Next Article in Journal
Gender Diversity and Mental Health: A Systematic Review of the Impact of Childhood Trauma
Previous Article in Journal
Navigating Burnout: The Mediating Role of Mindfulness and the Mediterranean Lifestyle in Fostering Job Satisfaction and Well-Being
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Dual Role of Anxiety in Crisis Response: Emotional Intensification and Prosocial Behavior During the COVID-19 Pandemic

Department of Communication Contents, Division of Future Convergence, Dongduk Women’s University, Seoul 02748, Republic of Korea
Psychiatry Int. 2025, 6(1), 12; https://doi.org/10.3390/psychiatryint6010012
Submission received: 30 November 2024 / Revised: 27 December 2024 / Accepted: 20 January 2025 / Published: 24 January 2025

Abstract

:
The COVID-19 pandemic has profoundly impacted mental health, intensifying negative emotions and challenging collective psychological resilience. This study examines the complex psychological interplay between social media engagement, emotional responses, and behavioral outcomes during crises. Using an online survey (N = 500) conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic, the findings reveal that heightened social media use for social interaction is associated with amplified emotional responses—particularly anxiety, anger, and sadness. Among these, anxiety demonstrated a dual role, acting as both a psychological stressor and a motivator for cooperative and prosocial behaviors, such as supporting government policies, volunteering, and donating. In contrast, anger and sadness were not associated with similar behavioral outcomes. These findings highlight the nuanced psychological impact of anxiety during crises, emphasizing its potential to drive adaptive responses amidst heightened emotional distress. This study contributes to a deeper understanding of how emotional and psychological processes, particularly anxiety, influence public behavior during crises, providing insights for mental health interventions and policy strategies.

1. Introduction

The COVID-19 pandemic has profoundly shaped mental health landscapes, amplifying negative emotions and psychological stressors on a global scale. Social media platforms, such as Twitter and Facebook, emerged as critical spaces for information exchange and emotional expression, where hashtags like #COVIDAnxiety and #PandemicStress reflected widespread public fear and uncertainty. However, while these platforms offered a sense of connection, they also served as conduits for anxiety and fear, exposing users to distressing news updates, rising death tolls, and personal stories of loss and illness. Surveys indicate that approximately 71% of Americans relied on social media for pandemic-related updates [1], yet many people feel overwhelmed and anxious due to the relentless influx of alarming information [2,3,4]. This reinforces existing evidence linking heavy social media use to heightened anxiety, depression, and other adverse psychological outcomes. Research suggests that the framing of information in social media news or stories can significantly influence emotional responses, particularly during crises. How information is presented on social media shapes how individuals feel, interpret, and react to content [5]. Consequently, during crises, media frames emphasizing fear, uncertainty, or negative consequences often intensify negative emotional states such as anxiety, anger, and sadness [6].
While considerable research has examined the role of social media in exacerbating negative emotional states during the pandemic, less is understood about how these emotions shape broader societal responses. This study builds upon existing literature by exploring the dual psychological role of negative emotions in times of crisis—not merely as sources of distress but as potential motivators of cooperative and prosocial behaviors. Theoretical frameworks, such as the cognitive appraisal theory of emotion, suggest that discrete emotional responses to risk can trigger diverse psychological and behavioral outcomes. For example, anxiety may act as a catalyst for actions aimed at reducing uncertainty and perceived threats, strengthening social bonds, or contributing to communal well-being. However, the pathways through which social media-driven emotional responses translate into tangible prosocial behaviors remain underexplored.
This study aims to address this gap by examining the complex interplay between social media use, emotional intensification, and behavioral outcomes during the COVID-19 pandemic. Specifically, it investigates whether and how emotional responses, such as anxiety, anger, and sadness, associated with social media engagement, are linked to public cooperation and prosocial behaviors. Moreover, the study focuses on social interaction—a core function of social media—as a mechanism that fosters emotional responses and behavioral shifts. By analyzing the nuanced roles of specific emotions, particularly anxiety, in shaping public behaviors during crises, this research advances our understanding of the psychological processes underlying collective action.
This research also contributes to the growing body of literature on the pandemic’s psychological consequences, exploring the dual role of social media as both a source of emotional distress and a platform for fostering community resilience and prosocial engagement. The findings have practical implications for mental health interventions and public policy, emphasizing the potential to harness emotional mechanisms to promote cooperative behaviors during public health crises. While COVID-19 has transitioned from a pandemic to an endemic stage, its social, psychological, and behavioral impacts remain highly relevant. Insights from this research extend beyond the pandemic itself, offering valuable applications to other ongoing or emerging global crises, such as climate change, natural disasters, and geopolitical conflicts, where social media significantly influences public behavior and responses. By retrospectively examining the effects of social media use during COVID-19, this study provides empirical evidence on how digital platforms can facilitate—or hinder—collective prosocial behavior during crises, offering important lessons for both researchers and policymakers.

2. Literature Review

2.1. Social Media Use and Emotional Responses

Despite the multiple roles social media plays in public crises, one factor that has garnered increasing attention among scholars is how social media use influences behaviors aimed at mitigating risks. Based on the premise that public engagement and cooperation are crucial for reducing public risks, researchers have examined the mechanisms by which social media can enhance cooperative and prosocial actions. While various factors, such as risk perceptions [7,8], information [9], and perceived knowledge [10], have been identified as mediators of social media’s effects, scholars have increasingly focused on the role of emotions in driving risk-reducing behavior [11]. Beyond cognitive factors, emotions are believed to exert a powerful influence on people’s judgments and behavioral intentions regarding risks. When individuals experience strong emotions, they often make decisions to mitigate risks [12,13] and are motivated to adjust their behaviors to prevent potential negative consequences [14].
Studies examining the relationship between social media use and emotion show that social media users frequently experience negative emotions during crises [4,15]. On platforms like Instagram and TikTok, users often post about their experiences with isolation, depression, and anxiety. As a health crisis signals a significant threat to safety, individuals often respond with strong emotional reactions, leading to more intense expressions of emotions on social media. Gao and colleagues explain that social media use amplifies negative affective responses because disinformation and false reports about COVID-19, prevalent on these platforms, stoke unfounded fears and anxiety [3]. These negative emotions are also contagious on social media, where the expression of such emotions tends to amplify the emotional responses of other users. Empirical studies also indicate that social media promotes the social sharing of emotions, particularly in times of crisis, leading people to experience more negative emotions [16,17].
Although several theories have been proposed to explore the relationship between media use and emotional responses, appraisal theories of emotion [18,19,20] offer a valuable framework for understanding the connection between social media use and emotional experiences during crises. According to appraisal theories, emotions arise from individuals’ evaluations of their environment in relation to their goals, and these appraisals lead to specific emotional responses. For instance, if an individual appraises a situation as threatening and uncertain, they may feel anxiety, whereas if they attribute the situation to the fault of others, they may feel anger. On the other hand, if an individual perceives an irrevocable loss resulting from the situation, they are likely to experience sadness or depression [18]. As a result, different appraisals of events give rise to specific emotions, as individuals interpret situations uniquely based on their expectations, beliefs, and perceived level of control.
Given the vast amount of information and personal stories shared about the pandemic on social media, users are exposed to increased opportunities to evaluate the crisis, potentially leading to heightened emotional reactions. In particular, the cognitive appraisal process, through which individuals assess the severity and personal relevance of events, is heavily influenced by the type of information shared on social media. Specifically, the abundance of pandemic-related content on social media can be associated with particular negative emotions. For instance, when users repeatedly encountered content framing COVID-19 as unpredictable and uncontrollable, as seen in widespread reports of shifting infection rates, evolving virus variants, and inconsistent public health guidelines, they are likely to appraise the situation as an uncertain threat, thereby triggering anxiety. Furthermore, personal stories shared on social media often amplify narratives of irreversible loss, including reports of lives and livelihoods destroyed, which reinforces the belief that the pandemic’s damage is permanent and, in turn, fosters sadness. Additionally, social media discussions about the pandemic have highlighted external sources of blame for the crisis, such as government failures or foreign entities, which can intensify anger by attributing responsibility to factors outside an individual’s control. Thus, it is postulated that social media use not only fosters emotional engagement with the pandemic but also shapes the way individuals appraise the crisis, leading to discrete emotional responses like anxiety, sadness, and anger.
Empirical studies also suggest that social media use during the pandemic can promote these feelings. For example, a study found that social media amplified negative emotions, including anxiety and depression, among users due to exposure to alarming news updates and content that heightened fears related to the pandemic [21]. Lu and Hong demonstrated that social media platforms were utilized to express and amplify negative emotions during the pandemic [22]. Their findings suggest that, particularly during crises like the COVID-19 pandemic, social media intensifies negative emotions through mechanisms such as emotional contagion. In addition, Yang and Tian [23] investigated how interaction-focused social media use (i.e., communal engagement; [24]) influenced emotions, self-efficacy, perceived knowledge, and perceptions of fake news regarding COVID-19. Their findings revealed that communal engagement on social media was associated with increased anxiety, fear, and worry, as well as heightened third-person perception.
Based on appraisal theories of emotion and existing research on the relationship between social media use and emotional responses, it is hypothesized that social media use during the COVID-19 pandemic will intensify emotional responses. Specifically, it is expected that increased social media interaction will elicit stronger emotional experiences, such as anxiety, anger, and sadness, in response to the pandemic.
Accordingly, the first hypothesis is proposed.
H1. 
Social media interaction will be positively related to emotional responses (anxiety, anger, and sadness) to COVID-19.

2.2. Social Media Use and Behavioral Responses to Risk

Social media’s role extends beyond emotional engagement to shaping behaviors during crises. Research has demonstrated that social media use can encourage cooperative behaviors, such as compliance with government policies [25]. This is often attributed to the accessibility of timely information, which allows individuals to remain informed about ongoing health guidelines and public safety measures. Social media platforms provide a direct line of communication between government agencies and the public, fostering an environment where people can stay updated on evolving policies and regulations. Furthermore, the widespread dissemination of information on platforms like Facebook and Twitter has been shown to boost collective awareness and shape social norms [26], reinforcing the importance of following government mandates during crises.
Another way social media may promote public cooperation is by cultivating a deep sense of connectedness through shared interactions and experiences. Indeed, social interactions online increase opportunities for users to understand others’ emotions and develop empathy [27]. These interactions contribute to a sense of social connectedness [28] and the quality of online relationships [29], potentially intensifying emotional reactions to the perceived risks of the pandemic. Additionally, unlike traditional news media, social media interactions often involve more personalized information, coming directly from one’s network members, which can deepen the emotional impact of events [30]. Studies have shown that social media interactions help users comprehend others’ experiences and emotions related to the crisis, fostering feelings of empathy [31,32]. Consequently, these feelings of connectedness and belonging, associated with social media use, may facilitate cooperative intentions.
While previous research has explored the link between social media use and cooperative behaviors, the emphasis has often been on individual-level actions, such as precautionary behaviors [33], adherence to government measures [34], or support for policy interventions [35]. However, beyond simply supporting or complying with authority-imposed measures, engaging in prosocial behaviors embodies a proactive and critical public response to risk. Prosocial behavior in risk contexts includes actions such as volunteering within the community, offering assistance to those affected by the risk, or donating resources to mitigate the threat.
Building on previous research, this study expects that behaviors with broader social impacts, such as prosocial behaviors, may result from discrete emotions induced by social media use. Empirical evidence also suggests that prosocial behaviors can function as a valuable outlet for alleviating negative emotional experiences [36]. Engaging in prosocial behavior has been widely recognized as a coping mechanism, helping individuals alleviate psychological and physiological stress triggered by perceived threats [36]. Therefore, individuals experiencing higher levels of negative emotions may be more inclined to engage in actions that mitigate these emotions. Drawing on insights gained from previous research, the second hypothesis is formulated.
H2a. 
Social media interaction will be positively related to cooperative behavior (e.g., support for government policies).
H2b. 
Social media interaction will be positively related to prosocial behavior (e.g., volunteering).

2.3. Emotion as a Mediator

Emotions play a pivotal role in shaping behavioral responses to risk. When individuals experience negative emotions, particularly those triggered by threats to their lives or health, they are likely to take protective actions. Several theories explain the connection between emotion and action. For instance, the feelings-as-information model posits that people use emotions as a source of information when making decisions, especially when they lack the motivation or ability to deliberate on an issue [37]. Consequently, specific emotions lead individuals to interpret messages in an affect-consistent manner, prompting them to engage in corresponding behaviors. Similarly, appraisal theories of emotion [18,19,20] propose that discrete emotions, which arise from particular appraisals of a situation, are characterized by distinct action tendencies [38]. For instance, anger motivates individuals to approach and confront a problem, while anxiety encourages actions to avoid or mitigate the threat, and sadness or depression leads to introspection and a desire for reassurance [18]. Therefore, emotional responses to risk play a critical role in shaping risk judgments and behaviors [8,39].
During crises, emotions are expected to trigger behaviors that reduce perceived threats, such as increased support for government policies and other prosocial behaviors. Specifically, feeling anxiety about COVID-19 may lead individuals to support policies aimed at addressing the risk, as these measures can help prevent or mitigate the perceived threat. Feeling anger toward the pandemic might motivate people to take proactive actions, such as advocating for solutions or supporting policies that aim to reduce the risk. Similarly, individuals experiencing sadness or depression may seek relief and protection, driving them to support policies designed to safeguard the public. These same emotions may also steer prosocial behaviors—anxiety, for example, could promote prosocial actions, as individuals strive to ensure both personal and communal safety [40]. Anger and sadness, like anxiety, can also lead to prosocial behavior during crises. Anger, when directed towards perceived injustices or external factors responsible for the crisis, can motivate individuals to advocate for change, engage in activism, or support policies that hold responsible entities accountable [41]. It can drive a desire to correct the wrongs and protect others from similar threats in the future [42]. On the other hand, sadness can evoke feelings of empathy and compassion for those affected by the crisis [43]. Experiencing sadness may push individuals to offer support, donate resources, or volunteer to alleviate suffering. The introspective nature of sadness often leads to a desire to create connections with others, fostering communal action aimed at overcoming adversity together [42]. These emotions, therefore, can serve as powerful motivators for collective efforts to mitigate the impact of a crisis.
Thus, emotions triggered by social media use are likely to mediate the relationship between social media engagement and cooperative or prosocial behaviors. This notion is well supported by empirical evidence. For instance, anxiety and worry are powerful predictors of precautionary behaviors, often surpassing factors such as risk perception [44]. Peters and colleagues found that anxiety, more than cognitive risk perception, predicted actions like donating or volunteering during crises, which benefit others [44]. Similarly, anger has been associated with increased support for policies [45] and a higher likelihood of contributing to causes that aim to mitigate harm [46]. Sadness has been associated with increased support for protective measures [47]. Furthermore, Stellar and colleagues suggest that sadness can foster empathy and strengthen prosocial tendencies, encouraging individuals to take action to alleviate the suffering of others [48].
Taken together, this study anticipates that feelings of anxiety, anger, and sadness related to COVID-19 will positively correlate with prosocial behaviors and support for policies aimed at reducing the threat. However, it is important to acknowledge that previous research indicates these emotions may not consistently lead to the anticipated prosocial outcomes. For example, while some studies propose that anger can facilitate prosocial behaviors like compensating victims [49], others highlight that anger may lead to more antagonistic or aggressive actions [50]. Similarly, sadness has been shown to enhance compassion and prosocial tendencies [48], yet it has also been linked to passivity or inaction [51]. This suggests that the relationship between these emotions and behaviors is more complex than initially expected.
Given the mixed findings, the impact of these specific emotions on cooperative and prosocial behaviors, particularly in the context of public health risks, remains uncertain. As a result, this study frames the exploration of these emotional influences as a research question, rather than a definitive hypothesis, to better capture the nuanced and potentially divergent effects of these emotions on behavior.
H3. 
Social media interaction will be positively related to cooperative and prosocial behaviors, mediated by emotional responses to COVID-19 (anxiety, anger, and sadness).
RQ1: Are there differences between anxiety, anger, and sadness in their impact on eliciting cooperative and prosocial behaviors?
The conceptual model of the study is presented in Figure 1.

3. Method

3.1. Sample and Procedure

To test the hypotheses and address the research questions, an online survey was administered via Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk) between 18 January and 20 May 2023. The survey targeted individuals aged 18 and older residing in the United States, with all participants providing written, informed consent prior to participation. MTurk was selected for its proven reliability in recruiting diverse samples from the general population [52,53].
A brief explanation of the study was provided to online survey panels registered on MTurk, and individuals who chose to participate were subsequently enrolled in the study. A total of 500 individuals initiated the survey; however, approximately 11% of responses were excluded due to incompleteness, resulting in a final sample size of 445 participants. The average age of respondents was 45 years (M = 45.24, SD = 11.54), and 44% were female. Additionally, 57% of participants had attained a college degree or higher. The majority identified as White (78.13%), followed by 9.34% identifying as Black, 8.66% as Asian, and 3.87% as another race or ethnicity. Upon providing informed consent, participants answered a series of questions regarding their emotional responses to COVID-19, judgments, and behavioral intentions. Subsequently, they provided information about their social media usage, demographic characteristics, and other relevant factors, such as perceived knowledge and self-efficacy.

3.2. Measures

Social media interaction was measured using items from Lim and colleagues’ study [24], originally designed to assess communal social media use, and adapted to the context of COVID-19. Participants were asked to indicate how frequently they: (1) shared important COVID-19-related news, (2) commented on posts about COVID-19, (3) contributed useful information to social media communities, and (4) used COVID-19-related hashtags. Responses were recorded on a 7-point Likert scale, and the items were averaged to create an index (M = 3.11, SD = 1.55, α = 0.90). To measure emotional responses, respondents were asked to report their emotional responses to COVID-19. They indicated on a 7-point Likert scale the extent to which they felt worried (anxiety: M = 4.12, SD = 1.88), angry (M = 4.00, SD = 1.8), or depressed (sadness: M = 3.47, SD = 1.89) when thinking about the pandemic. Policy support was measured by assessing respondents’ level of agreement with various COVID-19 mitigation policies, including a U.S. government-imposed mandatory quarantine, voluntary home quarantine, and the cancellation of mass gatherings. These items were measured on a 7-point Likert scale, and the average score created an index (M = 4.64, SD = 1.79, α = 0.84). Prosocial behavior was examined by asking participants the extent to which they would engage in actions like seeking volunteer opportunities, making donations, or helping those impacted by COVID-19. These were measured on a 7-point Likert scale (M = 4.26, SD = 1.62, α = 0.89). Control variables included age (M = 45.24, SD = 11.54), gender (44% female, 56% male), education (M = 4.28, SD = 1.29; 1 = Less than high school, 7 = Doctorate), and political orientation (M = 3.59, SD = 1.76; 1 = extremely liberal, 7 = extremely conservative). Additional covariates included perceived knowledge (M = 5.46, SD = 0.86, α = 0.73), self-efficacy (M = 5.63, SD = 0.88, α = 0.72), and general social media use (M = 4.56, SD = 1.53; 1 = never, 7 = more than 10 times a day). Table 1 shows the correlations between the variables.

4. Results

4.1. The Relationship Between Social Media Use and Emotional Responses to COVID-19

To test H1, which predicts that social media interaction is positively related to emotional responses to COVID-19, hierarchical logistic regression models were used. This method was chosen to control for sociodemographic variables and other relevant factors while assessing the unique contribution of social media use variables. Table 2 presents the results. Regarding anxiety, the first block of control variables showed that political conservatism (β = −0.42, SE = 0.05, p < 0.001) and self-efficacy (β = −0.67, SE = 0.11, p < 0.001) were negatively associated with anxiety, while perceived knowledge was positively associated (β = 0.33, SE = 0.11, p < 0.01). In the second block, social media interaction was positively associated with anxiety (β = 0.39, SE = 0.05, p < 0.001), whereas general social media use was negatively related to anxiety (β = −0.20, SE = 0.05, p < 0.001). For anger, self-efficacy was negatively associated (β = −0.73, SE = 0.11, p < 0.001), and perceived knowledge was positively associated (β = 0.46, SE = 0.11, p < 0.001). Social media interaction was positively associated with anger (β = 0.22, SE = 0.05, p < 0.001), while general social media use was negatively related to anger (β = −0.13, SE = 0.06, p < 0.05). For sadness, political conservatism (β = −0.25, SE = 0.05, p < 0.001) and self-efficacy (β = −0.59, SE = 0.11, p < 0.001) were negatively associated. Social media interaction was positively related to sadness (β = 0.25, SE = 0.05, p < 0.001), while general social media use was negatively associated with sadness (β = −0.11, SE = 0.06, p < 0.05). Thus, H1 was supported.

4.2. The Relationship Between Social Media Interaction and Behavioral Responses

H2a,b predicted that social media interaction would be positively related to support for COVID-19 mitigation policies and prosocial behaviors. Additionally, RQ1 examined whether anxiety, anger, and sadness have distinct effects on cooperative and prosocial behaviors.
Table 3 presents the results. For policy support, the first block of control variables revealed that political conservatism (β = −0.61, SE = 0.04, p < 0.001) was negatively associated with policy support. In the second block, social media interaction was positively associated with policy support (β = 0.29, SE = 0.04, p < 0.001). In the third block, social media interaction remained positively associated with policy support (β = 0.19, SE = 0.04, p < 0.001). Among the emotions, anxiety was positively associated with policy support (β = 0.35, SE = 0.05, p < 0.001), while sadness was marginally associated with policy support (β = −0.09, SE = 0.05, p < 0.10). For prosocial behavior, the first block of control variables showed political conservatism (β = −0.29, SE = 0.04, p < 0.001) was negatively associated, while perceived knowledge (β = 0.52, SE = 0.09, p < 0.001) was positively associated. In the second block, social media interaction was positively associated with prosocial behavior (β = 0.39, SE = 0.04, p < 0.001), whereas general social media use was negatively associated with prosocial behavior (β = −0.09, SE = 0.04, p < 0.05). In the third block, social media interaction remained positively associated with prosocial behavior (β = 0.36, SE = 0.04, p < 0.001). Again, only anxiety was significantly and positively associated with the intention to engage in prosocial behavior (β = 0.13, SE = 0.05, p < 0.05).

4.3. Emotion as a Mediator

H3 aimed to examine whether social media interaction is positively related to cooperative and prosocial behaviors, with emotional responses to COVID-19 mediating this relationship. Given the significant role of anxiety in predicting policy support and prosocial behavior, mediation analyses were conducted to explore whether anxiety mediated the relationship between social media use and these outcomes. Bootstrapping was employed as it provides a robust method for testing indirect effects by generating confidence intervals for the indirect paths [54]. The results confirmed that social media interaction indirectly increased both policy support (β = 0.18, SE = 0.03, 95% CI: [0.131, 0.230]) and prosocial behavior (β = 0.07, SE = 0.02, 95% CI: [0.02, 0.11]) through the emotion of anxiety. Therefore, only anxiety served as a mediator in the relationship between social media use and these key behavioral outcomes.

5. Discussion

Building on prior research examining the impact of social media on public responses to risk, this study aimed to explore how social media use influences emotional reactions to risk and how these emotional responses subsequently impact cooperative and prosocial behaviors. While previous studies often highlight the negative mental health consequences of emotions [3,15], our findings suggest a more nuanced perspective: these negative emotions, when amplified by social media interaction, can also have a positive societal impact. This shift in focus reveals that, although negative emotions such as anxiety are often viewed as detrimental to individual mental health, they may serve as catalysts for collective action during public crises. By fostering a sense of interconnectedness and empathy, social media enables users to not only feel the weight of societal risks but also to channel these emotions into behaviors that benefit the broader community.
The finding that social media interaction amplifies emotional responses to risk is especially important when considering the phenomenon of optimistic bias, wherein individuals believe they are less likely than others to be affected by negative events [55]. Previous research suggests that this discrepancy between personal and societal risk perceptions can demotivate people from taking public action to mitigate risks [56,57]. By amplifying emotional responses, social media may play a critical role in helping people recognize the personal relevance of risks, thereby challenging the belief that risks only affect others. This heightened emotional engagement can, in turn, lead to greater cooperation and efforts to reduce risks. Given the crucial role that emotions play in mobilizing public responses to risk [39], this study extends existing research by demonstrating that social media can be used not only to regulate emotions during crises [31] but also to amplify emotional responses that prompt public action.
Furthermore, the study found that anxiety was a strong predictor of prosocial behavior and cooperation with government measures. This is consistent with research showing that anxiety positively influences risk-reducing behaviors [58,59]. Importantly, anxiety not only influenced individual-level risk-related behaviors (e.g., supporting policies) but also spurred social-level behaviors with prosocial implications (e.g., volunteering, donating). Contrary to some previous research suggesting that anxiety might inhibit political or civic participation [60], the findings of this study indicate that anxiety, when induced by active social media interaction, has the potential to foster public engagement in prosocial activities.
The finding that sadness was marginally negatively associated with policy support raises further questions. Contrary to the hypothesis, individuals who felt sadness about the risk were less likely to support policies aimed at mitigating it. This aligns with the idea that sadness is often characterized as an inaction-oriented emotion [18]. However, considering that other studies suggest that sadness can increase preferences for welfare policies [61] or foster greater intentions to help others [62], these mixed findings imply that the influence of sadness on behavior may depend on other relevant factors such as the type of risk (e.g., natural vs. human-made) and individual traits (e.g., empathy levels).
The lack of a significant relationship between anger and behavioral responses is somewhat surprising, given prior research indicating that anger can shape policy preferences and actions in various risk contexts [45,46]. One possible explanation is that anger may be more influential in contexts where the core action tendency—blaming or punishing—can effectively reduce the perceived threat. In the case of COVID-19, a public health crisis, blaming others may not translate into meaningful actions that mitigate the risk, which could explain why anger did not drive prosocial behavior in this study. Future research should further investigate the circumstances under which different emotions promote cooperative or prosocial behaviors.
One important finding of this study is that social media interaction itself was positively associated with greater support for policies and prosocial behaviors, independent of the mediating role of emotions. This supports existing literature suggesting that the inherent characteristics of social media—such as connectedness and interactivity—can foster mutual understanding and empathy [27]. By enabling users to connect with others and understand their emotions [63], social media may promote prosocial behaviors during times of risk. Future studies could explore which specific aspects of social media use (e.g., frequency of exposure to crisis-related content, engagement with misinformation, interactions within supportive online communities, or the use of emotion-laden narratives) are most closely linked to emotional, behavioral, and cognitive effects, such as changes in risk perception.
The findings of this study also showed that self-efficacy was associated with reduced emotional responses to COVID-19. Self-efficacy, defined as the belief that one can effectively manage or mitigate risks [64], is often viewed as a critical driver of action. However, in this case, higher self-efficacy may have reduced concern or anxiety about the risk, potentially leading to lower engagement in risk mitigation efforts. This aligns with research suggesting that self-efficacy can sometimes result in overconfidence [65], thereby reducing the likelihood of generating emotional responses. Future research should further explore the conditions under which self-efficacy fosters positive action versus when it leads to complacency.

5.1. Implications

This study adds to the growing research on how social media shapes public emotional responses to risks and encourages prosocial behavior. While much of the existing literature emphasizes the negative effects of emotions like anxiety and sadness—particularly in terms of their contribution to mental health disorders—this study offers a more nuanced perspective. While negative emotions can exacerbate psychological distress and mental health issues, such as increased stress and emotional instability, when these emotions are channeled through socially interactive and empathetic uses of social media, they have the potential to yield positive societal outcomes. Rather than framing these emotions as solely harmful, the findings suggest they can be channeled into fostering prosocial actions, including volunteering, donating, and supporting public health policies. Furthermore, considering the persistent gap between personal and general risk perceptions [7], this study highlights how social media may help bridge that gap by providing personalized, interpersonal avenues for risk communication. This helps individuals recognize the relevance of societal risks to their own lives, thereby promoting greater cooperation and public engagement.
In addition, the findings can be understood and their implications extended within a broader context. Similar mechanisms—such as the role of social media in amplifying emotions and influencing prosocial behaviors—are likely to be relevant in other crises, including natural disasters, economic crises, or geopolitical conflicts. These events often generate widespread emotional responses and collective actions facilitated by digital platforms, making them comparable to the dynamics observed during the pandemic.
From a practical perspective, the findings of this study provide valuable insights for both mental health practitioners and public health policymakers. Anxiety, often seen as a mental health challenge, emerges in this study as a dual-faceted emotion that can motivate cooperative and prosocial actions. This nuanced understanding highlights opportunities for interventions that leverage anxiety constructively, rather than solely mitigating its effects. For instance, mental health interventions could focus on anxiety management techniques that channel this emotion toward productive actions, such as volunteering, donating, or compliance with health guidelines, rather than allowing it to escalate into maladaptive stress or panic. In the public health domain, campaigns could strategically use messages that evoke manageable levels of anxiety, emphasizing collective responsibility and achievable actions. Indeed, campaigns have traditionally relied on fear appeals to encourage compliance with public health measures; however, such messaging can backfire by eliciting avoidance or withdrawal behaviors [66]. This study suggests that focusing on anxiety, rather than fear, may encourage greater public engagement without the risk of these unintended negative consequences. By inducing a sense of concern and responsibility, anxiety might serve as a more effective emotional trigger for fostering cooperation and prosocial actions.
Furthermore, the study’s findings suggest that social media platforms could be optimized to facilitate emotional engagement more effectively. Design features that promote emotional sharing, community-building, and empathetic understanding could enhance the public’s willingness to cooperate during crises. For example, platforms could integrate features that highlight community-driven support systems or allow users to share their personal experiences with public health risks, making the threat feel more immediate and relatable. These design elements could create a more engaging and interactive environment that encourages users to take collective action in response to societal risks. In a broader context, policymakers and social media companies could collaborate to leverage these insights, ensuring that digital spaces are designed not only for information dissemination but also for emotional and behavioral engagement. By fostering a sense of shared responsibility and empathy through tailored social media interactions, these platforms can play a crucial role in addressing public health risks and encouraging cooperative behaviors that contribute to the overall well-being of society.
Finally, the study underscores the need for systemic support for mental health during crises. Governments and healthcare providers should allocate resources to expand access to mental health services, particularly for individuals disproportionately affected by emotional distress linked to social media use. This includes enhancing crisis hotline availability and training mental health professionals to address the unique challenges posed by digital engagement during crises.

5.2. Limitations and Future Research

Although this study provides valuable insights into the role of social media in shaping emotional responses and fostering prosocial behavior, it is not without limitations. First, the cross-sectional nature of the data makes it difficult to establish clear causal relationships between social media use, emotional responses, and prosocial behavior. Future research could benefit from using longitudinal or experimental designs to better understand the directionality and potential causality between these variables. For example, a longitudinal study could track changes in social media use, emotional states, and prosocial behaviors over time, providing a clearer picture of how these factors interact during crises.
Second, this study focused primarily on negative emotional responses (anxiety, anger, sadness) and their potential for prosocial outcomes. However, future research should explore a wider range of emotions, including positive emotions such as hope or compassion, which may also play important roles in shaping prosocial behavior. Examining how both positive and negative emotions interact in driving behavior would offer a more comprehensive understanding of social media’s emotional impact. Additionally, the study used self-reported data for measuring emotions and behaviors, which can introduce biases such as social desirability or recall errors. Future studies could incorporate more objective measures, such as physiological indicators of emotion (e.g., heart rate) or behavioral tracking data from social media platforms, to complement self-reported responses and provide a more robust assessment of the emotional and behavioral impacts of social media use.
In addition, the sample for this study was non-probabilistic, meaning that participants were not randomly selected, which limits the representativeness of the sample. Consequently, the findings of this study cannot be generalized to other contexts, populations, or topics with confidence. This sampling method might have introduced biases, as participants self-selected into the study, potentially skewing the results based on the characteristics of those more inclined to participate. Future research using probabilistic sampling methods or a more diverse and representative sample is recommended to enhance the generalizability and applicability of the findings.
This study was conducted during the endemic stage of COVID-19, raising questions about the continued relevance of the pandemic’s psychological impact on the sample. Although the design and sample of this study do not allow for a direct and explicit examination of the pandemic’s immediate psychological impact, its consequences likely continue to shape people’s emotional and behavioral responses. The participants, having lived through the pandemic, may still experience residual effects, such as heightened anxiety, sadness, and shifts in social behaviors. These lingering emotions provide a valuable lens for understanding how individuals process and respond to crises, even after the immediate threat has subsided. Nevertheless, considering that the psychological effects of the pandemic may differ between its acute phase and the post-pandemic period [67], future research could benefit from employing a longitudinal design to track emotional and behavioral shifts over time. Such an approach would offer a clearer understanding of both the short-term and long-term effects of the pandemic. Additionally, broadening the scope to include comparative analyses with other crises could yield deeper insights into the universality or specificity of these findings. These approaches would contribute to a more comprehensive understanding of the interplay between social media, emotions, and behavioral responses in crisis contexts.
Relatedly, the participants’ responses in this study likely reflect a combination of their current perceptions of COVID-19 as a relevant event and their memories or past experiences of the pandemic [8]. This possibility presents an important caveat when interpreting the study’s findings. Future research could benefit from directly assessing participants’ perceptions of COVID-19’s relevance at the time of data collection, allowing for a clearer distinction between the influence of present perceptions and past experiences. The emotional responses captured in this study are likely shaped by the lingering effects of the pandemic or retrospective reflections, rather than acute emotional states driven by the urgency of addressing the crisis. Future research that differentiates between risk behaviors exhibited during the peak of the pandemic and those observed over time could provide valuable insights into the evolution of emotional and behavioral responses across different stages of a crisis.
The effects of social media use are also likely influenced by individual differences, such as information-processing abilities, pre-existing risk perceptions, or emotional tendencies. These personal characteristics can moderate the effects of social media on risk-related behaviors. For instance, individuals with greater information-processing skills may better filter misinformation, while those with higher risk perceptions may be more likely to engage in prosocial behaviors when exposed to emotionally charged content. Future research should explore how these individual characteristics interact with social media use to affect emotional and behavioral responses. Lastly, this study examined social media use in a generalized manner, without differentiating between specific platforms or types of engagement. Social media platforms vary in their design, affordances, and user interactions, all of which may influence emotional experiences and behaviors differently. Future research could explore how various types of social media platforms uniquely influence emotions and behaviors during crises, providing more nuanced insights into the intricate relationship between social media use, emotional responses, and public actions in times of crisis.

Funding

This study was supported by a grant from Dongduk Women’s University.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted according to the Helsinki Declaration. Approval for the study was not required in accordance with local/national legislation.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data are available upon request.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Anderson, M.; Vogels, E.A. Americans Turn to Technology During COVID-19 Outbreak, Say an Outage Would Be a Problem. Pew Research Center. Available online: https://www.pewresearch.org/internet/2020/03/31/americans-turn-to-technology-during-covid-19-outbreak-say-an-outage-would-be-a-problem/ (accessed on 5 February 2024).
  2. Buchanan, K.; Aknin, L.B.; Lotun, S.; Sandstrom, G.M. Brief exposure to social media during the COVID-19 pandemic: Doom-scrolling has negative emotional consequences, but kindness-scrolling does not. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0257728. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Gao, J.; Zheng, P.; Jia, Y.; Chen, H.; Mao, Y.; Chen, S.; Wang, Y.; Fu, H.; Dai, J. Mental health problems and social media exposure during COVID-19 outbreak. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0231924. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Wheaton, M.G.; Prikhidko, A.; Messner, G.R. Is fear of COVID-19 contagious? The effects of emotion contagion and social media use on anxiety in response to the coronavirus pandemic. Front. Psychol. 2021, 11, 567379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Entman, R.M. Framing: Toward clarification of a fractured paradigm. J. Commun. 1993, 43, 51–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Chong, D.; Druckman, J.N. Framing theory. Annu. Rev. Polit. Sci. 2007, 10, 103–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Sjöberg, L. Distal factors in risk perception. J. Risk Res. 2003, 6, 187–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Slovic, P. Perception of risk. Reflections on the psychometric paradigm. In Social Theories of Risk, edited by Sheldon Krimsky and Dominic Golding, Praeger; Krimsky, S., Godling, D., Eds.; Praeger: New York, NY, USA, 1992; pp. 117–152. [Google Scholar]
  9. Chen, Q.; Min, C.; Zhang, W.; Wang, G.; Ma, X.; Evans, R. Unpacking the black box: How to promote citizen engagement through government social media during the COVID-19 crisis. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2020, 110, 106380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Rudyanto, R.; Pramono, R.; Juliana, J. Perception of knowledge of the risk of the COVID-19 pandemic regarding touring intentions and tourism travel recommendations. J. Environ. Manag. Tour. 2021, 12, 929–947. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Oh, S.H.; Lee, S.Y.; Han, C. The effects of social media use on preventive behaviors during infectious disease outbreaks: The mediating role of self-relevant emotions and public risk perception. Health Commun. 2021, 36, 972–981. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Lench, H.C.; Flores, S.A.; Bench, S.W. Discrete emotions predict changes in cognition, judgment, experience, behavior, and physiology: A meta-analysis of experimental emotion elicitations. Psychol. Bull. 2011, 137, 834–855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Loewenstein, G.F.; Weber, E.U.; Hsee, C.K.; Welch, N. Risk as feelings. Psychol. Bull. 2001, 127, 267–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Chapman, G.B.; Coups, E.J. Emotions and preventive health behavior: Worry, regret, and influenza vaccination. Health Psychol. 2006, 25, 82–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Beaudoin, C.E.; Hong, T. Emotions in the time of coronavirus: Antecedents of digital and social media use among millennials. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2021, 123, 106876. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Austin, L.; Kim, S.; Saffer, A.J. Emotion as a predictor of crisis communicative behaviors: Examining information seeking and sharing during Hurricane Florence. J. Appl. Commun. Res. 2023, 51, 559–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Kušen, E.; Strembeck, M. Building blocks of communication networks in times of crises: Emotion-exchange motifs. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2021, 123, 106883. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Lazarus, R.S. Cognition and motivation in emotion. Am. Psychol. 1991, 46, 352–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Roseman, I.J. Cognitive determinants of emotion: A structural theory. Rev. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1984, 5, 11–36. [Google Scholar]
  20. Scherer, K.R. On the nature and function of emotion: A component process approach. In Approaches to Emotion; Psychology Press: Hove, UK, 1984; pp. 293–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Li, L.; Wang, Z.; Zhang, Q.; Wen, H. Effect of anger, anxiety, and sadness on the propagation scale of social media posts after natural disasters. Inf. Process. Manag. 2020, 57, 102313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Lu, D.; Hong, D. Emotional contagion: Research on the influencing factors of social media users’ negative emotional communication during the COVID-19 pandemic. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 931835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Yang, J.; Tian, Y. “Others are more vulnerable to fake news than I am”: Third-person effect of COVID-19 fake news on social media users. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2021, 125, 106950. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Lim, J.S.; Hwang, Y.; Kim, S.; Biocca, F.A. How social media engagement leads to sports channel loyalty: Mediating roles of social presence and channel commitment. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2015, 46, 158–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Tsoy, D.; Tirasawasdichai, T.; Kurpayanidi, K.I. Role of social media in shaping public risk perception during COVID-19 pandemic: A theoretical review. Int. J. Manag. Sci. Bus. Adm. 2021, 7, 35–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Kahne, J.; Bowyer, B. The political significance of social media activity and social networks. Polit. Commun. 2018, 35, 470–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Vossen, H.G.; Valkenburg, P.M. Do social media foster or curtail adolescents’ empathy? A longitudinal study. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2016, 63, 118–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Grieve, R.; Indian, M.; Witteveen, K.; Tolan, G.A.; Marrington, J. Face-to-face or Facebook: Can social connectedness be derived online? Comput. Hum. Behav. 2013, 29, 604–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Valkenburg, P.M.; Peter, J. The effects of instant messaging on the quality of adolescents’ existing friendships: A longitudinal study. J. Commun. 2009, 59, 79–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Qin, X.; Yang, F.; Jiang, Z.; Zhong, B. Empathy not quarantined: Social support via social media helps maintain empathy during the COVID-19 pandemic. Soc. Media Soc. 2022, 8, 20563051221086234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Neubaum, G.; Rösner, L.; Rosenthal-von der Pütten, A.M.; Krämer, N.C. Psychosocial functions of social media usage in a disaster situation: A multi-methodological approach. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2014, 34, 28–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Li, J.; Stephens, K.K.; Zhu, Y.; Murthy, D. Using social media to call for help in Hurricane Harvey: Bonding emotion, culture, and community relationships. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 2019, 38, 101212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Dennis, D.; Radnitz, C.; Wheaton, M.G. A perfect storm? Health anxiety, contamination fears, and COVID-19: Lessons learned from past pandemics and current challenges. Int. J. Cogn. Ther. 2021, 14, 497–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Harper, C.A.; Satchell, L.P.; Fido, D.; Latzman, R.D. Functional fear predicts public health compliance in the COVID-19 pandemic. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 2021, 19, 1875–1888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Renström, E.A.; Bäck, H. Emotions during the COVID-19 pandemic: Fear, anxiety, and anger as mediators between threats and policy support and political actions. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2021, 51, 861–877. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Lazar, L.; Eisenberger, N.I. The benefits of giving: Effects of prosocial behavior on recovery from stress. Psychophysiology 2022, 59, e13954. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Schwarz, N.; Clore, G.L. Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1983, 45, 513–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Frijda, N.H.; Kuipers, P.; Ter Schure, E. Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1989, 57, 212–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Slovic, P. The Feeling of Risk: New Perspectives on Risk Perception; Routledge: London, UK, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  40. Drury, J.; Reicher, S. Collective psychological empowerment as a model of social change: Researching crowds and power. J. Soc. Issues 2009, 65, 707–725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Van Zomeren, M.; Spears, R.; Leach, C.W. Exploring psychological mechanisms of collective action: Does relevance of group identity influence how people cope with collective disadvantage? Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 47, 353–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Frijda, N.H. The Emotions; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1986. [Google Scholar]
  43. Gross, J.J. Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology 2002, 39, 281–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Peters, E.; Slovic, P.; Hibbard, J.H.; Tusler, M. Why worry? Worry, risk perceptions, and willingness to act to reduce medical errors. Health Psychol. 2006, 25, 144–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Gault, B.A.; Sabini, J. The roles of empathy, anger, and gender in predicting attitudes toward punitive, reparative, and preventative public policies. Cogn. Emot. 2000, 14, 495–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Skurka, C. You mad? Using anger appeals to promote activism intentions and policy support in the context of sugary drink marketing to kids. Health Commun. 2019, 34, 1775–1787. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Lu, H. The effects of emotional appeals and gain versus loss framing in communicating sea star wasting disease. Sci. Commun. 2016, 38, 143–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Stellar, J.E.; Cohen, A.; Oveis, C.; Keltner, D. Affective and physiological responses to the suffering of others: Compassion and vagal activity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2015, 108, 572–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Van Doorn, J.; Zeelenberg, M.; Breugelmans, S.M.; Berger, S.; Okimoto, T.G. Prosocial consequences of third-party anger. Theory Decis. 2018, 84, 585–599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Lemay, E.P.; Overall, N.C.; Clark, M.S. Experiences and interpersonal consequences of hurt feelings and anger. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2012, 103, 982–1006. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Rucker, D.D.; Petty, R.E. Emotion specificity and consumer behavior: Anger, sadness, and preference for activity. Motiv. Emot. 2004, 28, 3–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Paolacci, G.; Chandler, J.; Ipeirotis, P.G. Running experiments on Amazon Mechanical Turk. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2010, 5, 411–419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Thomas, K.A.; Clifford, S. Validity and Mechanical Turk: An assessment of exclusion methods and interactive experiments. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2017, 77, 184–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Hayes, A.F. Partial, conditional, and moderated moderated mediation: Quantification, inference, and interpretation. Commun. Monogr. 2018, 85, 4–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Weinstein, N.D. Unrealistic optimism about future life events. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1980, 39, 806–820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Costa-Font, J.; Mossialos, E.; Rudisill, C. Optimism and the perceptions of new risks. J. Risk Res. 2009, 12, 27–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Jiménez-Castillo, D.; Ortega-Egea, J.M. Too positive to change? Examining optimism bias as a barrier to media effects on environmental activism. J. Environ. Psychol. 2015, 43, 216–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Cypryańska, M.; Nezlek, J.B. Anxiety as a mediator of relationships between perceptions of the threat of COVID-19 and coping behaviors during the onset of the pandemic in Poland. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0241464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Locander, W.B.; Hermann, P.W. The effect of self-confidence and anxiety on information seeking in consumer risk reduction. J. Mark. Res. 1979, 16, 268–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Best, S.J.; Krueger, B.S. Government monitoring and political participation in the United States: The distinct roles of anger and anxiety. Am. Polit. Res. 2011, 39, 85–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Small, D.A.; Lerner, J.S. Emotional policy: Personal sadness and anger shape judgments about a welfare case. Polit. Psychol. 2008, 29, 149–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Maner, J.K.; Gailliot, M.T. Altruism and egoism: Prosocial motivations for helping depend on relationship context. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 37, 347–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Alloway, T.; Runac, R.; Quershi, M.; Kemp, G. Is Facebook linked to selfishness? Investigating the relationships among social media use, empathy, and narcissism. Soc. Netw. 2014, 3, 211–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Bandura, A.; Adams, N.E. Analysis of self-efficacy theory of behavioral change. Cogn. Ther. Res. 1977, 1, 287–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Vancouver, J.B.; Thompson, C.M.; Tischner, E.C.; Putka, D.J. Two studies examining the negative effect of self-efficacy on performance. J. Appl. Psychol. 2002, 87, 506–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Cho, H.; Salmon, C.T. Fear appeals for individuals in different stages of change: Intended and unintended effects and implications on public health campaigns. Health Commun. 2006, 20, 91–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Horita, R.; Nishio, A.; Yamamoto, M. Lingering effects of COVID-19 on the mental health of first-year university students in Japan. PLoS ONE 2022, 17, e0262550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Conceptual framework of the study. Note. * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001.
Figure 1. Conceptual framework of the study. Note. * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001.
Psychiatryint 06 00012 g001
Table 1. Correlation matrix for social media use, emotions, and behavioral responses to COVID-19.
Table 1. Correlation matrix for social media use, emotions, and behavioral responses to COVID-19.
12345678910111213
1. Age
2. Gender0.20 ***
3. Education0.16 ***0.02
4. Political orientation−0.00−0.05−0.05
5. Perceived knowledge0.020.080.05−0.06
6. Self-efficacy0.010.010.03−0.060.53 ***
7. General social media use−0.18 ***−0.06−0.10 *−0.080.090.06
8. Social media interaction0.050.070.07−0.21 ***0.16 ***−0.020.17 ***
9. Anxiety0.10 *0.11 *0.03−0.39 ***0.02−0.21 ***−0.090.41 ***
10. Anger0.030.05−0.020.060.04−0.22 ***−0.050.22 ***0.40 ***
11. Sadness0.040.060.07−0.23 ***−0.06−0.25 ***−0.060.26 ***0.63 ***0.52 ***
12. Policy support−0.060.020.04−0.61 ***0.12 *0.080.09 *0.38 ***0.50 ***0.040.23 ***
13. Prosocial behavior−0.000.02−0.01−0.33 ***0.28 ***0.13 **0.030.46 ***0.33 ***0.070.14 **0.41 ***
Note. N = 445. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table 2. Hierarchical regression analysis predicting emotional responses to COVID-19.
Table 2. Hierarchical regression analysis predicting emotional responses to COVID-19.
AnxietyAngerSadness
Model 1Model 2Model 1Model 2Model 1Model 2
Age0.010.010.000.000.000.00
Gender0.230.170.110.070.150.11
Education0.00−0.05−0.03−0.050.090.06
Political orientation−0.42 ***−0.36 ***0.050.09−0.25 ***−0.21 **
Perceived knowledge0.33 **0.21 *0.46 ***0.38 ***0.170.09
Self-efficacy−0.67 ***−0.56 ***−0.73 ***−0.62 ***−0.66 ***−0.59 ***
General social media use −0.20 *** −0.13 * −0.11 *
Social media interaction 0.39 *** 0.22 *** 0.25 ***
Adjusted R20.230.340.080.120.120.26
F22.4628.956.898.1210.9611.49
Note. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table 3. Hierarchical regression analysis predicting policy support and prosocial behavior.
Table 3. Hierarchical regression analysis predicting policy support and prosocial behavior.
Policy SupportProsocial Behavior
Model 1Model 2Model 3Model 1Model 2Model 3
Age−0.01−0.01 *−0.02 **0.00−0.00−0.00
Gender−0.01−0.04−0.09−0.04−0.10−0.11
Education0.020.000.02−0.05−0.08−0.08
Political orientation−0.61 ***−0.56 ***−0.45 ***−0.29 ***−0.23 ***−0.18 ***
Perceived knowledge0.17 +0.060.010.52 ***0.37 ***0.36 ***
Self-efficacy−0.000.080.19−0.060.040.08
General social media use −0.020.04 −0.09 *−0.07 *
Social media interaction 0.29 ***0.19 *** 0.39 ***0.36 ***
Anxiety 0.35 *** 0.13 *
Anger −0.05 −0.03
Sadness −0.09 + −0.04
Adjusted R20.370.440.500.170.310.32
F44.6043.5940.5715.7125.6719.45
Note. + p < 0.10, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Kim, S. The Dual Role of Anxiety in Crisis Response: Emotional Intensification and Prosocial Behavior During the COVID-19 Pandemic. Psychiatry Int. 2025, 6, 12. https://doi.org/10.3390/psychiatryint6010012

AMA Style

Kim S. The Dual Role of Anxiety in Crisis Response: Emotional Intensification and Prosocial Behavior During the COVID-19 Pandemic. Psychiatry International. 2025; 6(1):12. https://doi.org/10.3390/psychiatryint6010012

Chicago/Turabian Style

Kim, Soohee. 2025. "The Dual Role of Anxiety in Crisis Response: Emotional Intensification and Prosocial Behavior During the COVID-19 Pandemic" Psychiatry International 6, no. 1: 12. https://doi.org/10.3390/psychiatryint6010012

APA Style

Kim, S. (2025). The Dual Role of Anxiety in Crisis Response: Emotional Intensification and Prosocial Behavior During the COVID-19 Pandemic. Psychiatry International, 6(1), 12. https://doi.org/10.3390/psychiatryint6010012

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop