In the following sections, we summarize key findings and various constructs to explain their occurrence. Furthermore, we discuss possible directions for future work that apply these findings and enable further insights.
4.1. Key Findings and Discussion
Conducting this experiment and subsequent analysis enabled us to glean several key insights regarding how DMs use crowdsourced knowledge for organizational decision making and several other associated phenomena. The following list summarizes conclusions and key findings from this experiment, and the following paragraphs provide possible rationales and/or implications:
CTs and DMs had significantly different mental models regarding the relative impact and feasibility of candidate solutions to an organizational inefficiency. There were significant differences in how ideal each solution was perceived to be; however, there was no consistent pattern as to why. Given these differences, there was an opportunity for the CT-generated knowledge to have an actionable impact on the voting of DMs.
Providing crowdsourced organizational knowledge from CTs had no significant impact on DM voting. That is, providing votes and argumentation from CTs caused only minor changes in DM voting, which were not significant in any dimension. These findings add weight to the case for phenomena such as decision inertia, confirmation bias, and anchoring, where DMs seemed to deviate only minimally from perspectives held prior to reviewing the crowdsourced knowledge.
The aspect of CT arguments that had the greatest impact on DM voting was whether the solution conveyed an adequate sense of urgency that was commensurate with the inefficiency to be solved (RS = −0.54, p < 0.01). Despite this relationship, more DM voting was explained by whether they agreed or disagreed with the solution (RS = −0.83, p < 0.01). Thus, we found that DM voting was more strongly influenced by whether they inherently agreed with the solution, rather than any specific aspect of the argument (e.g., clarity or quality of evidence presented).
Of the three key findings, the second was the most surprising (although there exists mixed literature suggesting that results could have gone this way, or differently); therefore, we will spend some effort to discuss why crowdsourced knowledge had no significant impact on DM voting. First, we will discuss more mechanical issues (i.e., low-level issues with how the system was designed and/or study was conducted), followed by more psychological constructs that may have affected the outcome. The SUS survey results regarding the DM user interface (
n = 11,
M = 74.32,
SD = 15.93) were well above the overall mean score of 68 [
35], allowing us to be confident that these findings (i.e., the lack of influence on decision making) were not affected by issues with the software itself.
Because of the nature of each role (where CTs are affected day-to-day by inefficiencies and DMs are responsible for implementing solutions to inefficiencies), one can reasonably assume that each role will look at the same set of problems and solutions from different viewpoints. As such, we find it likely that CTs will be concerned with how well solutions resolve the problem (i.e., the impact), whereas DMs will be focused on the feasibility of the solutions. If CT argumentation focused on the impact of the solutions and DMs place greater weight on the feasibility, it may explain why DMs were unmoved. We asked CTs (n = 13) in a previous experiment (it should be noted that while there were only 13 responses to this particular survey question, but there were 40 participants in each of the previous studies that generated and voted on the top 5 solutions presented to DMs in this study) and DMs (n = 11) in this experiment to indicate the degree to which their decision making was weighted towards impact and feasibility (which must total 100%). As expected, CTs placed a higher emphasis on Solution Impact (M = 48.08, SD = 20.79) than the DMs (M = 41.27, SD = 16.25), although the difference was not significant, F(1,23) = 0.78, p > 0.05, η = 0.03. Further research with a larger sample is required to determine if this phenomenon played a role.
Next, there may have been a ceiling effect in this particular set of solution topics. As part of the rhetorical analysis survey, DMs were asked, “as it is written, do you agree that this is a valid solution that merits implementation?” DMs were able to respond only with Yes (1) or No (0) to this question. Given the sample of 11 DMs and the set of 5 solution topics, the maximum number of votes could equal 55 (in the event that all DMs wanted to implement all solutions). To our surprise, there were a total of 0 Yes votes in the sample. Said differently, not a single DM thought that a single solution merited implementation. It is possible that DMs did not significantly change their voting after being exposed to CT knowledge because they found this particular set of solutions to not be preferable, and that despite their different relative preferability, they all fell below a certain threshold.
At a higher-level, there may have been an issue with psychological ownership—an individual’s perception of whether they own tangible or intangible outcomes of their efforts [
36]. There are at least two types of psychological ownership: Organizational Psychological Ownership (OPO) and Knowledge-Based Psychological Ownership (KPO). Research has shown that increased OPO (the extent to which a person feels ownership of their organization) leads to increased knowledge-sharing behavior. In other words, DMs (who are generally more senior in the organization and have a more personalized perception of what solutions work) may be more likely to rely on their own knowledge when making decisions. This is somewhat supported by UX survey data, where DMs held a slightly negative view on the efficacy of crowdsourcing (i.e., sharing knowledge and collectively making decisions) when given the statement that crowdsourcing is be an effective means to solve problems ((
n = 11,
M = 2.67,
SD = 0.50), where 1 = Strongly Disagree, 5 = Strongly Agree, and 3 = Indifferent). The concepts of OPO and KPO are similar to the concept of “locus of control,” or the degree to which a person attributes outcomes internally or to other external factors [
37]. It would be insightful to understand any differences in CTs and DMs regarding OPO, KPO, and locus of control.
Finally, there are two other constructs to be considered: exchange ideology and organizational distance. Exchange ideology is a disposition referring to the expectations of an employee on what they should offer their organization, and what their organization should offer to them [
38]. A low exchange ideology characterizes a free exchange of information with little regard to the sharer’s “return on investment,” whereas someone with a high exchange ideology is more likely to show more reserve in their knowledge sharing and focus on how they benefit from the exchange. Research has shown that individuals with a high exchange ideology who engage in participative decision making are more receptive to the idea of sharing information. It is possible that there is a disconnect between the participative decision making by CTs and the late-stage participation by the DMs, where DMs were not part of the formative participative decision making and therefore are less likely to value the organizational knowledge generated by the crowd. Organizational distance refers to the way that information exchange in an organization is shaped by the various networks and reward structures present in the organization [
39]; more specifically, knowledge distance refers to the conceptual distance in knowledge transfer between the source and recipient of knowledge. Research suggests that the closer the recipient (DMs, in this case) are to the problem and the people working the problem (the CTs, in this case), the more likely it is that a successful transfer of knowledge will occur. In the case of VARI, DMs being intentionally held distant from CTs (to allow the CTs to work candidly, without fear of reprisal) may have adversely affected knowledge transfer. To a broader degree, DMs do not encounter the same problems as CTs, and the CTs do not deal with the logistics of implementing solutions that DMs do, which may also increase knowledge distance. There is an opportunity in VARI, however, in that continued use could cultivate organizational norms of knowledge exchange.