An Approach to Assessing Zoo Animal Welfare in a Rarely Studied Species, the Common Cusimanse Crossarchus obscurus
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Crossarchus obscurus
1.2. Crossarchus obscurus in Captivity
Crossarchus obscurus at Marwell Zoo
2. Parameters of the AWAG
2.1. Physical
2.2. Psychological
2.3. Environmental
2.4. Procedural
3. Application of the Template to Real Data
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Broom, D.M. Animal welfare defined in terms of attempts to cope with the environment. Acta Agric. Scand. Sect. A Anim. Sci. Suppl. 1996, 27, 22–28. [Google Scholar]
- Bracke, M.B.; Spruijt, B.M.; Metz, J.H. Overall animal welfare assessment reviewed. Part 1: Is it possible? Neth. J. Agric. Sci. 1999, 47, 279–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buller Blokhuis, H.; Jensen, P.; Keeling, L. Towards Farm Animal Welfare and Sustainability. Animals 2018, 8, 81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Farm Animal Welfare Council. Farm Animal Welfare in Great Britain: Past, Present and Future; Farm Animal Welfare Council: London, UK, 2009.
- Justice, W.S.M.; O’Brien, M.F.; Szyszka, O.; Shotton, J.; Gilmour, J.E.M.; Riordan, P.; Wolfensohn, S. Adaptation of the animal welfare assessment grid (AWAG) for monitoring animal welfare in zoological collections. Vet. Rec. 2017, 181, 143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolfensohn, S.; Shotton, J.; Bowley, H.; Davies, S.; Thompson, S.; Justice, W.S.M. Assessment of welfare in zoo animals: Towards optimum quality of life. Animals 2018, 8, 110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brouwers, S.P.; José, M.; Duchateau, H.M. Feasibility and validity of the Animal Welfare Assessment Grid to monitor the welfare of zoo-housed gorillas Gorilla gorilla gorilla. J. Zoo Aquar. Res. 2021, 9, 208–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Angelici, F.M.; di Vittorio, M. Common Cusimanse Crossarchus obscurus in Ghana and Flat-headed Cusimanse C. platycephalus in Nigeria: A tentative comparison between habitat parameters affecting their distribution. Small Carniv. Conserv. 2013, 48, 96–100. [Google Scholar]
- Goldman, C.A. Crossarchus obscurus. Mamm. Species 1987, 290, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kingdon, J. The Kingdon Field Guide to African Mammals; Academic Press: London, UK, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Djagoun, C.A.M.S.; Akpona, H.A.; Sinsin, B.; Mensah, G.A.; Dossa, N.F. Mongoose species in southern Benin: Preliminary ecological survey and local community perceptions. Mammalia 2009, 73, 27–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Angelici, F.; do Lin Sanh, E. Crossarchus Obscurus, Common Cusimanse. The IUCN Red List of Threatened SpeciesTM. 2015. Available online: https://doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.UK.2015-4.RLTS.T41595A45205532.en (accessed on 13 June 2022). [CrossRef]
- Olson, A.L. The behavior and ecology of the long-nosed mongoose, Crossarchus obscurus. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Nowak, R.M.; Walker, E.P. Carnivora; Viverridae: Cusimanses. In Walker’s Mammals of the World, 6th ed.; Johns Hopkins University Press: Baltimore, MD, USA, 1999; Volume 1, p. 1168. [Google Scholar]
- Hunter, L. Carnivores of the World, 1st ed.; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Jennings, A.; Veron, G. Mongooses of the World; Whittles Publishing, Ltd.: Dunbeath, UK, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Estes, R.D. Genets, Civets, and Mongooses: Family Viverridae. In The Behaviour Guide to African Mammals: Including Hoofed Mammals, Carnivores, Primates; University of California Press, Ltd.: London, UK, 1991; pp. 278–322. [Google Scholar]
- Decker, D.M.; Ringelberg, D.; White, D.C. Lipid components in anal scent sacs of three mongoose species (Helogale parvula, Crossarchus obscurus, Suricata suricatta). J. Chem. Ecol. 1992, 18, 1511–1524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Struhsaker, T.T.; McKey, D. Two Cusimanse Mongooses Attack a Black Cobra. J. Mammal. 1975, 56, 721–722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Djagoun, C.A.M.S.; Gaubert, P. Small carnivorans from southern Benin: A preliminary assessment of diversity and hunting pressure. Small Carniv. Conserv. 2009, 40, 1–10. [Google Scholar]
- Bush, E.R.; Jeffery, K.; Bunnefeld, N.; Tutin, C.; Musgrave, R.; Moussavou, G.; Mihindou, V.; Malhi, Y.; Lehmann, D.; Ndong, J.E.; et al. Rare ground data confirm significant warming and drying in western equatorial Africa. PeerJ 2020, 8, e8732. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- AZA Small Carnivore TAG. Mongoose, Meerkat, & Fossa (Herpestidae/Eupleridae) Care Manual; Association of Zoos and Aquariums: Silver Spring, MD, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Brando, S.; Buchanan-Smith, H.M. The 24/7 approach to promoting optimal welfare for captive wild animals. Behav. Processes 2018, 156, 83–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Queiroz, M.B.; Young, R.J. The Different Physical and Behavioural Characteristics of Zoo Mammals That Influence Their Response to Visitors. Animals 2018, 8, 139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sherwen, S.L.; Hemsworth, P.H. The Visitor Effect on Zoo Animals: Implications and Opportunities for Zoo Animal Welfare. Animals 2019, 9, 366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Zeeland, Y.; Schoemaker, N. Current anaesthetic considerations and techniques in rabbits Part I: Pre-anaesthetic considerations and commonly used analgesics and anaesthetics. EJCAP 2014, 24, 19–30. [Google Scholar]
- Totton, J. Cholesterol in the Common Cusimanse (Crossarchus obscurus). An Intake Study: Observing the Effects of a Diet Change. Master’s Thesis, Zoo Conservation Biology Research Project, Plymouth University, Plymouth, UK, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Totten, J.; Plowman, A. A Potentially Cholesterol-Reducing Diet is Palatable and Practical for Cusimanse Crossarchus obscurus; Whitley Wildlife Conservation Trust (Paignton Zoo Environmental Park); Biological Sciences, Plymouth University: Plymouth, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Arnold, C. Personal Communication; Marwell Zoo: Winchester, UK, 2022. [Google Scholar]
- Flecknell, P.A.; Cruz, I.J.; Liles, J.H.; Whelan, G. Induction of anaesthesia with halothane and isoflurane in the rabbit: A comparison of the use of a face-mask or an anaesthetic chamber. Lab. Anim. 1996, 30, 67–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Greening, L. Stereotypies and other abnormal behavior in welfare assessment. In Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior, 2nd ed.; Choe, J., Ed.; Academic Press: Gloucester, UK, 2019; Volume 1, pp. 141–146. [Google Scholar]
- Department for Environment Food and Rural Affairs. Zoos Expert Committee Handbook; Department for Environment Food and Rural Affairs: Bristol, UK, 2012.
- Wechsler, B. Coping and coping strategies: A behavioural view. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1995, 43, 123–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolfensohn, S.; Lloyd, M. Handbook of Laboratory Animal Management and Welfare, 4th ed.; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Wolfensohn, S.; Sharpe, S.; Hall, I.; Lawrence, S.; Kitchen, S.; Dennis, M. Refinement of welfare through development of a quantitative system for assessment of lifetime experience. Anim. Welf. 2015, 24, 139–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Freeman, L.M.; Lachaud, M.P.; Matthews, S.; Rhodes, L.; Zollers, B. Evaluation of Weight Loss over Time in Cats with Chronic Kidney Disease. J. Vet. Intern. Med. 2016, 30, 1661–1666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Morfeld, K.A.; Meehan, C.L.; Hogan, J.N.; Brown, J.L. Assessment of Body Condition in African (Loxodonta africana) and Asian (Elephas maximus) Elephants in North American Zoos and Management Practices Associated with High Body Condition Scores. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0155146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Teng, K.T.; McGreevy, P.D.; Toribio, J.A.L.M.L.; Raubenheimer, D.; Kendall, K.; Dhand, N.K. Associations of body condition score with health conditions related to overweight and obesity in cats. J. Small Anim. Pract. 2018, 59, 603–615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hut, P.R.; Hostens, M.M.; Beijaard, M.J.; van Eerdenburg, F.J.C.M.; Hulsen, J.H.J.L.; Hooijer, G.A.; Stassen, E.N.; Nielen, M. Associations between body condition score, locomotion score, and sensor-based time budgets of dairy cattle during the dry period and early lactation. J. Dairy Sci. 2021, 104, 4746–4763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schiffmann, C.; Clauss, M.; Hoby, S.; Hatt, J.M. Visual body condition scoring in zoo animals—Composite, algorithm and overview approaches. J. Zoo Aquar. Res. 2017, 5, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Binding, S.; Farmer, H.; Krusin, L.; Cronin, K. Status of animal welfare research in zoos and aquariums: Where are we, where to next? J. Zoo Aquar. Res. 2020, 8, 166–174. [Google Scholar]
- Moore, R.A.; Waheed, A.; Burns, B. Rule of Nines. In StatPearls [Internet]; StatPearls Publishing: Treasure Island, FL, USA, 2021. [Google Scholar]
- Hosey, G. A preliminary model of human-animal relationships in the zoo. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 109, 105–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin, R.A.; Melfi, V. A Comparison of Zoo Animal Behavior in the Presence of Familiar and Unfamiliar People. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2016, 19, 234–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mellor, D.J.; Beausoleil, N.J. Extending the ‘Five Domains’ model for animal welfare assessment to incorporate positive welfare states. Anim. Welf. 2015, 24, 241–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sharma, A.; Phillips, C.J.C. Avoidance distance in sheltered cows and its association with other welfare parameters. Animals 2019, 9, 396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watters, J. Searching for behavioral indicators of welfare in zoos: Uncovering anticipatory behavior. Zoo Biol. 2014, 33, 251–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ward, S.J.; Sherwen, S.; Clark, F.E. Advances in applied zoo animal welfare science. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2018, 21, 23–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Boissy, A.; Manteuffel, G.; Jensen, M.B.; Moe, R.O.; Spruijt, B.; Keeling, L.J.; Winckler, C.; Forkman, B.; Dimitrov, I.; Langbein, J.; et al. Assessment of positive emotions in animals to improve their welfare. Physiol. Behav. 2007, 92, 375–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jones, N.; Sherwen, S.L.; Robbins, R.; McLelland, D.J.; Whittaker, A.L. Welfare Assessment Tools in Zoos: From Theory to Practice. Vet. Sci. 2022, 9, 170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miller, L.J.; Vicino, G.A.; Sheftel, J.; Lauderdale, L.K. Behavioral Diversity as a Potential Indicator of Positive Animal Welfare. Animals 2020, 10, 1211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mellor, D.J.; Beausoleil, N.J.; Littlewood, K.E.; McLean, A.N.; McGreevy, P.D.; Jones, B.; Wilkins, C. The 2020 Five Domains Model: Including Human–Animal Interactions in Assessments of Animal Welfare. Animals 2020, 10, 1870. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Body Condition Score | Food Intake | Activity Levels (Based on Wild Behaviour). Diurnal Species. | Faecal Consistency | Clinical Assessment (Excluding Skin Condition) | Skin Condition | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Score | ||||||
1 | 3—Ideal | Eating normally. No signs of increased/decreased hunger. All food is consumed. | Normal balance between activity and resting. Little to no sleeping seen during the day. Activity begins around sunrise and continues throughout day until sunset or drop in temperature, with periods of resting. | Normal | Nothing observed | Skin looks healthy, no dryness or flaking. No wounds. |
2 | More/less than 3 but not quite 2.5/3.5 | Food intake slightly lower than normal for one day OR animal reported hungry | Slight increase/decrease in activity not related to normal daily variation | Loose (below 10% of total) or clumpy (below 30%) | Mild clinical signs that show no impact on the animal’s ability to perform normal behaviours. Full recovery expected | Small area/s of dry, flaky skin visible. |
3 | 2.5/3.5—Slightly over/under | Food intake significantly lower than normal for one day OR reported hungry for 2–3 days | Moderate increase/decrease in activity (no obvious cause)- showing full recovery within 8 h | Loose (below 20% of total) or clumpy (below 40%) | Mild clinical signs having a short term impact on the animal’s ability to perform normal behaviours. Full recovery expected. | Large area/s of dry, flaky skin visible. |
4 | More/less than 2.5/3.5 but not quite 2/4 | Food intake slightly lower than normal for 2 days (lower than 80%) OR reported hungry for 4–5 days | Moderate increase/decrease in activity (no obvious cause)- showing full recovery within 12 h | Loose (below 30% of total) or clumpy (below 50%) | Mild clinical signs having a longer term impact on the animal’s ability to perform normal behaviours. Full recovery expected. | Healing/scabbed sore/s |
5 | 2/4—Over or under | Food intake significantly lower for 2 days (lower than 50%) OR reported hungry for 6–7 days | Significant increase/decrease in activity (no obvious cause)—showing full recovery within 12 h | Loose (below 40% of total) or clumpy (below 60%) | Moderate clinical signs having limited impact on the animal’s ability to perform normal behaviours. Full recovery expected | One small open sore |
6 | More/less than 2/4 but not quite 1.5/4.5 | Food intake slightly lower than normal for 3 days (lower than 80%) OR reported hungry for 8–9 days | Significant increase/decrease in activity (no obvious cause)—not showing full recovery to normal or recovery taking over 12 h. | Loose (below 50% of total) or clumpy (below 70%) | Moderate clinical signs having limited impact on the animal’s ability to perform normal behaviours. Recovery potential unknown. | One medium OR multiple small open sores OR multiple medium/large wounds scabbed and beginning to heal |
7 | 1.5/4.5—Very over/under | Food intake significantly lower than normal for 3 days (lower than 50%) OR reported hungry for 10–11 days | Inactive but does get up to eat/drink/defecate OR very active throughout the day with not much rest | Loose (below 60% of total) or clumpy (below 80%) | Moderate clinical signs with medium to long term impact on animals ability to perform normal behaviours. Recovery potential unknown. | Multiple medium open sores OR one large open sore OR open wound on face, ear/s, groin or foot/feet |
8 | More/less than 1.5/4.5 but not quite 1/5 | No sign animal has eaten for 1 day OR reported hungry for 12–13 days | Very minimal movement/signs of hyperactivity | All clumpy or all loose | Severe clinical signs but with short term impact and expected recovery OR moderate to severe signs with long term impact on animal’s welfare and little chance of recovery. | One large open wound present for >1 week <4 weeks |
9 | 1/5—Very Thin/Obese | No sign animal has eaten for 2 days or reported hungry for 14–15 days | Completely hyperactive OR inactive | All watery diarrhoea | Severe or chronic clinical signs that are having serious negative impact on the animal’s ability to perform normal behaviours | Multiple open wounds, various locations on body for <4 weeks OR one large open wound present for >4 weeks. |
10 | 0—Emaciated/Starving OR 6—Morbidly Obese | No sign animal has eaten for 3 days OR reported hungry for >15 days | Animal causing itself harm through inactivity or hyperactivity OR completely recumbent | Presence of abnormal elements e.g., mucus or blood | Severe clinical signs that are rendering the animal recumbent/unable to carry out any normal behaviour | Multiple open wounds, various locations on body, present for >4 weeks |
Abnormal Behaviour (Overgrooming, Barbering, Increased Scratching—For Hair Loss (Use ‘Rule Of Nines’) | Response to Presence of Unfamiliar People (e.g., Guests, Contractors, Keepers from Other Sections) | Response to Normal Events | Response to Restricted Access to Part of the Enclosure | Social Interaction (with Conspecifics) | Species-Typical Behaviours—Either Observed Occurring or Evidence of | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Score | ||||||
1 | No hair loss, coat is in healthy condition, looking bright and glossy. No abnormal behaviour or frequency of behaviour observed. | Actively seeks out familiar and unfamiliar people. Appears to prefer parts of enclosure close to people. | Actively seeks out keeper during normal events, exhibiting positive behaviours. | No restricted access to part of the enclosure | Animals are interacting with one another positively. Exhibiting affiliative behaviours e.g., bundling, frequently | Animal is observed performing all expected natural, primarily positive, behaviours (e.g., group foraging, exploration, scent marking, walking, running, trotting, climbing, resting, sleeping, bundling, digging, hunting, foraging with nose, foraging in vegetation, egg smashing, stalking, sun bathing, sniffing, drinking, aggression, allogrooming, mating, scratching, stretching, play, predator defence and hiding) at expected rate with no abnormal behaviours. |
2 | No hair loss but coat is looking dull, greasy or wet AND/OR limited time spent, low frequency, distractable | Completely habituated to presence of unfamiliar people, no concern shown. No preference for particular parts of enclosure noticeable. | Well habituated to keeper interactions/daily events, no response | Animal comes in easily with call and no further intervention required. Animal remains calm until released. | Animals are interacting with one another positively. Exhibiting affiliative behaviours e.g., bundling, occasionally | Animal is observed performing most natural behaviours as expected in captivity (may not include, for example, mating, egg smashing or predator defence) at expected rate with no abnormal behaviours. |
3 | Hair is thinning in patches but there are no bald areas AND/OR limited time spent, low frequency, distractable | Preference to avoid areas close to viewing areas but does use rest of enclosure. No signs of stress observed. | Anticipatory behaviour begins on seeing keeper heading to enclosure but stops immediately on keeper arrival. Well habituated to keeper interactions/daily events. | Animal well trained and/or habituated and comes in easily but intervention/enticement is required. Animal remains calm until released. | Animal interacts with conspecifics without affiliative behaviours or fear/stress/aggression (exhibited/received) | Animal is observed performing a wide variety of positive natural behaviours including: foraging, stalking/hunting, digging, locomotion, social, self-maintenance, climbing, resting, at expected rate with no abnormal behaviours. |
4 | 1–5% hair loss AND/OR moderate time spent, medium frequency, distractable | Spends most of the day away from viewing areas or hiding out of sight, utilises enclosure when zoo is closed. No signs of stress observed | Well habituated to most keeper interactions/daily events but mild stress seen for single specific interaction. | Animal shows some reluctance to come in and mild signs of stress. Takes between 5–15 minutes to get the animal inside/attempt abandoned. OR animal is showing signs of stress as a result of attempts to shut conspecific in. AND/OR mild signs of stress when shut in. | Animal has opportunity to interact with conspecific but choses not to. No fear/stress/aggression (exhibited/received) noted. | Animal is observed only performing fundamental behaviours such as feeding, resting, drinking, urinating, defecating but no negative or abnormal behaviours. |
5 | >5–15% hair loss AND/OR moderate time spent, higher frequency, no damage done and distractable OR 2–4 and not distractable | Preference to avoid viewing areas/hide out of sight but does use rest of enclosure for some of the day. Shows mild signs of stress when unfamiliar people around but recovers when they leave area. | Shows some mild signs of stress during normal daily interactions but recovers as soon as interaction is over AND/OR short-term anticipatory behaviour in lead up to keeper arrival for a single routine event. | Animal very reluctant to come in and shows moderate signs of stress. Single attempt required but takes over 15 minutes/abandoned after 15 mins. | Animal shows mild fear/stress/aggression (exhibited/received) in interactions with conspecifics which is temporary e.g., around food. Majority of interactions are normal. | Animal is observed performing a wide variety of positive natural behaviours including: foraging, stalking/hunting, digging, locomotion, social, self-maintenance, climbing, resting, at expected rate with some negative or abnormal behaviours. |
6 | >15–25% hair loss AND/OR significant time spent, higher frequency, not distractable | Spends most of the day away from viewing areas/hide out of sight and show mild signs of stress when unfamiliar people are around but recovers when they leave area. | Shows some moderate signs of stress during normal daily interactions AND/OR short-term anticipatory behaviour in lead up to keeper arrival for all routine events. | Animal very reluctant to come in and shows moderate signs of stress. Multiple attempts required/abandoned after multiple attempts. AND/OR moderate signs of stress when shut in. | Animal shows moderate fear/stress/aggression (exhibited/received) in interactions with conspecifics that is temporary. | Animal is observed primarily performing fundamental behaviours such as feeding, resting, drinking as well as a few positive behaviours (e.g., digging, climbing or self-maintenance) with an increase of negative or abnormal behaviour. |
7 | >25–35% hair loss AND/OR significant time spent, higher frequency, not distractable | Avoid viewing areas/hide out of sight and show moderate signs of stress when unfamiliar people around but recovers when they leave area. | Shows significant stress behaviour during normal daily interactions AND/OR considerable time spent in anticipatory behaviour in lead up to keeper arrival for a single routine event. | Animal very reluctant to come in and showing severe signs of stress/fear. Significant time (over half an hour) and/or multiple attempts to get in/abandoned. | Animals shows moderate fear/stress/aggression (exhibited/received) in interactions with conspecifics OR no opportunity to interact with conspecific (gregarious species) | Animal is observed performing fundamental behaviours such as feeding, resting, drinking but spending some time on negative or abnormal behaviours. |
8 | >35–45% AND/OR majority of time spent, high frequency, not distractable. | Significant signs of stress when unfamiliar people are around but recover within an hour when they leave area. | Shows significant stress/fear behaviour during normal daily interactions AND/OR considerable time spent in anticipatory behaviour in lead up to keeper arrival for all routine events. | Animal very reluctant to come in and showing severe signs of stress/fear. Significant time spent (over 1 hour). AND/OR showing severe signs of stress when shut in. | Animal shows severe fear/stress/aggression (exhibited/received) in most interactions with conspecifics. | Animal is observed performing fundamental behaviours such as feeding, resting, drinking but spending greater proportion of time exhibiting negative or abnormal behaviours. |
9 | >45–55% AND/OR majority of time spent, very high frequency, not distractable. | Significant signs of stress when unfamiliar people are present. Takes up to 8 h to recover when they leave area. | Animal is stressed and aggressive during normal daily routine events | Animal extremely difficult to get in and showing aggressive behaviour in response to attempts | Animal shows fear/stress/aggression (exhibited/received) in all interactions with conspecifics. | Animal is observed performing fundamental behaviours such as feeding, resting, drinking but spending most of their time exhibiting negative or abnormal behaviours. |
10 | >55% hair loss AND/OR majority of time spent, very high frequency, not distractable. | Significant signs of stress AND/OR aggression/self harm in response to presence of unfamiliar people. | Animal is self-harming as a result of a normal or routine event. | Animal harming itself and/or conspecifics as a result of being shut in or attempts at shutting in. | Animal is aggressive and either self harming or harming conspecific. | Complete lack of natural behaviour observed, overwhelming abnormal or negative behaviour exhibited (e.g., aggression, hiding, pacing, self-directed or escape behaviours) |
Enclosure (Species Specific, e.g., Size, Lighting, Shelter, Ventilation, Temperature, Drainage, Noise Levels, Substrate etc.) | Enclosure Complexity (Species Specific, e.g., Planting, Water Bodies, Food, Shelter, Choice, Hiding Places, Furniture, Sunlight/Heat Lamp) Plus Opportunities Provided by Keepers | Group Size/Structure (Based on Wild Size and Composition) | Contingent Events (e.g., Animal Movement, Enclosure Changes, Building Works, Visitor Event) | Nutrition | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Score | |||||
1 | Enclosure mirrors the species’ wild habitat preference (tropical rainforest, transitional forest, logged forest, agricultural land), size (20–30 ha) with access to temperature >22 degrees C, >60% humidity, 12/12 photoperiod, shelter from inclement weather and sun, guest viewing less than 360 degrees, off show area, ventilation, UVB, low noise level, adequate drainage and substrate. | Enclosure complexity is reflective of the wild environment, including waterbody/ies, suitable substrates for foraging and digging burrows, dense vegetation plus some open areas, climbing opportunities, ability to sleep at height as a group in a variety of locations, and be able to rest as a group elsewhere, variety of weather conditions. All natural behaviours can be expressed. | Group size is reflective of natural wild group size (4–20 individuals) and suitable group structure (dominant breeding pair and young from current or previous litters, both adult males and females, groups appear fairly stable). Stocking density is appropriate for the enclosure. | None | Diet available is optimally suited to the species-specific needs (nutritional, physiological and behavioural (natural acquisition and manipulation)) and the individual. Diet includes a variety of tastes, textures and smells. |
2 | Enclosure is smaller than wild territory but mirrors other elements | All natural behaviours can be expressed with little reliance on keepers | Group size and structure is similar to wild. No stress observed and natural behaviours seen from all of the group. | External works/visitor event with minimal disturbance | Diet provided is suited to the species-specific needs (nutritional, physiological and behavioural (natural acquisition and manipulation)) and the individual but regularly lacks variety. |
3 | Enclosure is smaller than wild territory and lacks one other element. | All natural behaviours can be expressed with considerable reliance on keepers | Group size and structure are dissimilar to the wild but no stress observed and natural behaviours seen from all of the group. Stress behaviours NOT seen when separated. | Enclosure move to familiar enclosure with no other events taking place | Diet provided has a slightly reduced suitability to species and/or individual needs AND/OR lacks variety. |
4 | Enclosure is smaller than wild territory and lacks 2 other elements. | Most natural behaviour can be expressed with minimal reliance on keepers | Group size and structure are not like wild but no stress observed and natural behaviours seen from all of the group. Some stress behaviours seen when separated. | External works/visitor event with some disturbance including visitor event outside of usual opening times but during daylight. | Diet provided has reduced suitability to the individual needs |
5 | Enclosure is smaller than wild territory and lacks 3 other elements. | Most natural behaviours can be expressed with considerable reliance on keepers | Group size is reflective of natural wild group size and suitable group structure. Stocking density is slightly high for the enclosure (e.g., presence of young) | Enclosure move to a completely new enclosure OR significant change to the existing furniture of the enclosure. | Diet provided has reduced suitability to the species needs |
6 | Enclosure is smaller than wild territory and lacks 4–5 other elements. | Some natural behaviours can be expressed with considerable reliance on keepers | Group size and structure is similar to wild but environmental pressures cause stress/aggressive behaviours | External works/visitor event taking place with definite disturbance, including visitor event taking place after sunset. | Diet provided lacks behavioural requirements for the species and individual. |
7 | Enclosure is smaller than wild territory and lacks 6–7 other elements. | Enclosure complexity and keeper intervention are minimal, preventing the expression of numerous natural behaviours. | Group size and structure not completely like wild. Moderate stress behaviours observed either when together OR separated. | Introduction of new unfamiliar animal to group. | Diet provided lacks physiological requirements for the species and individual |
8 | Enclosure is smaller than wild territory and lacks 8–9 other elements. | Enclosure complexity and keeper intervention are minimal, preventing the expression of most natural behaviours | Group size and structure not completely like wild. Significant stress behaviours observed either when together OR separated. | Prolonged external works with definite disruption. | Diet provided lacks nutritional requirements for the species and individual |
9 | Enclosure is smaller than wild territory and lacks 10–11 other elements. | Enclosure complexity and keeper intervention is very limited, preventing the expression of almost all natural behaviours. | Group structure very different to wild group and inappropriate for species (e.g., solitary) and/or high degree of overstocking. | New enclosure and new animals introduced at the same time. | Diet provided lacks 2 requirements for the species and individual. |
10 | Enclosure is smaller than wild territory and lacks 12+ other elements | The options are not available in the enclosure nor provided additionally for the animal to express natural behaviours | Group structure very different to wild group and dangerous for species OR harmful degree of overstocking. | Multiple events happening at the same time (e.g., new enclosure, new group and external works) | Diet provided lacks all requirements for the species and the individual. |
Isolation (From Conspecifics) | Vet Procedures (e.g., Daily Medication, Routine Vaccinations, Sedation, Anaesthesia) | Impact of Vet Procedures and/or Catch up for Other Purpose (e.g., Stress/Fear) | Sedation/Anaesthesia | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Score | ||||
1 | Not isolated | No vet procedure occurred | No vet procedure/catch up occurred | No sedation/anaesthesia |
2 | Isolated for less than 2 h | Minor procedure performed with minimal effect on animal (e.g., delivery of oral medication in food). | Procedure can be performed easily with no stress or aggressive behaviour (e.g., delivery of oral medication in food). | Mild sedation (e.g., sedated not asleep/recumbent). Calm induction and recovery. Rapid return to normal feeding and behaviour |
3 | Isolated for less than 6 h | Minor procedure, short term low impact effecting animal (e.g., parasite spot-on treatment) | Animal does not show anticipatory stress/fear behaviour before the procedure (e.g., triggered by arrival of vet or change in husbandry) but some mild stress/fear shown afterwards. Recovery from stress takes less than 8 h | Deeper sedation (e.g., asleep) with calm induction and recovery. Rapid return to normal feeding and behaviour. |
4 | Isolated for less than 12 h | Minor procedure, medium term low impact effecting animal (e.g., nail clipping). | Animal does not show anticipatory stress/fear behaviour before the procedure but mild stress/fear shown afterwards. Recovery from stress takes less than 12 h | Sedation with stressful induction and/or recovery but rapid return to normal feeding and behaviour after procedure |
5 | Isolated for >12 <24 h | Moderate procedure with short or medium term moderate impact effecting animal. | Animal shows mild anticipatory stress/fear behaviour before and stress/fear after procedure but recovers from stress within 4 h. | Sedation with stressful induction and/or recovery and/or effects on normal feeding and behaviour for a few hours after procedure. |
6 | Isolated for >24 <48 h | Moderate procedure with longer term moderate impact effecting animal. | Animal shows moderate anticipatory stress/fear behaviour before and moderate stress/fear behaviour after procedure but recovers from stress within 8 h. | Sedation with stressful induction and/or recovery and/or up to 12 h for normal feeding and behaviour to return after procedure. |
7 | Isolated for more than 2 days | Moderate procedure with longer term serious impact effecting animal. | Animal shows moderate anticipatory stress/fear behaviour before and severe stress/fear behaviour after procedure but recovers from stress within 12 h | Sedation with stressful induction and/or recovery and/or over 12 h for normal feeding and behaviour to return after procedure. |
8 | Isolated for more than 1 week | Severe procedure with short or medium term moderate impact effecting animal | Animal shows severe anticipatory stress/fear behaviour before and severe stress/fear behaviour after procedure and takes up to 24 h to recover. | Sedation with stressful induction and/or recovery and/or over 24 h for normal feeding and behaviour to return after procedure. |
9 | Isolated for more than 2 weeks | Extensive procedure with significant impact on animal and short term pain despite appropriate treatment and analgesia (e.g., tail amputation) | Animal shows severe anticipatory stress/fear behaviour before and aggressive behaviour after procedure. | Sedation with highly stressful induction and moderate to long term effects on normal feeding and behaviour after the procedure |
10 | Isolated for more than 1 month. | Extensive procedure with significant impact on animal and long term pain despite appropriate treatment and analgesia (e.g., tail amputation) | Animal shows severe anticipatory stress/fear behaviour before and aggressive behaviour after procedure. Animal continues to be aggressive to keepers more than 24 h after the procedure | Sedation with highly stressful induction and prolonged effects on normal feeding and behaviour after the procedure |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Free, D.; Justice, W.S.M.; Smith, S.J.; Howard, V.; Wolfensohn, S. An Approach to Assessing Zoo Animal Welfare in a Rarely Studied Species, the Common Cusimanse Crossarchus obscurus. J. Zool. Bot. Gard. 2022, 3, 420-441. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg3030032
Free D, Justice WSM, Smith SJ, Howard V, Wolfensohn S. An Approach to Assessing Zoo Animal Welfare in a Rarely Studied Species, the Common Cusimanse Crossarchus obscurus. Journal of Zoological and Botanical Gardens. 2022; 3(3):420-441. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg3030032
Chicago/Turabian StyleFree, Danielle, William S. M. Justice, Sarah Jayne Smith, Vittoria Howard, and Sarah Wolfensohn. 2022. "An Approach to Assessing Zoo Animal Welfare in a Rarely Studied Species, the Common Cusimanse Crossarchus obscurus" Journal of Zoological and Botanical Gardens 3, no. 3: 420-441. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg3030032
APA StyleFree, D., Justice, W. S. M., Smith, S. J., Howard, V., & Wolfensohn, S. (2022). An Approach to Assessing Zoo Animal Welfare in a Rarely Studied Species, the Common Cusimanse Crossarchus obscurus. Journal of Zoological and Botanical Gardens, 3(3), 420-441. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg3030032