker as both a secure base and as a safe haven [41,42,56]. Functioning as a secure base for one’s children requires providing a safe place for children to return to upon exploring unfamiliar experiences and environments [24]. The largely unfamiliar developmental tasks of early adolescence would, then, seem to necessitate a caretaker relationship, which includes several positive relationship qualities that enable the relationship to act as a secure base [26,48]. Attachment relationships that function as a safe haven require caretakers to provide direct support to children in times of distress [24]. The increasingly complicated and potentially distressing social and romantic contexts in which late adolescents are involved would, then, seem to necessitate a caretaker to engage in the direct modeling of positive emotion-related behaviors to enable the relationship to act as a safe haven for late adolescents [26,48]. Indeed, the current study found that, in early adolescence, parental relationship qualities were significant predictors of young adult emotion regulation beyond what was accounted for by parent relationship behaviors, and in late adolescence, parental relationship behaviors were significant predictors of young adult emotion regulation beyond what was accounted for by parent relationship qualities. These findings further previous research examining the impact of parental relationships and family factors on children and adolescents’ development of intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation by revealing a differential pattern of effects for parental relationship qualities and behaviors. Though previous literature has established the impact that parent relationships have on emotional development, this research has less frequently been conducted in adolescence. Furthermore, disparate ends of adolescence have yet to be explored regarding the unique emotional needs of early and late teens.
The parental relationship quality of general agreement between parents in early adolescence, interparental relationship consensus, was the only significant predictor of young adult emotion regulation in early adolescent models. Observations of mothers’ valuing of teens in their interactions were not significant predictors of intra- or interpersonal emotion regulation. Thus, when early adolescents observe positive qualities in their parents’ relationship, as opposed to when they experience positive qualities within the parent–teen relationship, they are more likely to engage in positive emotion regulation strategies. In late adolescent models, behavioral engagement between parents, interparental cohesion, was a significant predictor of interpersonal emotion regulation in young adulthood. These results provide further evidence for the well-documented impact that interparental relationships have on the adjustment of children and adolescents, as evidenced in the emotional security hypothesis [37]. Researchers have suggested that the effects of interparental conflict on emotion regulation may also operate through parent–child relationships factors, like attachment and parent warmth [57,58]. Other researchers have suggested that children’s responses to interparental conflict are dependent upon their emotion regulation [59]. The results of the current study extend such findings and suggest that positive aspects of interparental relationships, rather than just interparental conflict, may also play a role in social and emotional development.
Continuing with late adolescent predictors, the current study also found that mothers’ self-disclosure to teens predicted adaptive intrapersonal emotion regulation through a greater acceptance of and less denial of emotions in young adulthood. However, fathers’ self-disclosure to teens predicted maladaptive intrapersonal emotion regulation use through a greater denial of emotions in young adulthood. Disparate findings regarding the effects of parent self-disclosure may relate to the existing relationship between teens and parents at the time of or to topics which were disclosed during the Supportive Behavior Task [51]. Though there has been little research conducted on the effect or process of parents’ self-disclosure to children and adolescents, and that which has been conducted may be outdated, general self-disclosure research can be utilized to understand the results of the current study. The literature suggests that perceptions of appropriateness and responses to self-disclosure by those on the receiving end depends upon the closeness of the relationship between these two people, such that receivers of self-disclosure view self-disclosure as warm and appropriate in the context of close relationships but maladjusted and inappropriate outside of such close relationships [60]. Thus, it may be that fathers engaging in more revealing self-disclosure with teens may not have close enough relationships for such self-disclosure to be viewed positively, whereas mothers may have closer relationships with their teens in early adolescence, allowing self-disclosure to strengthen feelings of warmth and closeness within the dyad and promote greater regulation in young adulthood. This aligns with findings that suggest that mothers are perceived by late adolescents as more supportive than fathers [44] and that mothers engage more and are more involved with their adolescents (i.e., spend more time with, talk more with) than fathers [61,62].
Importantly, the current study did not examine the topics of parents’ self-disclosure during the interaction task but rather the level of self-disclosure, with more private or unusual topics shared being rated higher on this scale. A study conducted by Stern and colleagues may provide insight regarding why the topic of parents’ self-disclosure may be notable [63]. This study examined the efficacy of an intervention intended to repair attachment relationships between parents and adolescents. The study found that, in successful sessions, self-disclosure was viewed by raters as warm, disclosing, and expressing; in the unsuccessful session, parents’ self-disclosure was rated as hostile, sulking, and scurrying [63]. Hence, it may also be that the topic of fathers’ self-disclosure was similar to that examined by Stern and colleagues, hostile, sulking, and scurrying, resulting in the destabilization of or representing already unstable relationships between teens and fathers [63]. As a result, self-disclosure would not prove to be the positive relationship behavior and learning opportunity that it was proposed to be and would not be expected to be predictive of adaptive functioning in adulthood. Should future researchers wish to examine associations between parent’s self-disclosure and emotion regulation, these researchers should consider both the content of self-disclosure and the existing relationship between parents and teens.
Notably, the results from the current study have practical implications regarding the respective focus of the parenting environments in early and late adolescence. Parents who create a secure base for their early adolescents to return to, composed of positive qualities, may support more positive emotion regulation development. In late adolescence, parents who model positive relationship behaviors and provide direct support through the creation of a safe haven may promote more positive emotion regulation development. Therefore, parents should consider the overall qualities of their relationships with one another when teens are younger, ensuring to maintain consensus so as to facilitate emotional security in early adolescents. Parents of older adolescents should consider making explicit efforts to provide support for teens and model to them appropriate relationship behaviors. Positive interparental relationships composed of both general agreement and behavioral engagement may have specific positive implications for teens’ later use of adaptive emotion regulation strategies in young adulthood. Parents should consider how the positive and negative aspects of the interparental relationship impact teens’ overall emotional development. Lastly, parents who engage in self-disclosure with their adolescents in response to adolescents’ seeking of advice or support should consider both the strength of their existing relationship with adolescents as well as the topic of self-disclosure.
The implications of this study also extend to the area of clinical practice. The current study provides evidence that parents may be valuable conduits for delivering emotion regulation interventions in adolescence. Furthermore, the results of the current study cement the value of working with both parents and teens throughout adolescence when working to foster emotional regulation strategies in individual clinical work with youth. Clinicians developing emotion regulation-related interventions for parents to deliver to teens, and those working directly with teens and families, may benefit from considering the qualities and behaviors parents engage in at different developmental stages, as well as the overall impact of the interparental relationship. Efforts made to foster greater emotion regulation capacity, then, may consider the role that interparental relationships play and encourage parents to maintain positive qualities like consensus when clients are early adolescents and enact positive behavioral engagement with one another when clients are late adolescents. Within the parent–teen relationship, interventions that utilize self-disclosure to facilitate closeness and regulation capacities should consider the content of self-disclosure as well as the existing relationship between parents and teens.
There were several strengths to the study that should be noted. Most importantly, the multi-method and longitudinal design represented significant strengths. The use of interaction tasks to assess parent–child relationships allowed for less biased, applied measurements of parents’ interactions with teens. The use of multiple reporters in assessing interparental relationship factors provided the opportunity to examine how each parents’ report of their dyadic relationship provides different information about the impact of interparental relationships on teens’ social and emotional development. As it relates to data analysis, potential correlates (i.e., sex and income) were controlled for in all study analyses to account for any possible effects that these demographic factors might have on the use of emotion regulation strategies. Furthermore, all study analyses included measures of each outcome 5 years prior to the period examined in the current study to allow the authors to examine the impact of parental relationships on emotion regulation above and beyond stable emotion regulation tendencies.
There are also several limitations in the current study that are of note. Importantly, causal claims cannot be drawn from naturalistic, longitudinal study designs due to the correlational nature of the data. Furthermore, some study variables rely solely upon self-report data, which may bias or contribute to general inaccuracies in the data. Additionally, recent directions in relational literature have emphasized the bidirectional nature of parent–child relationships [26]. This research suggests that disagreement in reports within dyads may mean that neither report is an accurate depiction of the construct, suggesting that parents’ reports of their dyadic functioning in and of themselves may not provide a complete understanding of relationship qualities and behaviors within the dyad. Regarding the inclusion of earlier measurements of outcome variables, though this represents a strength to the analyses conducted, such controls would have benefitted from even earlier measurement to ensure that the comparison of emotion regulation across both time periods accurately accounted for stable emotion regulation across development. Notably, father interaction variables were not available during the early adolescent wave of measurement. This precluded the authors from examining and comparing father–teen qualities and behaviors in early adolescence as predictors of young adult emotion regulation. Results showed insignificant findings for the prediction of emotion regulation by mother valuing in interactions with teens. Thus, researchers may consider examining other parent–teen relationship qualities in early adolescence or father valuing in early adolescence, as it is possible that insignificant results are specific to valuing and/or the mother–teen relationship. Furthermore, the father report and father interaction variables often had lower sample sizes than other variables utilized in the study, making results related to fathers somewhat more challenging to interpret and generalize.
Future research may take several directions based on the results of the current study. The results of the current study suggest that developmental tasks at opposing ends of adolescence may pose different demands from parental relationships. While compelling, further research is needed to confirm this pattern. This research may consider examining such patterns with other social and emotional developmental outcomes, such as social competence and emotional awareness. Researchers may also consider how other parental relationship qualities and behaviors that have established effects on emotion regulation, like parental conflict and discussion of emotion, fall into the pattern established by the current study. More research on the impact of positive interparental relationships, as opposed to negative ones, is further implicated by the present findings. Researchers may consider how other positive relationship qualities and behaviors between parents, like intimacy and affection, impact the social and emotional development of adolescents.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.V.W. and D.E.S.; methodology, S.V.W. and D.E.S., formal analysis, S.V.W.; resources, J.P.A.; data curation, J.P.A.; writing—original draft preparation, S.V.W.; writing—review and editing, S.V.W. and D.E.S.; supervision, D.E.S.; project administration, J.P.A.; funding acquisition, J.P.A. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the National Institute of Mental Health: R01- MH58066 and Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development: 5R37HD058305-23, R01HD058305-16A1.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Virginia (#2733)

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Dataset available upon request from the authors.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Gross, J.J. Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects. Psychol. Inq. 2015, 26, 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Porges, S.W. The polyvagal perspective. Biol. Psychol. 2007, 74, 116–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Bradley, B.; DeFife, J.A.; Guarnaccia, C.; Phifer, J.; Fani, N.; Ressler, K.J.; Westen, D. Emotion dysregulation and negative affect: Association with psychiatric symptoms. J. Clin. Psychiatry 2011, 72, 685–691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Sapolsky, R.M. Stress, stress-related disease, and emotional regulation. In Handbook of Emotion Regulation; Gross, J.J., Ed.; 2007-01392-030; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2007; pp. 606–615. [Google Scholar]
  5. Tani, F.; Pascuzzi, D.; Raffagnino, R. Emotion regulation and quality of close relationship: The effects of emotion dysregulation processes on couple intimacy. BPA Appl. Psychol. Bull. 2015, 63, 3–15. [Google Scholar]
  6. Dixon-Gordon, K.L.; Bernecker, S.L.; Christensen, K. Recent innovations in the field of interpersonal emotion regulation. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2015, 3, 36–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Reeck, C.; Ames, D.R.; Ochsner, K.N. The social regulation of emotion: An integrative, cross-disciplinary model. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2016, 20, 47–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Zaki, J.; Williams, W.C. Interpersonal emotion regulation. Emotion. 2013, 13, 803–810. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Gross, J.J.; Richards, J.M.; John, O.P. Emotion regulation in everyday life. In Emotion Regulation in Couples and Families: Pathways to Dysfunction and Health; Snyder, D.K., Simpson, J., Hughes, J.N., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2006; pp. 13–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Balasundaram, P.; Avulakunta, I.D. Human Growth and Development. In StatPearls; StatPearls Publishing: Treasure Island, FL, USA, 2024. Available online: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK567767/ (accessed on 17 March 2023).
  11. World Health Organization (n.d.). Adolescent Health. Available online: https://www.who.int/health-topics/adolescent-health#tab=tab_1 (accessed on 26 September 2024).
  12. Britannica, T. Editors of Encyclopaedia. Adulthood. Encyclopedia Britannica. 7 September 2024. Available online: https://www.britannica.com/science/adulthood (accessed on 26 September 2024).
  13. Young Adult Health and Well-Being: A Position Statement of the Society for Adolescent Health and Medicine. J. Adolesc. Health 2017, 60, 758–759. [CrossRef]
  14. Butner, J.; Diamond, L.M.; Hicks, A.M. Attachment style and two forms of affect coregulation between romantic partners. Pers. Relatsh. 2007, 14, 431–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Saxbe, D.; Repetti, R.L. For better or worse? Coregulation of couples’ cortisol levels and mood states. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2010, 98, 92–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Butler, E.A.; Randall, A.K. Emotional coregulation in close relationships. Emot. Rev. 2013, 5, 202–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Evans, C.A.; Porter, C.L. The emergence of mother–infant co-regulation during the first year: Links to infants’ developmental status and attachment. Infant Behav. Dev. 2009, 32, 147–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Lindsey, E.W. Relationship context and emotion regulation across the life span. Emotion 2020, 20, 59–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Perry, N.B.; Dollar, J.M.; Calkins, S.D.; Keane, S.P.; Shanahan, L. Maternal socialization of child emotion and adolescent adjustment: Indirect effects through emotion regulation. Dev. Psychol. 2020, 56, 541–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Shewark, E.A.; Blandon, A.Y. Mothers’ and fathers’ emotion socialization and children’s emotion regulation: A within-family model. Soc. Dev. 2015, 24, 266–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Gottman, J.M.; Katz, L.F.; Hooven, C. Meta-Emotion: How Families Communicate Emotionally, 1st ed.; Routledge: Abingdon, UK, 1997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Cole, P.M.; Armstrong, L.M.; Pemberton, C.K. The role of language in the development of emotion regulation. In Child Development at the Intersection of Emotion and Cognition; Calkins, S.D., Bell, M.A., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2010; pp. 59–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Grabell, A.S.; Huppert, T.J.; Fishburn, F.A.; Li, Y.; Hlutkowsky, C.O.; Jones, H.M.; Wakschlag, L.S.; Perlman, S.B. Neural correlates of early deliberate emotion regulation: Young children’s responses to interpersonal scaffolding. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2019, 40, 100708. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Paley, B.; Hajal, N.J. Conceptualizing emotion regulation and coregulation as family-level phenomena. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 2022, 25, 19–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Christie, D.; Viner, R. Adolescent development. BMJ 2005, 330, 301–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Morris, A.S.; Ratliff, E.L.; Cosgrove, K.T.; Steinberg, L. We know even more things: A decade review of parenting research. J. Res. Adolesc. 2021, 31, 870–888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Meier, A.; Allen, G. Romantic relationships from adolescence to young adulthood: Evidence from the national longitudinal study of adolescent health. Sociol. Q. 2009, 50, 308–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Barthel, A.L.; Hay, A.; Doan, S.N.; Hofmann, S.G. Interpersonal emotion regulation: A review of social and developmental components. Behav. Chang. 2018, 35, 203–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Eisenberg, N.; Spinrad, T.L.; Eggum, N.D. Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to children’s maladjustment. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2010, 6, 495–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Martin, R.E.; Ochsner, K.N. The neuroscience of emotion regulation development: Implications for education. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2016, 10, 142–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Silvers, J.A. Adolescence as a pivotal period for emotion regulation development. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2022, 44, 258–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Schweizer, S.; Gotlib, I.H.; Blakemore, S.-J. The role of affective control in emotion regulation during adolescence. Emotion 2020, 20, 80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Katz, L.F.; Hunter, E.C. Maternal meta-emotion philosophy and adolescent depressive symptomatology. Soc. Dev. 2007, 16, 343–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Maughan, A.; Cicchetti, D. Impact of child maltreatment and interadult violence on children’s emotion regulation abilities and socioemotional adjustment. Child Dev. 2002, 73, 1525–1542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Morris, A.S.; Silk, J.S.; Steinberg, L.; Myers, S.S.; Robinson, L.R. The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Soc. Dev. 2007, 16, 361–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Morris, A.S.; Criss, M.M.; Silk, J.S.; Houltberg, B.J. The impact of parenting on emotion regulation during childhood and adolescence. Child Dev. Perspect. 2017, 11, 233–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Davies, P.T.; Cummings, E.M. Marital conflict and child adjustment: An emotional security hypothesis. Psychol. Bull. 1994, 116, 387–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Davies, P.T.; Martin, M.J.; Sturge-Apple, M.L.; Ripple, M.T.; Cicchetti, D. The distinctive sequelae of children’s coping with interparental conflict: Testing the reformulated emotional security theory. Dev. Psychol. 2016, 52, 1646–1665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Harold, G.T.; Shelton, K.H.; Goeke-Morey, M.C.; Cummings, E.M. Marital conflict, child emotional security about family relationships and child adjustment. Soc. Dev. 2004, 13, 350–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Cabecinha-Alati, S.; O’Hara, G.; Kennedy, H.; Montreuil, T. Parental emotion socialization and adult outcomes: The relationships between parental supportiveness, emotion regulation, and trait anxiety. J. Adult Dev. 2020, 27, 268–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Ainsworth, M.S. Attachments beyond infancy. Am. Psychol. 1989, 44, 709–716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Bowlby, J. Attachment and loss: Retrospect and prospect. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 1982, 52, 664–678. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Gross, J.J. The extended process model of emotion regulation: Elaborations, applications, and future directions. Psychol. Inq. 2015, 26, 130–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Stocker, C.M.; Richmond, M.K.; Rhoades, G.K.; Kiang, L. Family emotional processes and adolescents’ adjustment. Soc. Dev. 2007, 16, 310–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Adrian, M.; Zeman, J.; Erdley, C.; Lisa, L.; Homan, K.; Sim, L. Social contextual links to emotion regulation in an adolescent psychiatric inpatient population: Do gender and symptomatology matter? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2009, 50, 1428–1436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Fosco, G.M.; Caruthers, A.S.; Dishion, T.J. A six-year predictive test of adolescent family relationship quality and effortful control pathways to emerging adult social and emotional health. J. Fam. Psychol. 2012, 26, 565–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Steinberg, L.; Silk, J.S. Parenting adolescents. In Handbook of Parenting; Routledge: Abingdon, UK, 2002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Berger, L.; Mcmakin, D.; Furman, W. The language of love: Romantic relationships in adolescence. In Talking Adolescence: Perspectives on Communication in the Teenage Years; Williams, A., Thurlow, C., Eds.; Peter Lang Publishing: New York, NY, USA, 2005; pp. 129–145. [Google Scholar]
  49. Bureau of Labor Statistics (n.d.). CPI Inflation Calculator. Available online: https://data.bls.gov/cgi-bin/cpicalc.pl (accessed on 17 March 2023).
  50. Spanier, G.B. Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads. J. Marriage Fam. 1976, 38, 15–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Allen, J.; Hall, F.; Insabella, G.; Land, D.; Marsh, P.; Porter, M. Supportive Behavior Task Coding Manual; Unpublished Manuscript; Department of Psychology, University of Virginia: Charlottesville, VA, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  52. Carver, C.S.; Scheier, M.F.; Weintraub, J.K. Assessing coping strategies: A theoretically based approach. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1989, 56, 267–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Compas, B.E.; Jaser, S.S.; Dunbar, J.P.; Watson, K.H.; Bettis, A.H.; Gruhn, M.A.; Williams, E.K. Coping and emotion regulation from childhood to early adulthood: Points of convergence and divergence. Aust. J. Psychol. 2014, 66, 71–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Arbuckle, J.L. Full information estimation in the presence of incomplete data. In Advanced Structural Equation Modeling: Issues and Techniques; Marcoulides, G.A., Schumacker, R.E., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1996; pp. 243–279. [Google Scholar]
  55. Kazak, A.E. Editorial: Journal article reporting standards. Am. Psychol. 2018, 73, 1–2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Kerns, K.A.; Mathews, B.L.; Koehn, A.J.; Williams, C.T.; Siener-Ciesla, S. Assessing both safe haven and secure base support in parent–child relationships. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2015, 17, 337–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Fosco, G.M.; Grych, J.H. Capturing the family context of emotion regulation: A family systems model comparison approach. J. Fam. Issues 2013, 34, 557–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Gong, X.; Paulson, S.E. Effect of family affective environment on individuals’ emotion regulation. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2017, 117, 144–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Davies, P.T.; Cummings, E.M. Exploring children’s emotional security as a mediator of the link between marital relations and child adjustment. Child Dev. 1998, 69, 124–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Chaikin, A.L.; Derlega, V.J. Variables affecting the appropriateness of self-disclosure. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1974, 42, 588–593. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Maccoby, E.E. The gender of child and parent as factors in family dynamics. In Children’s Influence on Family Dynamics: The Neglected Side of Family Relationships; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2003; pp. 191–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Phares, V.; Fields, S.; Kamboukos, D. Fathers’ and mothers’ involvement with their adolescents. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2009, 18, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Stern, R.S.; King, A.A.; Diamond, G. Repairing attachment in families with depressed adolescents: A task analysis. J. Clin. Psychol. 2023, 79, 201–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Heuristic representation of Hypotheses 1 and 2.
Figure 1. Heuristic representation of Hypotheses 1 and 2.
Youth 04 00090 g001
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for study variables.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for study variables.
VariableNMeanSDMin.Max.
Sex (85 male, 99 female)184----
Income184USD 40–60 k/yr.-<USD 5 k/yr.<USD 60 k/yr.
Age 13 Predictors
Mother-reported consensus18441.3713.162465
Father-reported consensus18438.6811.902765
Mother-reported cohesion12815.244.22424
Father-reported cohesion9914.643.46623
Mother valuing1682.000.9904
Mother self-disclosure1680.500.7504
Age 18 Predictors
Mother-reported consensus10844.9011.971465
Father-reported consensus8548.778.531365
Mother-reported cohesion10916.174.76426
Father-reported cohesion8214.903.96624
Mother valuing1032.100.8004
Father valuing661.950.720.54
Mother self-disclosure1030.270.5102
Father self-disclosure660.230.4601.75
Age 21 Outcomes
Acceptance1636.302.61012
Denial1652.002.37010
Emotional support seeking1657.352.99012
Instrumental support seeking1657.502.82012
Age 26 Outcomes
Acceptance1446.902.73112
Denial1441.311.8908
Emotional support seeking1447.253.05012
Instrumental support seeking1467.722.83112
Table 2. Correlations Between Study Predictors and Age 26 Outcomes.
Table 2. Correlations Between Study Predictors and Age 26 Outcomes.
Interpersonal Emotion
Regulation
Intrapersonal Emotion
Regulation
Variable TypeVariableInstrumental Support
Seeking
Emotional Support
Seeking
DenialAcceptance
ControlsSex0.20 *0.32 ***0.040.02
Income0.21 *0.21 *−0.020.20 *
Age 13
Predictors
Relationship Qualities
Father consensus0.29 ***0.30 ***−0.090.12
Mother consensus0.160.18 *−0.060.27 **
Mother valuing0.140.080.080.03
Relationship Behaviors
Father cohesion0.170.15−0.010.09
Mother cohesion0.24 *0.21 *−0.01−0.02
Mother self-disclosure0.020.02−0.120.11
Age 18
Predictors
Relationship Qualities
Father consensus0.080.100.120.04
Mother consensus0.020.020.030.05
Father valuing0.070.020.30 *0.06
Mother valuing0.090.07−0.170.05
Relationship Behaviors
Father cohesion0.24 *0.23−0.090.03
Mother cohesion0.170.180.06−0.04
Father self-disclosure−0.080.030.28*0.08
Mother self-disclosure−0.11−0.11−0.190.21
Note. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table 3. Correlations among study predictors.
Table 3. Correlations among study predictors.
123456789101112131415
1. Sex-
2. Income−0.12-
Age 13 Predictors
3. Father consensus−0.070.43 ***-
4. Father cohesion0.010.050.36 ***-
5. Mother consensus0.120.32 ***0.46 ***0.15-
6. Mother cohesion −0.000.020.18 *0.44 ***0.50 ***-
7. Mother valuing−0.010.20 *0.23 **−0.030.21 **0.17-
8. Mother self-disclosure0.090.080.060.15−0.03−0.030.27 **-
Age 18 Predictors
9. Mother valuing0.040.090.22 *0.200.020.28 **0.23 *0.28 **-
10. Mother self-disclosure−0.18−0.11−0.000.190.060.070.050.130.41 ***-
11. Father self-disclosure−0.03−0.030.140.08−0.020.11−10−0.00−0.020.19-
12. Father valuing0.010.25 *0.220.140.110.29 *0.150.060.250.210.32 **-
13. Mother consensus−0.01−0.13−0.090.070.010.24 *0.050.010.32 **0.27 *−0.010.08-
14. Mother cohesion0.060.140.170.36 **0.160.54 ***0.09−0.01−0.08−0.120.030.15−0.17-
15. Father consensus −0.010.23 *0.36 ***0.41 ***0.190.210.080.14−0.090.11−0.240.01−0.080.34 **-
16. Father cohesion−0.050.200.110.60 ***−0.190.13−0.03−0.020.120.160.050.15−0.060.49 ***0.39 ***
Note. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table 4. Parental relationship qualities and behaviors at age 13 as predictors of emotional support.
Table 4. Parental relationship qualities and behaviors at age 13 as predictors of emotional support.
Emotional Support (26)
β Entryβ Final95% CIR2
Step 1: Controls 0.31
Sex0.20 **0.25 ***0.11, 0.39
Income0.18 **0.11−0.05, 0.27
Emotional support (21)0.43 ***0.40 ***0.26, 0.53
Step 2: Behaviors 0.33
Mother self-disclosure−0.05−0.04−0.19, 0.10
Father-reported cohesion0.090.10−0.16, 0.36
Mother-reported cohesion0.080.08−0.19, 0.35
Step 3: Qualities 0.39
Mother valuing−0.08−0.08−0.23, 0.07
Father-reported consensus0.21 *0.21 *0.01, 0.42
Mother-reported consensus−0.16−0.16−0.39, 0.07
Note. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table 5. Parental relationship qualities and behaviors at age 13 as predictors of acceptance.
Table 5. Parental relationship qualities and behaviors at age 13 as predictors of acceptance.
Acceptance (26)
β Entryβ Final95% CIR2
Step 1: Controls 0.21
Sex0.01−0.02−0.17, 0.13
Income0.18 *0.09−0.08 0.26
Acceptance (21)0.41 ***0.40 ***0.25, 0.54
Step 2: Behaviors 0.21
Mother self-disclosure0.070.11−0.05, 0.27
Father-reported cohesion0.02−0.05−0.34, 0.23
Mother-reported cohesion−0.05−0.06−0.34, 0.21
Step 3: Qualities 0.24
Mother valuing−0.09−0.09−0.25, 0.08
Father-reported consensus0.030.03−0.20, 0.26
Mother-reported consensus0.27 *0.27 *0.02, 0.51
Note. * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001.
Table 6. Parental relationship qualities and behaviors at age 18 as predictors of emotional support.
Table 6. Parental relationship qualities and behaviors at age 18 as predictors of emotional support.
Emotional Support (26)
β Entryβ Final95% CIR2
Step 1: Controls 0.31
Sex0.20 **0.19 *0.03, 0.35
Income0.18 **0.14−0.08, 0.36
Emotional support (21)0.43 ***0.49 ***0.32, 0.65
Step 2: Qualities 0.34
Mother valuing0.040.06−0.15, 0.28
Father valuing−0.16−0.18−0.45, 0.09
Father-reported consensus0.05−0.03−0.29, 0.24
Mother-reported consensus0.080.08−0.13, 0.28
Step 3: Behaviors 0.40
Mother self-disclosure−0.08−0.08−0.32, 0.15
Father self-disclosure−0.03−0.03−0.30, 0.24
Father-reported cohesion0.30 *0.30 *0.03, 0.58
Mother-reported cohesion0.130.13−0.31, 0.20
Note. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table 7. Parental relationship qualities and behaviors at age 18 as predictors of instrumental support.
Table 7. Parental relationship qualities and behaviors at age 18 as predictors of instrumental support.
Instrumental Support (26)
β Entryβ Final95% CIR2
Step 1: Controls 0.24
Sex0.140.14−0.02, 0.30
Income0.17 *0.00−0.22, 0.23
Instrumental support (21)0.39 ***0.41 ***0.26, 0.56
Step 2: Qualities 0.25
Mother valuing0.060.10−0.12, 0.32
Father valuing0.050.08−0.20, 0.37
Father-reported consensus0.04−0.07−0.35, 0.21
Mother-reported consensus0.070.05−0.16, 0.27
Step 3: Behaviors 0.38
Mother self-disclosure−0.11−0.11−0.36, 0.14
Father self-disclosure−0.27−0.27−0.56, 0.03
Father-reported cohesion0.33 *0.33*0.02, 0.63
Mother-reported cohesion−0.04−0.04−0.31, 0.24
Note. * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001.
Table 8. Parental relationship qualities and behaviors at age 18 as predictors of acceptance.
Table 8. Parental relationship qualities and behaviors at age 18 as predictors of acceptance.
Acceptance (26)
β Entryβ Final95% CIR2
Step 1: Controls 0.21
Sex0.010.09−0.07, 0.26
Income0.18 *0.26 *0.04, 0.48
Acceptance (21)0.41 ***0.41 ***0.26, 0.57
Step 2: Qualities 0.23
Mother valuing0.09−0.03−0.26, 0.19
Father valuing−0.12−0.12−0.42, 0.18
Father-reported consensus0.090.91−0.28, 0.30
Mother-reported consensus0.02−0.04−0.25, 0.17
Step 3: Behaviors 0.29
Mother self-disclosure0.30 *0.30 *0.05, 0.56
Father self-disclosure−0.07−0.07−0.37, 0.24
Father-reported cohesion−0.04−0.04−0.36, 0.28
Mother-reported cohesion−0.01−0.01−0.28, 0.26
Note. * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001.
Table 9. Parental relationship qualities and behaviors at age 18 as predictors of denial.
Table 9. Parental relationship qualities and behaviors at age 18 as predictors of denial.
Denial (26)
β Entryβ Final95% CIR2
Step 1 0.10
Sex0.040.01−0.16, 0.18
Income0.03−0.05−0.29, 0.19
Denial (21)0.32 ***0.32 ***0.15, 0.50
Step 2: Qualities 0.22
Mother valuing−0.19−0.05−0.28, 0.18
Father valuing0.30 *0.23−0.02, 0.47
Father-reported consensus0.110.29 *0.02, 0.56
Mother-reported consensus0.080.13−0.08, 0.35
Step 3: Behaviors 0.35
Mother self-disclosure−0.30 *−0.30 *−0.54, −0.06
Father self-disclosure0.36 **0.36 **0.12, 0.61
Father-reported cohesion−0.10−0.10−0.40, 0.20
Mother-reported cohesion0.020.02−0.24, 0.28
Note. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Wilson, S.V.; Szwedo, D.E.; Allen, J.P. Interparental and Parent–Teen Relationships during Adolescence as Predictors of Intra- and Interpersonal Emotion Regulation in Young Adulthood. Youth 2024, 4, 1417-1436. https://doi.org/10.3390/youth4040090

AMA Style

Wilson SV, Szwedo DE, Allen JP. Interparental and Parent–Teen Relationships during Adolescence as Predictors of Intra- and Interpersonal Emotion Regulation in Young Adulthood. Youth. 2024; 4(4):1417-1436. https://doi.org/10.3390/youth4040090

Chicago/Turabian Style

Wilson, Saleena V., David E. Szwedo, and Joseph P. Allen. 2024. "Interparental and Parent–Teen Relationships during Adolescence as Predictors of Intra- and Interpersonal Emotion Regulation in Young Adulthood" Youth 4, no. 4: 1417-1436. https://doi.org/10.3390/youth4040090

APA Style

Wilson, S. V., Szwedo, D. E., & Allen, J. P. (2024). Interparental and Parent–Teen Relationships during Adolescence as Predictors of Intra- and Interpersonal Emotion Regulation in Young Adulthood. Youth, 4(4), 1417-1436. https://doi.org/10.3390/youth4040090

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop