Emotions, Risk Perception and Preventive Behavior during the COVID-19 Pandemic: The Mediating Role of Media Use
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Literature Review
3. Methods
3.1. Sample
3.2. Questionnaire
- (1)
- Media Exposure: Extent of exposure to news in the media during the pandemic, measured on a five-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (all the time). This variable included four items: (1) How often do you follow developments and news regarding the coronavirus? (2) During the pandemic I follow the news on social media very often. (3) During the COVID-19 crisis I get my news from different sources. (4) During the pandemic, I am exposed to the media more than in ordinary times. The average of the four items was calculated to form the Media Exposure variable. Alpha Cronbach is 0.67.
- (2)
- Emotions: This part of the questionnaire was based on the questionnaire devised by [24], which was translated into Hebrew, retested, adapted to the Israeli situation, and validated in [28] and in [29] in the context of terrorism and war. The version used in the current study was adapted for the COVID-19 pandemic crisis. Participants were asked to rank statements regarding their emotions during the COVID-19 pandemic on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (to a very large degree). This variable included four items: (1) During the pandemic I felt very stressed. (2) During the pandemic I felt very depressed. (3) During the pandemic worries prevented me from sleeping. (4) During the pandemic I felt I was losing my self-confidence. The average of the four items was calculated to form the COVID-19 Negative Emotions variable. Alpha Cronbach is 0.83. In addition, we asked participants to rank four statements regarding the degree of emotions they felt during the pandemic with respect to their media consumption: Media reports about the pandemic made me feel (1) worried; (2) sad; (3) angry; (4) bad. The average of the four items was calculated to form the COVID-19 Media Emotions variable. Alpha Cronbach is 0.83.
- (3)
- Preventive behavior: Participants were asked to rank the frequency of their compliance with Ministry of Health guidelines (MOH) on a five-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (to a very large degree). The two items were: (1) During the pandemic I always comply with the most recent MOH guidelines. (2) I am more careful than usual about hygiene rules during the pandemic. The average of the two items was calculated to form the Preventive Behavior variable. Alpha Cronbach is 0.74.
- (4)
- Risk Attitude: This variable was measured using the validated general risk question from [30]: ‘How willing are you to take risks in general?’ Participants answered on a 10-point scale ranging from 1 (avoid risk at all costs) to 10 (engage in risky activities to a large degree).
- (5)
- COVID-19 Economic Impact: Participants were asked to respond to the following question: ‘To what extent were you or your family financially harmed during the COVID-19 pandemic?’ Participants ranked these items on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (very little or no economic harm) to 5 (significant economic harm).
- (6)
- Exposure to coronavirus disease: The respondents were asked to rank the degree of their exposure to the COVID-19 virus (exposure to an infected person). Values range from 1 (the respondent himself was infected in Corona) to 3 (not exposed and/or infected by Corona at all).
- (7)
- Perceived risk of being infected by coronavirus disease: Participants were asked to rank their chances of being infected in the next 12 months on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (no chance at all) to 5 (to a very large extent).
- (8)
- Socio-demographic data, including gender, age, education, religiosity, and marital status.
4. Results
SEM Analysis
5. Discussion
6. Conclusions
Study Limitations
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A. The Questionnaire
- During the pandemic, I follow developments and media news regarding the COVID-19.
- During the pandemic I follow the news on social media very often.
- During the COVID-19 crisis I get my news from different sources.
- During the pandemic, I am exposed to the media more than in ordinary times.
- During the pandemic I felt very stressed.
- During the pandemic I felt very depressed.
- During the pandemic worries prevented me from sleeping.
- During the pandemic I felt I was losing my self-confidence.
- Media reports about the pandemic made me feel:
- 9.1
- worried
- 9.2
- sad
- 9.3
- angry
- 9.4
- bad.
- During the pandemic I always comply with the most recent MOH guidelines.
- I am more careful than usual about hygiene rules during the pandemic.
- 12.
- How willing are you to take risks in general?
- 13.
- To what extent were you or your family financially harmed during the COVID-19 pandemic?
- 14.
- Rank your chances of being infected with COVID-19 in the next 12 months
- 15.
- Socio-demographic data, including gender, age, education, religiosity, and marital status.
References
- Nan, X.; Iles, I.A.; Yang, B.; Ma, Z. Public health messaging during the COVID-19 pandemic and beyond: Lessons from communication science. Health Commun. 2022, 37, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chou, W.Y.S.; Budenz, A. Considering emotion in COVID-19 vaccine communication: Addressing vaccine hesitancy and fostering vaccine confidence. Health Commun. 2020, 35, 1718–1722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Oh, S.H.; Lee, S.Y.; Han, C. The effects of social media use on preventive behaviors during infectious disease outbreaks: The mediating role of self-relevant emotions and public risk perception. Health Commun. 2020, 36, 972–981. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chan, M.; Pui, S.; Winneg, K.; Hawkins, L.; Farhadloo, M.; Jamieson, K.H.; Albarracín, D. Legacy and social media respectively influence risk perceptions and protective behaviors during emerging health threats: A multi-wave analysis of communications on Zika virus cases. Soc. Sci. Med. 2018, 212, 50–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bakir, V. Media and risk: Old and new research directions. J. Risk Res. 2010, 13, 5–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shim, M.; You, M. Cognitive and affective risk perceptions toward food safety: Mediating the relation between news use and food consumption intention. Asian J. Commun. 2015, 25, 48–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jones, R.; Mougouei, D.; Evans, S.L. Understanding the emotional response to COVID-19 information in news and social media: A mental health perspective. Hum. Behav. Emerg. Technol. 2021, 3, 832–842. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wheaton, M.G.; Abramowitz, J.S.; Berman, N.C.; Fabricant, L.E.; Olatunji, B.O. Psychological predictors of anxiety in response to the H1N1 (swine flu) pandemic. Cogn. Ther. Res. 2012, 36, 210–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lahav, E.; Shahrabani, S.; Benzion, U. Emotions, risk perceptions and precautionary actions of citizens during a military operation using a new defence technology: The Israeli case of the iron dome. Def. Peace Econ. 2019, 30, 666–686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Witteveen, D.; Velthorst, E. Economic hardship and mental health complaints during COVID-19. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2020, 117, 27277–27284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Griffin, R.J.; Dunwoody, S.; Neuwirth, K. Proposed model of the relationship of risk information seeking and processing to the development of preventive behaviors. Environ. Res. 1999, 80 Pt 2, S230–S245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ratcliff, C.L.; Wicke, R.; Harvill, B. Communicating uncertainty to the public during the COVID-19 pandemic: A scoping review of the literature. Ann. Int. Commun. Assoc. 2022, 46, 260–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Park, T.; Ju, I.; Ohs, J.E.; Hinsley, A.; Muzumdar, J. Information seeking during the COVID-19 pandemic: Application of the risk information seeking and processing model. Res. Soc. Adm. Pharm. 2023, 19, 956–964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [PubMed Central]
- Yang, Z.J. Too scared or too capable? why do college students stay away from the H1N1 vaccine? Risk Anal. 2012, 32, 1703–1716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cross, M.; Heeren, A.; Cornicelli, L.J.; Fulton, D.C. Bovine tuberculosis management in northwest Minnesota and implications of the risk information seeking and processing (RISP) model for wildlife disease management. Front. Vet. Sci. 2018, 5, 190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Galoni, C.; Carpenter, G.S.; Rao, H. Disgusted and afraid: Consumer choices under the threat of contagious disease. J. Consum. Res. 2020, 47, 373–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yoo, W.; Paek, H.; Hove, T. Differential effects of content-oriented versus user-oriented social media on risk perceptions and behavioral intentions. Health Commun. 2018, 35, 99–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Frijda, N.H. The Emotions; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Steinert, S. Corona and value change. The role of social media and emotional contagion. Ethics Inf. Technol. 2021, 23, 59–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Medford, R.J.; Saleh, S.N.; Sumarsono, A.; Perl, T.M.; Lehmann, C.U. An “infodemic”: Leveraging high-volume Twitter data to understand early public sentiment for the coronavirus disease 2019 outbreak. Open Forum Infect. Dis. 2020, 7, ofaa258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmidt, A.; Brose, A.; Kramer, A.C.; Schmiedek, F.; Witthöft, M.; Neubauer, A.B. Cyclical across-day dynamics of corona-related media exposure and worries in people’s daily lives during the COVID-19 pandemic. arXiv 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liu, Y.; Duong, H.T.; Nguyen, H.T. Media exposure and intentions to wear face masks in the early stages of the COVID-19 outbreak: The mediating role of negative emotions and risk perception. Atl. J. Commun. 2021, 30, 467–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bauldry, S.; Stainback, K. Media consumption and psychological distress among older adults in the United States. PLoS ONE 2022, 17, e0279587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lerner, J.S.; Gonzalez, R.M.; Small, D.A.; Fischhoff, B. Effects of fear and anger on perceived risks of terrorism: A national field experiment. Psychol. Sci. 2003, 14, 144–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Choi, D.; Park, K. Exploring risk perception and intention to engage in social and economic activities during the South Korean MERS outbreak. Int. J. Commun. 2018, 12, 21. [Google Scholar]
- Lanciano, T.; Graziano, G.; Curci, A.; Costadura, S.; Monaco, A. Risk perceptions and psychological effects during the Italian COVID-19 emergency. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 580053. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kim, S.; Cho, S.K.; LoCascio, S.P. The role of media use and emotions in risk perception and preventive behaviors related to COVID-19 in South Korea. Asian J. Public Opin. Res. 2020, 8, 297–323. [Google Scholar]
- Benzion, U.; Shahrabani, S.; Shavit, T. Emotions and perceived risks after the 2006 Israel-Lebanon war. Mind Soc. 2009, 8, 21–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosenboim, M.; Benzion, U.; Shahrabani, S.; Shavit, T. Emotions, risk perceptions, and precautionary behavior under the threat of terror attacks: A field study among Israeli college students. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2012, 25, 248–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dohmen, T.; Falk, A.; Huffman, D.; Sunde, U.; Schupp, J.; Wagner, G.G. Individual risk attitudes: Measurement, determinants, and behavioral consequences. J. Eur. Econ. Assoc. 2011, 9, 522–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alheialy, O.S.; Mohammed, M.J. A comparison among robust estimation methods for structural equations modeling with ordinal categorical variables. Iraqi J. Stat. Sci. 2020, 17, 132–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xia, Y.; Yang, Y. RMSEA, CFI, and TLI in structural equation modeling with ordered categorical data: The story they tell depends on the estimation methods. Behav. Res. Methods 2019, 51, 409–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, E.J.; Tversky, A. Affect, generalization, and the perception of risk. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1983, 45, 20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wright, W.F.; Bower, G.H. Mood effects on subjective probability assessment. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1992, 52, 276–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shahrabani, S.; Benzion, U.; Shavit, T. Recall emotions and risk judgments: Field study of the 2006 Israel-Lebanon war. J. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2009, 4, 326–336. [Google Scholar]
- Shavit, T.; Shahrabani, S.; Benzion, U.; Rosenboim, M. The effect of a forest fire disaster on emotions and perceptions of risk: A field study after the Carmel fire. J. Environ. Psychol. 2013, 36, 129–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lahav, E.; Rosenboim, M.; Shahrabani, S.; Song, Y. Optimism and precautionary measures during the COVID-19 outbreak in China. Am. J. Health Behav. 2021, 45, 978–992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tsutsui, Y.; Shahrabani, S.; Yamamura, E.; Hayashi, R.; Kohsaka, Y.; Ohtake, F. The willingness to pay for a hypothetical vaccine for the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19). Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 12450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Abraham, T. The price of poor pandemic communication. BMJ 2010, 340, c2952. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yeo, S.L.; Phua, D.Y.; Hong, Y.Y. The effects of dangerous world beliefs on COVID-19 preventive behaviors in Singapore. The moderating role of public health communication. Int. J. Strateg. Commun. 2022, 16, 485–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hanson, C.; Luedtke, S.; Spicer, N.; Stilhoff Sörensen, J.; Mayhew, S.; Mounier-Jack, S. National health governance, science and the media: Drivers of COVID-19 responses in Germany, Sweden and the UK in 2020. BMJ Glob. Health 2021, 6, e006691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vaala, S.E.; Ritter, M.B.; Palakshappa, D. Framing effects on US adults’ reactions to COVID-19 public health messages: Moderating role of source trust. Am. Behav. Sci. 2022, 00027642221124664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
(N = 780) | Mean | S.D. | Min. | Max. |
---|---|---|---|---|
Female | 49.10% | |||
Arabs | 33.20% | |||
Not Married | 35.40% | |||
Age | 38.14 | 13.75 | 18 | 70 |
Income | 3.62 | 1.13 | 1 | 5 |
Education level (years) | 14.47 | 2.47 | 10 | 25 |
Religiosity level | 3.20 | 0.80 | 1 | 5 |
Risk Attitude | 3.93 | 2.19 | 1 | 10 |
Mean | S.D. | |
---|---|---|
COVID-19 economic impact | 3.08 | 1.31 |
Exposure to coronavirus disease | 1.99 | 0.16 |
Perceived risk of infection | 2.36 | 0.9 |
Media exposure | 3.43 | 0.76 |
COVID-19 negative emotions | 2.52 | 0.94 |
COVID-19 media emotions | 3.12 | 0.85 |
Preventive behavior | 4.35 | 0.73 |
Gender | Social Group | Marital Status | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Male | Female | t | Jews | Arabs | t | Married | Unmarried | t | ||
N | 383 | 397 | 521 | 259 | 504 | 276 | ||||
COVID-19 economic impact | M + | 3.04 | 3.11 | 0.683 | 2.82 | 3.59 | 8.056 ** | 3.03 | 3.16 | 1.248 |
SD ++ | 1.33 | 1.28 | 1.27 | 1.23 | 1.32 | 1.29 | ||||
Perceived infection risk | M | 2.31 | 2.41 | 1.549 | 2.29 | 2.51 | 3.225 ** | 2.43 | 2.24 | 2.843 ** |
SD | 0.89 | 0.91 | 0.86 | 0.97 | 0.91 | 0.88 | ||||
Media exposure | M | 3.38 | 3.47 | 1.628 | 3.33 | 3.62 | 5.120 ** | 3.46 | 3.36 | 1.705 |
SD | 0.74 | 0.78 | 0.73 | 0.78 | 0.76 | 0.77 | ||||
COVID-19 negative emotions | M | 2.49 | 2.56 | 1.026 | 2.38 | 2.82 | 6.416 ** | 2.52 | 2.53 | 0.069 |
SD | 0.97 | 0.91 | 0.89 | 0.96 | 0.96 | 0.91 | ||||
COVID-19 media emotions | M | 3 | 3.24 | 3.881 ** | 3.06 | 3.26 | 3.108 ** | 3.14 | 3.1 | 0.674 |
SD | 0.89 | 0.79 | 0.81 | 0.9 | 0.83 | 0.88 | ||||
Preventive behavior | M | 4.33 | 4.37 | 0.844 | 4.28 | 4.49 | 3.822 ** | 4.36 | 4.33 | 0.438 |
SD | 0.74 | 0.72 | 0.69 | 0.77 | 0.68 | 0.81 |
Age | Education (Years) | Religiosity Level | |
---|---|---|---|
COVID-19 economic impact | −0.151 ** | −0.126 ** | −0.115 ** |
Perceived risk of infection | −0.030 | 0.126 ** | 0.010 |
Media exposure | −0.030 | 0.033 | −0.043 |
COVID-19 negative emotions | −0.219 ** | −0.057 | −0.024 |
COVID-19 media emotions | −0.023 | 0.016 | 0.000 |
Preventive behavior | 0.000 | −0.069 | −0.047 |
COVID-19 Economic Impact | Perceived Infection Risk | Media Exposure | COVID-19 Negative Emotions | COVID-19 Media Emotions | Preventive Behavior | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
COVID-19 economic impact | 1 | |||||
Perceived infection risk | 0.114 ** | 1 | ||||
Media exposure | 0.171 ** | 0.097 ** | 1 | |||
COVID-19 negative emotions | 0.338 ** | 0.169 ** | 0.276 ** | 1 | ||
COVID-19 media emotions | 0.198 ** | 0.134 ** | 0.290 ** | 0.376 ** | 1 | |
Preventive behavior | 0.146 ** | −0.013 | 0.275 ** | 0.114 ** | 0.116 ** | 1 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Shahrabani, S.; Garyn-Tal, S. Emotions, Risk Perception and Preventive Behavior during the COVID-19 Pandemic: The Mediating Role of Media Use. COVID 2024, 4, 872-883. https://doi.org/10.3390/covid4070059
Shahrabani S, Garyn-Tal S. Emotions, Risk Perception and Preventive Behavior during the COVID-19 Pandemic: The Mediating Role of Media Use. COVID. 2024; 4(7):872-883. https://doi.org/10.3390/covid4070059
Chicago/Turabian StyleShahrabani, Shosh, and Sharon Garyn-Tal. 2024. "Emotions, Risk Perception and Preventive Behavior during the COVID-19 Pandemic: The Mediating Role of Media Use" COVID 4, no. 7: 872-883. https://doi.org/10.3390/covid4070059
APA StyleShahrabani, S., & Garyn-Tal, S. (2024). Emotions, Risk Perception and Preventive Behavior during the COVID-19 Pandemic: The Mediating Role of Media Use. COVID, 4(7), 872-883. https://doi.org/10.3390/covid4070059